Acta Psychologica Sinica ›› 2022, Vol. 54 ›› Issue (6): 595-603.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2022.00595
• Reports of Empirical Studies • Previous Articles Next Articles
PAN Yuean1, JIANG Yunpeng1,2,3, GUO Maojie1, WU Xia1,2,3,4()
Received:
2021-05-02
Online:
2022-04-26
Contact:
WU Xia
E-mail:wuxia@tjnu.edu.cn
Supported by:
PAN Yuean, JIANG Yunpeng, GUO Maojie, WU Xia. (2022). The influence of uncertainty and validity of expectation on the perceptual decision of motion direction. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 54(6), 595-603.
Add to citation manager EndNote|Ris|BibTeX
URL: https://journal.psych.ac.cn/acps/EN/10.3724/SP.J.1041.2022.00595
PR | High uncertainty | Low uncertainty |
---|---|---|
High validity | 57.06 ± 17.41 | 91.49 ± 2.43 |
Low validity | 46.97 ± 9.60 | 89.9 ± 3.35 |
Table 1. The response accuracy PR (M ± SD, %) under different conditions
PR | High uncertainty | Low uncertainty |
---|---|---|
High validity | 57.06 ± 17.41 | 91.49 ± 2.43 |
Low validity | 46.97 ± 9.60 | 89.9 ± 3.35 |
[1] |
Aitchison, L., & Lengyel, M. (2017). With or without you: Predictive coding and Bayesian inference in the brain. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 46, 219-227.
doi: S0959-4388(17)30045-4 pmid: 28942084 |
[2] |
Aitken, F., Turner, G., & Kok, P. (2020). Prior expectations of motion direction modulate early sensory processing. Journal of Neuroscience, 40(33), 6389-6397.
doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0537-20.2020 URL |
[3] |
Barbosa, L. S., Vlassova, A., & Kouider, S. (2017). Prior expectations modulate unconscious evidence accumulation. Consciousness and Cognition, 51, 236-242.
doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2017.04.001 URL |
[4] |
Botvinick, M. M., Carter, C. S., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review, 108(3), 624-652.
pmid: 11488380 |
[5] |
Brainard, D. H. (1997). The psychophysics toolbox. Spatial vision, 10(4), 433-436.
pmid: 9176952 |
[6] | Braver, T. S. (2012). The variable nature of cognitive control: A dual mechanisms framework. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(2), 105-112. |
[7] |
Burgess, G. C., & Braver, T. S. (2010). Neural mechanisms of interference control in working memory: Effects of interference expectancy and fluid intelligence. PloS One, 5(9), e12861. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0012861
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0012861 URL |
[8] | Chalk, M., Seitz, A. R., & Seriès, P. (2010). Rapidly learned stimulus expectations alter perception of motion. Journal of Vision, 10(8), 1-18. |
[9] |
Clark, A. (2013). Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(3), 181-204.
doi: 10.1017/S0140525X12000477 pmid: 23663408 |
[10] |
Cullen, K. E., & Taube, J. S. (2017). Our sense of direction: Progress, controversies and challenges. Nature Neuroscience, 20(11), 1465-1473.
doi: 10.1038/nn.4658 URL |
[11] |
de Lange, F. P., Heilbron, M., & Kok, P. (2018). How do expectations shape perception?. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22(9), 764-779.
doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2018.06.002 URL |
[12] | Dogge, M., Custers, R., Gayet, S., Hoijtink, H., & Aarts, H. (2019). Perception of action-outcomes is shaped by life-long and contextual expectations. Scientific Reports, 9(1), 1-9. |
[13] |
Endsley, M. R. (1995). Toward a theory of situation awareness in dynamic systems. Human Factors, 37(1), 32-64.
doi: 10.1518/001872095779049543 URL |
[14] | Fan, J. (2014). An information theory account of cognitive control. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 680. |
[15] | Gekas, N., Chalk, M., Seitz, A. R., & Seriès, P. (2013). Complexity and specificity of experimentally-induced expectations in motion perception. Journal of Vision, 13(4), 1-18. |
[16] | Hauser, C. K., & Salinas, E. (2015). Perceptual decision making. In Encyclopedia of Computational Neuroscience (pp.1-21). Springer New York. |
[17] |
Ho, C. S., & Giaschi, D. E. (2009). Low- and high-level first-order random-dot kinematograms: Evidence from fMRI. Vision Research, 49(14), 1814-1824.
doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2009.04.018 URL |
[18] |
Kayser, A. S., Buchsbaum, B. R., Erickson, D. T., & D’Esposito, M. (2010). The functional anatomy of a perceptual decision in the human brain. Journal of Neurophysiology, 103(3), 1179-1194.
doi: 10.1152/jn.00364.2009 URL |
[19] |
Kayser, A. S., Erickson, D. T., Buchsbaum, B. R., & D’Esposito, M. (2010). Neural representations of relevant and irrelevant features in perceptual decision making. Journal of Neuroscience, 30(47), 15778-15789.
doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3163-10.2010 URL |
[20] | Kayser, S. J., & Kayser, C. (2020). Shared physiological correlates of multisensory and expectation-based facilitation. ENeuro, 7(2), 1-13. |
[21] |
Kok, P., Brouwer, G. J., van Gerven, M. A. J., & de Lange, F. P. (2013). Prior expectations bias sensory representations in visual cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 33(41), 16275-16284.
doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0742-13.2013 URL |
[22] |
Liu, Q., Hu, Z. H., Zhao, G., Tao, W. D., Zhang, Q. L., & Sun, H. J. (2010). The prior knowledge of the reliability of sensory cues affects the multisensory integration in the early perceptual processing stage. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 42(2), 227-234.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2010.00227 URL |
[23] | Mackie, M.-A., & Fan, J. (2017). Functional neuroimaging of deficits in cognitive control. In E. Goldberg (Ed.), Executive functions in health and disease (pp.249-300). Elsevier Academic Press. |
[24] |
Matthews, N., & Qian, N. (1999). Axis-of-motion affects direction discrimination, not speed discrimination. Vision Research, 39(13), 2205-2211.
pmid: 10343802 |
[25] |
Moser, E. I., Moser, M.-B., & McNaughton, B. L. (2017). Spatial representation in the hippocampal formation: A history. Nature Neuroscience, 20(11), 1448-1464.
doi: 10.1038/nn.4653 URL |
[26] | Mushtaq, F., Bland, A. R., & Schaefer, A. (2011). Uncertainty and cognitive control. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 1-14. |
[27] |
Newsome, W. T., & Paré, E. B. (1998). A selective impairment of motion perception following lesions of the middle temporal visual area (MT). The Journal of Neuroscience, 8(6), 2201-2211.
doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.08-06-02201.1988 URL |
[28] |
Pelly, D. G., & Vision, S. (1997). The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: Transforming numbers into movies. Spatial Vision, 10, 437-442.
doi: 10.1163/156856897X00366 URL |
[29] | Piedimonte, A., J. Woods, A., & Chatterjee, A. (2015). Disambiguating ambiguous motion perception: What are the cues?. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1-13. |
[30] |
Pilly, P. K., & Seitz, A. R. (2009). What a difference a parameter makes: A psychophysical comparison of random dot motion algorithms. Vision Research, 49(13), 1599-1612.
doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2009.03.019 URL |
[31] | Ratcliff, R., & McKoon, G. (2008). The diffusion decision model: Theory and data for two-choice decision tasks. European Journal of Pharmacology, 20(4), 873-922. |
[32] |
Singh, K. D., & Fawcett, I. P. (2008). Transient and linearly graded deactivation of the human default-mode network by a visual detection task. NeuroImage, 41(1), 100-112.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.01.051 pmid: 18375149 |
[33] |
Sotiropoulos, G., Seitz, A. R., & Seriès, P. (2011). Changing expectations about speed alters perceived motion direction. Current Biology, 21(21), 883-884.
doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.09.013 pmid: 22075425 |
[34] |
Speer, N. K., Jacoby, L. L., & Braver, T. S. (2003). Strategy-dependent changes in memory: Effects on behavior and brain activity. Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience, 3, 155-167.
doi: 10.3758/CABN.3.3.155 URL |
[35] | Urgen, B. M., & Boyaci, H. (2019). When expectations are not met: Unraveling the computational mechanisms underlying the effect of expectation on perceptual thresholds. BioRxiv, 2-3. |
[36] |
van Maanen, L., Grasman, R. P., Forstmann, B. U., Keuken, M. C., Brown, S. D., & Wagenmakers, E. J. (2012). Similarity and number of alternatives in the random-dot motion paradigm. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 74(4), 739-753.
doi: 10.3758/s13414-011-0267-7 URL |
[37] |
Vlakveld, W., van Nes, N., de Bruin, J., Vissers, L., & van der Kroft, M. (2018). Situation awareness increases when drivers have more time to take over the wheel in a Level 3 automated car: A simulator study. Transportation Research part F: traffic psychology and behaviour, 58, 917-929.
doi: 10.1016/j.trf.2018.07.025 URL |
[38] | Wu, T., Chen, C., Spagna, A., Wu, X., Mackie, M.-A., Russell-Giller, S., ... Fan, J. (2019). The functional anatomy of cognitive control: A domain-general brain network for uncertainty processing. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 528(8), 1265-1292. |
[39] | Yu, A. J., & Dayan, P. (2005). Uncertainty, neuromodulation, and attention. Journal of Sensory Studies, 46(4), 681-692. |
[40] | Chen, X., Yang, J., Oeser, M., & Wang, H.(Eds.). (2020). Functional Pavements. CRC Press. https://doi.org/10.1201/9781003156222 |
[41] |
Zhang, L. J., Lu, J. Q., Wei, X. Y., & Pan, J. H. (2019). Bayesian structural equation modeling and its current researches. Advances in Psychological Science, 27(11), 1812-1825.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2019.01812 URL |
[1] | CHEN Li, SHI Xiao-ke, LI Wei-na, HU Yan. Influence of cognitive control based on different conflict levels on the expression of gender stereotypes [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2022, 54(6): 628-645. |
[2] | LI Jianhua, XIE Jiajia, ZHUANG Jin-Ying. An effect of menstrual cycle phase on episodic memory [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2022, 54(5): 466-480. |
[3] | WANG Yan, JIANG Jing. The effect of environmental disorderliness on variety seeking behavior and its mechanism [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2022, 54(1): 78-90. |
[4] | ZHANG Mengke, LI Qing, YIN Shouhang, CHEN Antao. Changes in the level of conflict trigger conflict adaptation [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2021, 53(2): 128-138. |
[5] | LIN Rang, YANG Yimiao. Effect of ambivalent attitudes on post-decision self-evaluation: Two-stage moderation effect with a mediator [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2021, 53(12): 1348-1360. |
[6] | HUANG Yuesheng, ZHANG Bao, FAN Xinhua, HUANG Jie. Can negative emotion of task-irrelevant working memory representation affect its attentional capture? A study of eye movements [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2021, 53(1): 26-37. |
[7] | SUN Yan, LV Jiaojiao, LAN Fan, ZHANG Lina. Emotion regulation strategy of self-focused and situation-focused reappraisal and their impact on subsequent cognitive control [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2020, 52(12): 1393-1406. |
[8] | ZHANG Hong,REN jingyuan,LIU Chenyang,LUO Jing. Conformity effect of the evaluation of creative products [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2019, 51(6): 688-698. |
[9] | Gerd GIGERENZER, LUAN Shenghua, LIU Yongfang. Are we truly irrational and almost impossible to educate? Analyzing the scientific evidence behind libertine paternalism [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2019, 51(4): 395-406. |
[10] | Xiao Tian WANG. Using behavioral economics to cope with uncertainty: Expand the scope of effective nudging [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2019, 51(4): 407-414. |
[11] | CUI Yichen, WANG Pei, CUI Yajuan. Cognitive control strategies from the perspective of perceptual conflict: An example of stereotyped information and counterstereotyped information [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2019, 51(10): 1157-1170. |
[12] | LYU Xiaokang, FU Chunye, WANG Xinjian. Effect and underlying mechanism of refutation texts on the trust and moral judgment of patients [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2019, 51(10): 1171-1186. |
[13] | WANG Yanqing, CHEN Antao, HU Xueping, YIN Shouhang. Reward improves cognitive control by enhancing signal monitoring [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2019, 51(1): 48-57. |
[14] | HU Cenlou, ZHANG Bao, HUANG Sai. Does irrelevant long-term memory representation guide the deployment of visual attention? [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2017, 49(5): 590-601. |
[15] | ZHANG Bao, HU Cenlou, Huang Sai. What do eye movements reveal about the role of cognitive control in attention guidance from working memory representation [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2016, 48(9): 1105-1118. |
Viewed | ||||||
Full text |
|
|||||
Abstract |
|
|||||