Acta Psychologica Sinica ›› 2022, Vol. 54 ›› Issue (5): 481-496.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2022.00481
• Reports of Empirical Studies • Previous Articles Next Articles
FAN Wei1,2, REN Mengmeng1,2, ZHANG Wenjie2,3, ZHONG Yiping1,2,*()
Published:
2022-05-25
Online:
2022-03-23
Contact:
ZHONG Yiping
E-mail:ypzhong@hunnu.edu.cn
Supported by:
FAN Wei, REN Mengmeng, ZHANG Wenjie, ZHONG Yiping. (2022). The impact of feedback on self-deception: Evidence from ERP. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 54(5), 481-496.
Add to citation manager EndNote|Ris|BibTeX
URL: https://journal.psych.ac.cn/acps/EN/10.3724/SP.J.1041.2022.00481
Score | M ± SD | t | p |
---|---|---|---|
Predicted Scores for test 2 | |||
No Answer Prompt Group | 48.36 ± 13.34 | -4.83*** | 0.00 |
Answer Prompt group | 65.36 ± 13.88 | ||
Actual score for test 2 | |||
No Answer Prompt Group | 53.82 ± 6.95 | -0.22 | 0.83 |
Answer Prompt group | 54.26 ± 8.91 | ||
The difference between the predicted score and the actual score of test 2 | |||
No Answer Prompt Group | -4.95 ± 14.85 | 2.11* | 0.04 |
Answer Prompt group | 9.80 ± 16.95 |
Table 1 The scores of the first stage of test 2 in the no answer prompt group and the answer prompt group (M ± SD)
Score | M ± SD | t | p |
---|---|---|---|
Predicted Scores for test 2 | |||
No Answer Prompt Group | 48.36 ± 13.34 | -4.83*** | 0.00 |
Answer Prompt group | 65.36 ± 13.88 | ||
Actual score for test 2 | |||
No Answer Prompt Group | 53.82 ± 6.95 | -0.22 | 0.83 |
Answer Prompt group | 54.26 ± 8.91 | ||
The difference between the predicted score and the actual score of test 2 | |||
No Answer Prompt Group | -4.95 ± 14.85 | 2.11* | 0.04 |
Answer Prompt group | 9.80 ± 16.95 |
Figure 2. Predicted scores of fuzzy feedback with different valence in the second stage of test 2; The error bar represents the standard error, *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001, the same below.
Figure 3. The experimental flow figure of Experiment 2. A. Experimental flow chart of test 1; B. Experimental flow chart of test 2; C. Three Feedback Conditions
Score | M ± SD | t | p |
---|---|---|---|
Predicted scores for test 2 | |||
No answer prompt group | 52.93 ± 16.59 | -2.89* | 0.006 |
Answer prompt group | 64.57 ± 13.66 | ||
Actual score for test 2 | |||
No answer prompt group | 53.72 ± 7.53 | -0.52 | 0.610 |
Answer prompt group | 54.82 ± 8.50 | ||
The difference between the predicted score and the actual score of test 2 | |||
No answer prompt group | 0.79 ± 19.57 | -2.12* | 0.038 |
Answer prompt group | 9.75 ± 147.91 |
Table 2 The scores of the first stage of test 2 in the no answer prompt group and the answer prompt group (M ± SD)
Score | M ± SD | t | p |
---|---|---|---|
Predicted scores for test 2 | |||
No answer prompt group | 52.93 ± 16.59 | -2.89* | 0.006 |
Answer prompt group | 64.57 ± 13.66 | ||
Actual score for test 2 | |||
No answer prompt group | 53.72 ± 7.53 | -0.52 | 0.610 |
Answer prompt group | 54.82 ± 8.50 | ||
The difference between the predicted score and the actual score of test 2 | |||
No answer prompt group | 0.79 ± 19.57 | -2.12* | 0.038 |
Answer prompt group | 9.75 ± 147.91 |
Group | N | M ± SD | F | p |
---|---|---|---|---|
The answer prompt group in Experiment 1 | 35 | 83.20 ± 12.10 | 0.23 | 0.80 |
The answer prompt group in Experiment 2 | 33 | 81.77 ± 11.72 | ||
The answer prompt group in Experiment 3 | 35 | 81.40 ± 11.45 |
Table 3 The scores of the three experiments (the answer prompt group) on the self-deception scale (M ± SD)
Group | N | M ± SD | F | p |
---|---|---|---|---|
The answer prompt group in Experiment 1 | 35 | 83.20 ± 12.10 | 0.23 | 0.80 |
The answer prompt group in Experiment 2 | 33 | 81.77 ± 11.72 | ||
The answer prompt group in Experiment 3 | 35 | 81.40 ± 11.45 |
Group | N | M ± SD | F | p |
---|---|---|---|---|
No answer prompt group in Experiment 1 | 35 | 48.36 ± 13.34 | 12.50*** | 0.00 |
No answer prompt group in Experiment 2 | 33 | 52.93 ± 16.59 | ||
The answer prompt group in Experiment 3 | 35 | 81.40 ± 11.45 |
Table 4 Comparison of the results of the first stage of test 2 in the three experiments
Group | N | M ± SD | F | p |
---|---|---|---|---|
No answer prompt group in Experiment 1 | 35 | 48.36 ± 13.34 | 12.50*** | 0.00 |
No answer prompt group in Experiment 2 | 33 | 52.93 ± 16.59 | ||
The answer prompt group in Experiment 3 | 35 | 81.40 ± 11.45 |
Figure 8. Total ERP average graph, brain topographic map and histogram of N1 component average at all analysis points for four conditions at Fz and Fcz electrode points (time window: 0~50 ms), *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001.
Figure 9. Total mean ERP graph, brain topographic map and histogram of mean P2 component of all analysis points at Fz and Fcz electrodes (100~250 ms) for four conditions, *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001.
Figure 10. Total ERP mean graph, brain topographic map and histogram of N400 component mean at all analysis points for four conditions at Fz and Fcz electrode points (300~500 ms), *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001
[1] |
Bandura A. (2011). Self-deception: A paradox revisited. Behavioral Brain Sciences, 34(1), 16-17.
doi: 10.1017/S0140525X10002499 URL |
[2] | Bayne T., & Fernández J. (2009). Delusion and self-deception:Mapping the terrain. In T. Bayne & J. Fernandez (Eds.), Delusion and self-deception: Affective and motivational influences on belief formation (pp.1-21). Psychology Press. |
[3] |
Chance Z., Gino F., Norton M. I., & Ariely D. (2015). The slow decay and quick revival of self-deception. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1075.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01075 pmid: 26347666 |
[4] |
Chance Z., & Norton M. I. (2015). The what and why of self-deception. Current Opinion in Psychology, 6, 104-107.
doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.07.008 URL |
[5] |
Chance Z., Norton M. I., Gino F., & Ariely D. (2011). Temporal view of the costs and benefits of self-deception. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(Suppl. 3), 15655-15659.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1010658108 URL |
[6] | Chen T. B., & Chai L. T. (2010). Attitude towards the environment and green products: Consumers’ perspective. Management Science and Engineering, 4(2), 27-39. |
[7] |
Dana J., Cain D. M., & Dawes R. M. (2006). What you don’t know won’t hurt me: Costly (but quiet) exit in dictator games. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 100(2), 193-201.
doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2005.10.001 URL |
[8] |
Delorme A., & Makeig S. (2004). EEGLAB: An open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 134(1), 9-21.
doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009 pmid: 15102499 |
[9] |
Dijkstra P. D., Schaafsma S. M., Hofmann H. A., & Groothuis T. G. (2012). ‘Winner effect’ without winning: Unresolved social conflicts increase the probability of winning a subsequent contest in a cichlid fish. Physiology & Behavior, 105(2), 489-492.
doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.08.029 URL |
[10] |
Duran K. A., O’Halloran H., Soder H., Yasin S., Kramer R., Rosen S., Brenya J., Chavarria K., Savitska L., & Keenan J. P. (2020). The medial prefrontal cortex: A potential link between self-deception and affect. International Journal of Neuroscience, 131(7), 701-707.
doi: 10.1080/00207454.2020.1753729 URL |
[11] |
Egerton A., Rees E., Bose S. K., Lappin J. M., Stokes P., & Turkheimer F. E., Reeves S. J. (2010). Truth, lies or self-deception? striatal d (2/3) receptor availability predicts individual differences in social conformity. NeuroImage, 53(2), 777-781.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.06.031 pmid: 20558302 |
[12] |
Fan W., Yang B., Liu J., & Fu X. (2017). Self-deception: For adjusting individual psychological states. Advances in Psychological Science, 25(8), 1349-1359.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2017.01349 URL |
[13] |
Farrow T. F. D., Burgess J., Wilkinson I. D., & Hunter M. D. (2015). Neural correlates of self-deception and impression-management. Neuropsychologia, 67, 159-174.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.12.016 pmid: 25527112 |
[14] |
Faul F., Erdfelder E., Buchner A., & Lang A.-G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behavior Research Methods, 41(4), 1149-1160.
doi: 10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149 URL |
[15] |
Federmeier K. D., & Kutas M. (2001). Meaning and modality: Influences of context, semantic memory organization, and perceptual predictability on picture processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27(1), 202-224.
doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.27.1.202 URL |
[16] |
Foster G., & Frijters P. (2014). The formation of expectations: Competing theories and new evidence. Journal of Behavioral and Experimental Economics, 53, 66-81.
doi: 10.1016/j.socec.2014.08.001 URL |
[17] | Galeotti A. E. (2016). Straight and twisted self-deception. Phenomenology and Mind, 11, 90-99. |
[18] |
Gao J. (2021). Self-deception and pragmatic encroachment: A dilemma for epistemic rationality. Ratio, 34(1), 20-32.
doi: 10.1111/rati.12288 URL |
[19] |
Gneezy U., Saccardo S., & van Veldhuizen R. (2019). Bribery: Behavioral drivers of distorted decisions. Journal of the European Economic Association, 17(3), 917-946.
doi: 10.1093/jeea/jvy043 |
[20] |
Grossman Z., & van der Weele J. J. (2017). Self-image and willful ignorance in social decisions. Journal of the European Economic Association, 15(1), 173-217.
doi: 10.1093/jeea/jvw001 URL |
[21] | Gur R. C., & Sackeim H. A. (1979). Self-deception: A concept in search of a phenomenon. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 37(2), 147-169. |
[22] | Haisley E. C., & Weber R. A. (2010). Self-serving interpretations of ambiguity in other-regarding behavior. Games & Economic Behavior, 68(2), 614-625. |
[23] |
Holroyd C. B., Hajcak G., & Larsen J. T. (2006). The good, the bad and the neutral: Electrophysiological responses to feedback stimuli. Brain Research, 1105(1), 93-101.
pmid: 16427615 |
[24] | Jian Z., Zhang W., Tian L., Fan W., & Zhong Y. (2019). Self-deception reduces cognitive load: The role of involuntary conscious memory impairment. Frontiers in Psychology, 10. |
[25] | Ju S. E., Zhao Y., & Fu X. (2003). Cognitive mechanism of self- deception. Journal of Sun Yat-sen University (Social Science Edition), 43(5), 19-26. |
[26] |
Kunda Z. (1990). The case for motivated reasoning. Psychological Bulletin, 108(3), 480-498.
pmid: 2270237 |
[27] |
Kutas M., & Federmeier K. D. (2011). Thirty years and counting: Finding meaning in the N400 component of the event-related brain potential (ERP). Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 621-647.
doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.131123 URL |
[28] |
Kutas M., & Hillyard S. A. (1980). Reading senseless sentences: Brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity. Science, 207(4427): 203-205.
pmid: 7350657 |
[29] |
Lai V. T., & Huettig F. (2016). When prediction is fulfilled: Insight from emotion processing. Neuropsychologia, 85, 110-117.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.03.014 URL |
[30] | Lauria F., & Preissmann D. (2018). What does emotion teach us about self-deception? Affective neuroscience in support of non-intentionalism. In Les ateliers de l'éthique/The Ethics Forum (Vol. 13, No. 2, pp. 70-94). Centre de recherche en éthique de l’Université de Montréal. |
[31] |
Liu J., Zhang W., Zhan Y., Song L., Guan P., Kang D. Jian J., Cai R., & Li M. (2019). The effect of negative feedback on positive beliefs in self-deception. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 702.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00702 URL |
[32] | Liu Y., & Shu H. (2003). ERP and language studies. Advances in Psychological Science, 11(3), 296-302. |
[33] |
Lopez J. K., & Fuxjager M. J. (2012). Self-deception’s adaptive value: Effects of positive thinking and the winner effect. Consciousness and Cognition, 21(1), 315-324.
doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2011.10.008 URL |
[34] | Mele A. R. (1997a). Real self-deception. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 20, 91-136. |
[35] | Mele A. R. (1997b). Understanding and explaining real self-deception. Behavioral Brain Sciences, 20(1), 127-134. |
[36] |
Näätänen R., Paavilainen P., Rinne T., & Alho K. (2007). The mismatch negativity (MMN) in basic research of central auditory processing: A review. Clinical Neurophysiology, 118(12), 2544-2590.
pmid: 17931964 |
[37] |
Neuber S. (2016). Self-awareness and self-deception: A Sartrean perspective. Continental Philosophy Review, 49(4), 485-507.
doi: 10.1007/s11007-016-9368-2 URL |
[38] |
Nieuwenhuis S., Aston-Jones G., & Cohen J. D. (2005). Decision making, the P3, and the locus coeruleus-norepinephrine system. Psychological Bulletin, 131(4), 510-532.
pmid: 16060800 |
[39] |
Paulmann S., & Kotz S. A. (2008). Early emotional prosody perception based on different speaker voices. Neuroreport, 19(2), 209-213.
doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e3282f454db pmid: 18185110 |
[40] | Plöchl M., Ossandón J. P., & König P. (2012). Combining EEG and eye tracking: Identification, characterization, and correction of eye movement artifacts in electroencephalographic data. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 278. |
[41] |
Quattrone G. A., & Tversky A. (1984). Causal versus diagnostic contingencies: On self-deception and on the voter's illusion. Journal of personality social psychology, 46(2), 237-248.
doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.46.2.237 URL |
[42] | Ren M., Zhong B., Fan W., Dai H., Yang B., Zhang W., Yin Z., Liu J., Li J., & Zhan Y. (2018). The influence of self-control and social status on self-deception. Frontier in Psychology, 9, 1256. |
[43] |
Rottenburger J. R., Carter C. R., & Kaufmann L. (2019). It’s alright, it’s just a bluff: Why do corporate codes reduce lying, but not bluffing?. Journal of Purchasing and Supply Management, 25(1), 30-39.
doi: 10.1016/j.pursup.2018.02.004 |
[44] |
Schapkin S. A., Gusev A. N., & Kuhl J. (2000). Categorization of unilaterally presented emotional words: An ERP analysis. Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis, 60(1), 17-28.
pmid: 10769926 |
[45] |
Schwardmann P., & van der Weele J. (2019). Deception and self-deception. Nature Human Behaviour, 3(10), 1055-1061.
doi: 10.1038/s41562-019-0666-7 pmid: 31358973 |
[46] |
Sharma M., Modi S., Khushu S., & Mandal M. K. (2010). Neural activation pattern in self-deceivers. Psychological Studies, 55(1), 71-76.
doi: 10.1007/s12646-010-0008-3 URL |
[47] | Sheridan Z., Boman P., Mergler A., & Furlong M. J. (2015). Examining well-being, anxiety, and self-deception in university students. Cogent Psychology, 2(1), 993850. |
[48] |
Sloman S. A., Fernbach P. M., & Hagmayer Y. (2010). Self-deception requires vagueness. Cognition, 115(2), 268-281.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.12.017 URL |
[49] |
Smith M. K., Trivers R., & von Hippel W. (2017). Self-deception facilitates interpersonal persuasion. Journal of Economic Psychology, 63, 93-101.
doi: 10.1016/j.joep.2017.02.012 URL |
[50] |
Stahl J., Wiese H., & Schweinberger S. R. (2010). Learning task affects ERP-correlates of the own-race bias, but not recognition memory performance. Neuropsychologia, 48(7), 2027-2040.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.03.024 URL |
[51] |
Surbey M. K. (2011). Adaptive significance of low levels of self-deception and cooperation in depression. Evolution and Human Behavior, 32(1), 29-40.
doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2010.08.009 URL |
[52] | Tan C., Zhu C., & Song H. (2020). The effect of emotional valency and emotional arousal on self-deception. Journal of Neijiang Normal University, 35(10), 1-6. |
[53] |
Taylor S. E., & Brown J. D. (1988). Illusion and well-being: A social psychological perspective on mental health. Psychological Bulletin, 103(2), 193-210.
pmid: 3283814 |
[54] | Uziel L., & Cohen B. (2020). Self-deception and discrepancies in self-evaluation. Journal of Research in Personality, 88, 1-5. |
[55] | von Hippel W., & Trivers R. (2011). The evolution and psychology of self-deception. Behavioral & Brain Science, 34(1), 1-16; discussion 16-56. |
[56] |
Wang Y., Kuhlman D. M., Roberts K., Yuan B., Zhang Z., Zhang W., & Simons R. F. (2017). Social value orientation modulates the FRN and P300 in the Chicken Game. Biological Psychology, 127, 89-98.
doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2017.04.012 URL |
[57] | Zhong L., & Mo L. (2019). A commentary on empirical study of self-deception. Journal Psychological Science, 42(3), 709-714. |
[58] | Zhong L., & Ru T. (2021). A Cognitive processing model of self-deception. Journal of South China Normal University (Social Science Edition), (3), 81-89. |
[59] |
Zhong L., Ru T., Fan M., & Mo L. (2019). The effect of cognitive vagueness and motivation on conscious and unconscious self-deception. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 51(12), 1330-1340.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2019.01330 URL |
[60] | Zhu J., & Jiang J. (2014). Self-deception: A new topic in contemporary psychology. Science of Social Psychology, 2, 53-58. |
[61] |
Zhu Y., Xi S., & Wu Y. (2016). Self-deception in autobiographical narrative: Individuals’ self-identity strategy. Advances in Psychological Science, 24(12), 1917-1925.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2016.01917 URL |
[1] | ZHAN Youlong, XIAO Xiao, TAN Qianbao, LI Jin, ZHONG Yiping. Influence of reputational concern and social distance on moral decision-making under the harmful dilemma: Evidence from behavioral and ERPs study [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2022, 54(6): 613-627. |
[2] | LI Jianhua, XIE Jiajia, ZHUANG Jin-Ying. An effect of menstrual cycle phase on episodic memory [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2022, 54(5): 466-480. |
[3] | LUO Wenbo, QI Zhengyang. The influence of concreteness on emotional nouns valence processing: An ERP study [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2022, 54(2): 111-121. |
[4] | CHEN Jie, WU Ke, SHI Yupeng, AI Xiaoqing. The relationship between dispositional self-construal and empathy for ingroup and outgroup members’ pain: evidence from ERPs [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2021, 53(6): 629-638. |
[5] | LI Jin, SUN Yu, YANG Zilu, ZHONG Yiping. Social value orientation modulates the processing of social rewards for self: Evidence from ERPs study [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2020, 52(6): 786-800. |
[6] | SUN Yan, LV Jiaojiao, LAN Fan, ZHANG Lina. Emotion regulation strategy of self-focused and situation-focused reappraisal and their impact on subsequent cognitive control [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2020, 52(12): 1393-1406. |
[7] | ZHONG Luojin, RU Taotao, FAN Meng, MO Lei. The effect of cognitive vagueness and motivation on conscious and unconscious self-deception [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2019, 51(12): 1330-1340. |
[8] | TIAN Lumei, YUAN Jingchi, LI Yongmei. Effects of peer presence and self-esteem on adolescent risk-taking behavior: Evidence from an ERP study [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2018, 50(1): 47-57. |
[9] | CHENG Jiaping; LUO Yuejia; CUI Fang. Empathy for pain influenced by cognitive load: Evidence from an ERP study [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2017, 49(5): 622-630. |
[10] | WEI Xiaona, QI Changzhu, XU Xia, HONG Xiaobin, LUO Yuejia. The effect of tennis expertise on motion-in-deep perception: An event-related potential study [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2017, 49(11): 1404-1413. |
[11] | FAN Wei, ZHONG Yiping, YANG Zilu, LI Jin, OUYANG Yi. The degree of self-reference effect of extroversion individuals [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2016, 48(8): 1002-1012. |
[12] | SONG Juan; GUO Fengbo; ZHANG Zhen; YUAN Sheng; JIN Hua; WANG Yiwen. Interpersonal distance influences on pain empathy: Friends priming effect [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2016, 48(7): 833-844. |
[13] | ZHANG Dandan, ZHAO Ting, LIU Yunzhe, CHEN Yuming. Comparison of Facial Expressions and Body Expressions: An Event-related Potential Study [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2015, 47(8): 963-970. |
[14] | FU Xinyuan; LU Zhiyuan; KOU Yu. Effects of a Stranger’s Presence and Behavior on Moral Hypocrisy [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2015, 47(8): 1058-1066. |
[15] | YANG Yaping, XU Qiang, ZHANG Lin, DENG Peizhuang, LIANG Ningjian. Scenes Differing in Spatial Frequencies Affect Facial Expression Processing: Evidence from ERP [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2015, 47(12): 1433-1444. |
Viewed | ||||||
Full text |
|
|||||
Abstract |
|
|||||