心理科学进展 ›› 2022, Vol. 30 ›› Issue (3): 684-692.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2022.00684
收稿日期:
2021-05-28
出版日期:
2022-03-15
发布日期:
2022-01-25
通讯作者:
崔倩,蒋重清
E-mail:cuiqian119@163.com;jzqcjj@hotmail.com
基金资助:
LI Dong1, WU Qi2, YAN Xiangbo1, CUI Qian1(), JIANG Zhongqing1()
Received:
2021-05-28
Online:
2022-03-15
Published:
2022-01-25
Contact:
CUI Qian,JIANG Zhongqing
E-mail:cuiqian119@163.com;jzqcjj@hotmail.com
摘要:
人们会根据陌生人的面孔线索或语音线索迅速地对其人格特质进行主观推断而形成第一印象。面孔-人格知觉第一印象和语音-人格知觉第一印象在维度结构和内在机制上具有相似性; 在对具体人格特质和维度的敏感性, 以及具体的认知机制方面又具有各自的特异性。未来研究可以基于同一批被知觉者开展面孔-人格知觉第一印象和语音-人格知觉第一印象的直接比较, 并着力探究二者的过程特点, 以及人格知觉第一印象形成时面孔和语音知觉的跨模态整合效应。
中图分类号:
李东, 吴琦, 闫向博, 崔倩, 蒋重清. (2022). 第一印象中面孔-人格知觉和语音-人格知觉的异同. 心理科学进展 , 30(3), 684-692.
LI Dong, WU Qi, YAN Xiangbo, CUI Qian, JIANG Zhongqing. (2022). Similarities and differences between face-personality perception and voice-personality perception in first impression. Advances in Psychological Science, 30(3), 684-692.
维度内容 | 面孔研究 | 语音研究 |
---|---|---|
“效价(valence)”和 “支配性(dominance)” | Jones et al., | Baus et al., McAleer et al., |
Oosterhof & Todorov, | ||
Morrison et al., | ||
Wang et al., | ||
“亲和性(approachability)”和“能力(capability)” | 吴琦 等, | Wu et al., |
Wang et al., |
表1 支持面孔-和语音-人格知觉第一印象二维模型的相关研究
维度内容 | 面孔研究 | 语音研究 |
---|---|---|
“效价(valence)”和 “支配性(dominance)” | Jones et al., | Baus et al., McAleer et al., |
Oosterhof & Todorov, | ||
Morrison et al., | ||
Wang et al., | ||
“亲和性(approachability)”和“能力(capability)” | 吴琦 等, | Wu et al., |
Wang et al., |
[1] | 蔡佳烨, 郑燕, 葛列众, 刘宏艳. (2018). 自我面孔吸引力的评价偏向. 中国临床心理学杂志, 26(2), 215-219. |
[2] | 刘燊, 韩尚锋, 王秀娟, 李婉悦, 甘烨彤, 徐强, 张林. (2020). 面孔社会知觉模型发展的回顾与展望. 心理科学, 43(1), 132-143. |
[3] | 李雅宁, 蒋重清, 吴琦, 冷海州, 李东. (2020). 青年人高兴与中性表情对面孔社会知觉的影响. 中国心理卫生杂志, 34(7), 64-70. |
[4] | 李雅宁, 田杨阳, 吴琦, 冷海州, 蒋重清, 杨伊生. (2021). 面孔社会知觉中的表情效应. 心理科学进展, 29(6), 1022-1029. |
[5] | 潘哲, 郭永玉, 徐步霄, 杨沈龙. (2017). 人格研究中的 “能动” 与 “共生” 及其关系. 心理科学进展, 25(1), 99-110. |
[6] | 吴琦, 刘颖, 李东, 冷海州, 蒋重清. (2020). 中国面孔中的人格知觉维度. 心理学探新, 40(2), 177-182. |
[7] | 岳童, 黄希庭, 刘光远. (2017). 催产素影响人类社会认知的性别差异. 心理科学进展, 25(12), 2157-2167. |
[8] | 佐斌, 代涛涛, 温芳芳, 索玉贤. (2015). 社会认知内容的 “大二” 模型. 心理科学, 38(4), 1019-1023. |
[9] |
Abele, A. E., & Wojciszke, B. (2007). Agency and communion from the perspective of self versus others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93(5), 751-763.
doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.93.5.751 URL |
[10] | Abele, A. E., & Wojciszke, B. (2014). Communal and agentic content in social cognition: A dual perspective model. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 50, 195-255. |
[11] | Baus, C., McAleer, P., Marcoux, K., Belin, P., & Costa, A. (2019). Forming social impressions from voices in native and foreign languages. Scientific Reports, 9(1), 1-14. |
[12] |
Belin, P. (2017). Similarities in face and voice cerebral processing. Visual Cognition, 25(4-6), 658-665.
doi: 10.1080/13506285.2016.1244137 URL |
[13] |
Berry, D. S. (1990). Vocal attractiveness and vocal babyishness: Effects on stranger, self, and friend impressions. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 14(3), 141-153.
doi: 10.1007/BF00996223 URL |
[14] |
Bruckert, L., Liénard, J. S., Lacroix, A., Kreutzer, M., & Leboucher, G. (2006). Women use voice parameters to assess men's characteristics. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 273(1582), 83-89.
doi: 10.1098/rspb.2005.3265 URL |
[15] |
Campanella, S., & Belin, P. (2007). Integrating face and voice in person perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(12), 535-543.
pmid: 17997124 |
[16] | Chanes, L., Wormwood, J. B., Betz, N., & Barrett, L. F. (2018). Facial expression predictions as drivers of social perception. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 114(3), 380-396. |
[17] |
Cheng, J. T., Tracy, J. L., Ho, S., & Henrich, J. (2016). Listen, follow me: Dynamic vocal signals of dominance predict emergent social rank in humans. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145(5), 536-547.
doi: 10.1037/xge0000166 URL |
[18] |
Colonnello, V., Russo, P. M., & Mattarozzi, K. (2019). First impression misleads emotion recognition. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 527. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00527
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00527 URL pmid: 30949088 |
[19] |
DeBruine, L. M. (2005). Trustworthy but not lust-worthy: Context-specific effects of facial resemblance. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 272(1566), 919-922.
pmid: 16024346 |
[20] |
Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J., Glick, P., & Xu, J. (2002). A model of (often mixed) stereotype content: Competence and warmth respectively follow from perceived status and competition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82(6), 878-902.
doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.82.6.878 URL |
[21] |
Hahn, A. C., & Perrett, D. I. (2014). Neural and behavioral responses to attractiveness in adult and infant faces. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 46(4), 591-603.
doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.08.015 URL |
[22] |
Haselhuhn, M. P., & Wong, E. M. (2012). Bad to the bone: Facial structure predicts unethical behaviour. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 279(1728), 571-576.
doi: 10.1098/rspb.2011.1193 URL |
[23] |
Hensel, L., Bzdok, D., Müller, V. I., Zilles, K., & Eickhoff, S. B. (2015). Neural correlates of explicit social judgments on vocal stimuli. Cerebral Cortex, 25(5), 1152-1162.
doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht307 URL |
[24] |
Hodges-Simeon, C. R., Gurven, M., & Gaulin, S. J. (2015). The low male voice is a costly signal of phenotypic quality among Bolivian adolescents. Evolution and Human Behavior, 36(4), 294-302.
doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2015.01.002 URL |
[25] |
Hönekopp, J. (2006). Once more: Is beauty in the eye of the beholder? Relative contributions of private and shared taste to judgments of facial attractiveness. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32(2), 199-209.
doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.32.2.199 URL |
[26] |
Jessen, S., & Grossmann, T. (2016). Neural and behavioral evidence for infants’ sensitivity to the trustworthiness of faces. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 28, 1728-1736.
pmid: 27315276 |
[27] |
Jessen, S., & Grossmann, T. (2019). Neural evidence for the subliminal processing of facial trustworthiness in infancy. Neuropsychologia, 126, 46-53.
doi: S0028-3932(17)30154-9 pmid: 28442339 |
[28] |
Jones, B. C., DeBruine, L. M., Flake, J. K., Liuzza, M. T., Antfolk, J., Arinze, N. C., ... Coles, N. A. (2021). To which world regions does the valence-dominance model of social perception apply? Nature Human Behaviour, 5, 159-169.
doi: 10.1038/s41562-020-01007-2 URL |
[29] |
Krendl, A. C., Rule, N. O., & Ambady, N. (2014). Does aging impair first impression accuracy? Differentiating emotion recognition from complex social inferences. Psychology and Aging, 29(3), 482-490.
doi: 10.1037/a0037146 pmid: 25244469 |
[30] |
Lambert, W. E., Frankle, H., & Tucker, G. R. (1966). Judging personality through speech: A French-Canadian example. Journal of Communication, 16(4), 305-321.
pmid: 5980191 |
[31] | Leng, H., Liu, Y., Li, Q., Wu, Q., & Jiang, Z. (2020). Facial trustworthiness affects outcome evaluation: An event-related potential study. Neuro Report, 31(10), 741-745. |
[32] |
Mahrholz, G., Belin, P., & McAleer, P. (2018). Judgements of a speaker’s personality are correlated across differing content and stimulus type. Plos One, 13(10), e0204991. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0204991
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0204991 URL |
[33] |
Mazur, A., & Booth, A. (1998). Testosterone and dominance in men. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21(3), 353-363.
pmid: 10097017 |
[34] |
McAleer, P., Todorov, A., & Belin, P. (2014). How do you say ‘hello’? Personality impressions from brief novel voices. Plos One, 9(3), e90779. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0090779
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0090779 URL |
[35] |
Mclarney-Vesotski, A. R., Bernieri, F., & Rempala, D. (2006). Personality perception: A developmental study. (5), Journal of Research in Personality, 40(5), 652-674.
doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2005.07.001 URL |
[36] |
Mileva, M., Tompkinson, J., Watt, D., & Burton, A. M. (2018). Audiovisual integration in social evaluation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 44(1), 128-138.
doi: 10.1037/xhp0000439 URL |
[37] |
Morrison, D., Wang, H., Hahn, A. C., Jones, B. C., & DeBruine, L. M. (2017). Predicting the reward value of faces and bodies from social perception. Plos One, 12(9), e0185093. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0185093
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0185093 URL |
[38] |
Ohala, J. J. (1984). An ethological perspective on common cross-language utilization of F₀ of voice. Phonetica, 41(1), 1-16.
pmid: 6204347 |
[39] |
Oosterhof, N. N., & Todorov, A. (2008). The functional basis of face evaluation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 105(32), 11087-11092.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.0805664105 pmid: 18685089 |
[40] |
Over, H., & Cook, R. (2018). Where do spontaneous first impressions of faces come from? Cognition, 170, 190-200.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2017.10.002 URL |
[41] | Pao, T. L., Chen, Y. T., Yenand, J. H., & Liao, W. Y. (2005). Detecting emotions in mandarin speech. Computational Linguistics and Chinese Language Processing, 10(3), 347-362. |
[42] |
Peng, Z., Wang, Y., Meng, L., Liu, H., & Hu, Z. (2019). One’s own and similar voices are more attractive than other voices. Australian Journal of Psychology, 71(3), 212-222.
doi: 10.1111/ajpy.12235 URL |
[43] |
Perrett, D. I., Burt, D. M., Penton-Voak, I. S., Lee, K. J., Rowland, D. A., & Edwards, R. (1999). Symmetry and human facial attractiveness. Evolution and Human Behavior, 20(5), 295-307.
doi: 10.1016/S1090-5138(99)00014-8 URL |
[44] | Pisanski, K., & Bryant, G. A. (2019). The evolution of voice perception. In N. S. Eidsheim, & K. L. Meizel (Eds.), The oxford handbook of voice studies (pp.269-300). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. |
[45] | Puts, D. A., Hill, A. K., Bailey, D. H., Walker, R. S., Rendall, D., Wheatley, J. R., ... Ramos-Fernandez, G. (2016). Sexual selection on male vocal fundamental frequency in humans and other anthropoids. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 283(1829), 1-8. |
[46] |
Rezlescu, C., Penton, T., Walsh, V., Tsujimura, H., Scott, S. K., & Banissy, M. J. (2015). Dominant voices and attractive faces: The contribution of visual and auditory information to integrated person impressions. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 39(4), 355-370.
doi: 10.1007/s10919-015-0214-8 URL |
[47] |
South Palomares, J. K., & Young, A. W. (2018). Facial first impressions of partner preference traits: Trustworthiness, status, and attractiveness. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 9(8), 990-1000.
doi: 10.1177/1948550617732388 URL |
[48] |
Sutherland, C. A., Liu, X., Zhang, L., Chu, Y., Oldmeadow, J. A., & Young, A. W. (2018). Facial first impressions across culture: Data-driven modeling of Chinese and British perceivers’ unconstrained facial impressions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 44(4), 521-537.
doi: 10.1177/0146167217744194 pmid: 29226785 |
[49] |
Sutherland, C. A., Oldmeadow, J. A., Santos, I. M., Towler, J., Burt, D. M., & Young, A. W. (2013). Social inferences from faces: Ambient images generate a three-dimensional model. Cognition, 127(1), 105-118.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2012.12.001 pmid: 23376296 |
[50] |
Sutherland, C. A., Young, A. W., & Rhodes, G. (2017). Facial first impressions from another angle: How social judgements are influenced by changeable and invariant facial properties. British Journal of Psychology, 108(2), 397-415.
doi: 10.1111/bjop.12206 pmid: 27443971 |
[51] | Todorov, A., & Oh, D. (2021). The structure and perceptual basis of social judgments from faces. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 63, 189-245. |
[52] |
Tsantani, M. S., Belin, P., Paterson, H. M., & McAleer, P. (2016). Low vocal pitch preference drives first impressions irrespective of context in male voices but not in female voices. Perception, 45(8), 946-963.
doi: 10.1177/0301006616643675 pmid: 27081101 |
[53] |
Wang, H., Hahn, A. C., DeBruine, L. M., & Jones, B. C. (2016). The motivational salience of faces is related to both their valence and dominance. Plos One, 11(8), e0161114. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0161114
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0161114 URL |
[54] |
Wang, H., Han, C., Hahn, A. C., Fasolt, V., Morrison, D. K., Holzleitner, I. J., ... Jones, B. C. (2019). A data-driven study of Chinese participants' social judgments of Chinese faces. Plos One, 14(1), e0210315. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0210315
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0210315 URL |
[55] | Wu, Q., Liu, Y., Li, D., Leng, H., & Jiang, Z. (2021). Understanding one’s character through the voice: Dimensions of personality perception from Chinese greeting word "Ni Hao". The Journal of Social Psychology, 1, 1-11. |
[56] |
Young, A. W., Frühholz, S., & Schweinberger, S. R. (2020). Face and voice perception: Understanding commonalities and differences. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 24(5), 398-410.
doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2020.02.001 URL |
[57] |
Yovel, G., & Belin, P. (2013). A unified coding strategy for processing faces and voices. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(6), 263-271.
doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.04.004 URL |
[58] | Zebrowitz, L. A. (2004). The origin of first impressions. Journal of Cultural and Evolutionary Psychology, 2(1-2), 93-108. |
[59] |
Zebrowitz, L. A., Fellous, J. M., Mignault, A., & Andreoletti, C. (2003). Trait impressions as overgeneralized responses to adaptively significant facial qualities: Evidence from connectionist modeling. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 7(3), 194-215.
pmid: 12788687 |
[60] |
Zebrowitz, L. A., Franklin, R. G., & Boshyan, J. (2015). Face shape and behavior: Implications of similarities in infants and adults. Personality and Individual Differences, 86, 312-317.
pmid: 26217067 |
[61] |
Zebrowitz, L. A., & Montepare, J. M. (1989). Contributions of a babyface and a childlink voice to impressions of moving and talking faces. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 13(3), 189-203.
doi: 10.1007/BF00987049 URL |
[62] |
Zhang, H., Liu, M., Li, W., & Sommer, W. (2020). Human voice attractiveness processing: Electrophysiological evidence. Biological Psychology, 150, 107827. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2019.107827
doi: S0301-0511(19)30611-8 URL pmid: 31756365 |
[63] | Ziomkiewicz-Wichary, A. (2016). Serotonin and dominance. In T. Shackelford, & V. Weekes-Shackelford (Eds.), Encyclopedia of evolutionary psychological science (pp.1-4). Springer, Cham. |
[64] |
Zuckerman, M., & Driver, R. E. (1989). What sounds beautiful is good: The vocal attractiveness stereotype. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 13(2), 67-82.
doi: 10.1007/BF00990791 URL |
[65] |
Zuckerman, M., & Sinicropi, V. (2011). When physical and vocal attractiveness differ: Effects on favorability of interpersonal impressions. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 35(2), 75-86.
doi: 10.1007/s10919-011-0106-5 URL |
[1] | 陈子炜, 付迪, 刘勋. 错认总比错过好——面孔视错觉的发生机制及其应用[J]. 心理科学进展, 2023, 31(2): 240-255. |
[2] | 周国梅, 郑若颖, 林佳, 刘心阁. 面孔吸引力的整体表征及其动态性增强[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(7): 1429-1438. |
[3] | 齐玥, 秦邵天, 王可昕, 陈文锋. 面孔可信度评价调节:经验迁移假说的提出与验证[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(4): 715-722. |
[4] | 刘俊材, 冉光明, 张琪. 不同情绪载体的神经活动及其异同——脑成像研究的ALE元分析[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(3): 536-555. |
[5] | 张超, 卫旭华, 黎英明. 面相识人:基于认知视角的解释[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(2): 308-323. |
[6] | 朱传林, 刘电芝, 罗文波. 情绪体验影响估算策略运用的认知与脑机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(12): 2639-2649. |
[7] | 田珈源, 龙彦伶, 杨虹, 吴惠芳, 薛鹏, 蒋重清. 面孔-人格知觉中眼部线索及其效应[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(12): 2735-2745. |
[8] | 马亚男, 黄艳利, 石宇婧, 谢久书. 语音象征的产生机制:基于敏感期的先天后天作用模型[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(11): 2487-2496. |
[9] | 侯文霞, 田欣然, 刘立志, 易冰, 欧玉晓, 陈文锋, 尚俊辰. 面孔吸引力同化的连续性效应[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(7): 1210-1215. |
[10] | 荆伟, 张婕, 付锦霞, 田琳, 赵微. 婴幼儿面孔注意偏向:先天倾向与发展轨迹——来自正常和孤独症婴幼儿的证据[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(7): 1216-1230. |
[11] | 李雅宁, 田杨阳, 吴琦, 冷海州, 蒋重清, 杨伊生. 面孔社会知觉中的表情效应[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(6): 1022-1029. |
[12] | 何蔚祺, 李帅霞, 赵东方. 群体面孔情绪感知的神经机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(5): 761-772. |
[13] | 霍鹏辉, 冯成志, 陈庭继. 注视者及观察者因素对注视知觉的影响[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(2): 238-251. |
[14] | 冯杰, 徐娟, 伍新春. 视觉经验缺失对盲人听觉词汇识别的影响[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(12): 2131-2146. |
[15] | 明莉莉, 胡学平. 人类嗓音加工的神经机制——来自正常视力者和盲人的脑神经证据[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(12): 2147-2160. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||