Please wait a minute...
心理学报  2019, Vol. 51 Issue (7): 747-758    DOI: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2019.00747
  研究报告 本期目录 | 过刊浏览 | 高级检索 |
状态焦虑对时距知觉的影响:认知评价和注意偏向有调节的中介作用
刘静远,李虹()
(清华大学心理学系, 北京 100084)(清华大学深圳研究生院, 深圳 518055)
How state anxiety influences time perception: Moderated mediating effect of cognitive appraisal and attentional bias
LIU Jingyuan,LI Hong()
(Department of Psychology, Tsinghua University, Beijing 100084, China)(Graduate School at Shenzhen, Tsinghua University, Shenzhen 518055, China)
全文: PDF(785 KB)   HTML 评审附件 (1 KB) 
输出: BibTeX | EndNote (RIS)       背景资料
文章导读  
摘要 

探讨状态焦虑对时距知觉的影响以及注意偏向与认知评价在其中的中介和调节作用。招募大学生60人, 随机分为高、低状态焦虑组, 用情绪诱导程序诱导高、低状态焦虑; 用点探测范式测量注意偏向; 用时间再现任务测量时距知觉; 用视觉模拟心境量表测量认知评价。结果发现:(1)状态焦虑会导致对2000 ms时距的高估; (2)注意偏向在状态焦虑对2000 ms时距知觉的影响中具有部分中介作用; (3)状态焦虑通过注意偏向影响时距知觉的中介过程受到认知评价的调节作用:只有当认知评价得分较高时, 即个体认为焦虑对心理健康有害程度较高时, 状态焦虑通过注意偏向影响2000 ms时距知觉。研究结果揭示了焦虑个体时距知觉的内部过程, 丰富了焦虑通过注意偏向影响时距知觉的解释视角, 为通过认知评价和注意偏向的调整改善焦虑个体时距偏差现象提供了重要参考。

服务
把本文推荐给朋友
加入引用管理器
E-mail Alert
RSS
作者相关文章
关键词 状态焦虑时距知觉注意偏向认知评价    
Abstract

Anxiety is associated with high levels of arousal. Both theoretical and empirical work have determined that when an individual experiences anxiety, he/she shows attentional bias toward negative stimuli. High arousal and negative attentional bias, as the two key characteristics of anxiety, are associated with a series of subjective feelings and experiences of individuals with state anxiety, among which time perception is significant. However, how this process operates remains an open question. In this article, we investigate how state anxious individuals perceive time, especially the roles of attention bias and cognitive appraisal in this process.
Sixty college students participated in the study and were randomly assigned to a high state anxiety group (n = 30, completed a procedure of anxious state induction) and a low state anxiety group (n = 30, completed a procedure of calm state induction). Then, a 2 (high state anxiety group vs. low state anxiety group) × 2 (negative stimuli vs. neutral stimuli) × 3 (2000 ms vs. 4000 ms vs. 8000 ms) mixed-design experiment was conducted with the attentional bias as the mediator, the cognitive appraisal as the moderator and the time perception as the dependent variable. State anxiety was manipulated by an induction process, time perception was measured by the time reproduction task, attentional bias was measured by the dot-probe task and cognitive appraisal was assessed by the visual analogue mood scales.
The results showed that (1) State anxiety had an effect on time perception, namely, individuals with high state anxiety overestimated the 2-second interval of the negative stimuli. (2) Attentional bias played a partial mediating role in the relationship between state anxiety and time perception of 2000 ms. (3) Cognitive appraisal moderated the mediation effect of attentional bias on the influence of state anxiety on time perception of 2000 ms. Specifically, when the score of cognitive appraisal was high, attentional bias played a mediating role in the influence of state anxiety on time perception, while when the score of cognitive appraisal was low, attentional bias did not play a mediating role in the influence of state anxiety on time perception.
Therefore, the effect of state anxiety on college students’ time perception was a moderated mediating effect. The moderated mediating model significantly revealed the effect mechanism of state anxiety on college students’ time perception, which can contribute to a better understanding of how individuals in an anxious state perceive time. Furthermore, it suggests that the adjustment of cognitive appraisal or attentional bias is an important way to alleviate the time distortion of anxious individuals.

Key wordsstate anxiety    time perception    attentional bias    cognitive appraisal
收稿日期: 2018-10-23      出版日期: 2019-05-22
中图分类号:  B842  
基金资助:* 深圳市科技创新基金项目(JCYJ20170817161546744)
通讯作者: 李虹     E-mail: lhong@mail.tsinghua.edu.cn
引用本文:   
刘静远,李虹. (2019). 状态焦虑对时距知觉的影响:认知评价和注意偏向有调节的中介作用. 心理学报, 51(7): 747-758.
LIU Jingyuan,LI Hong. (2019). How state anxiety influences time perception: Moderated mediating effect of cognitive appraisal and attentional bias. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 51(7), 747-758.
链接本文:  
http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/SP.J.1041.2019.00747      或      http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/Y2019/V51/I7/747
[1] Bar-Haim Y., Kerem A., Lamy D., & Zakay D . (2010). When time slows down: The influence of threat on time perception in anxiety. Cognition & Emotion, 24( 2), 255-263.
[2] Bar-Haim Y., Lamy D., Pergamin L., Bakermans-Kranenburg M. J & van IJzendoorn, M. H. ., ( 2007). Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: A meta-analytic study. Psychological Bulletin, 133( 1), 1-24.
[3] Block R.A., & Zakay D. , (1997). Prospective and retrospective duration judgments: A meta-analytic review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 4( 2), 184-197.
[4] Burle B. & Casini L. ,(2001). Dissociation between activation and attention effects in time estimation: Implications for internal clock models. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception & Performance, 27( 1), 195-205.
[5] Chaston A. & Kingstone A. , ( 2004). Time estimation: The effect of cortically mediated attention. Brain & Cognition, 55( 2), 286-289.
[6] Cohen J. ,(1992). Statistical power analysis. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 1( 3), 98-101.
[7] Droit-Volet S., Brunot S., & Niedenthal P. M . ( 2004). Perception of the duration of emotional events. Cognition & Emotion, 18( 6), 849-858.
[8] Effron D. A., Niedenthal P. M., Gil S., & Droit-Volet S . (2006). Embodied temporal perception of emotion. Emotion, 6( 1), 1-9.
[9] Eisler H. , ( 1976). Experiments on subjective duration 1968-1975: A collection of power function exponents. Psychological Bulletin, 83( 6), 1154-1171.
[10] Eysenck M. W. (1992). Anxiety: The cognitive perspective. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.
[11] Eysenck M. W. (1997). Anxiety and cognition: A unified theory. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.
[12] Faul F., Erdfelder E., Lang A.-G., & Buchner A . ( 2007). G* Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39( 2), 175-191.
[13] Folkman S., Lazarus R. S., Dunkel-Schetter C., Delongis A., & Gruen R. J . ( 1986). Dynamics of a stressful encounter: Cognitive appraisal, coping, and encounter outcomes. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 50( 5), 992-1003.
[14] Folkman S., Lazarus R. S., Gruen R. J., & Delongis A . ( 1986). Appraisal, coping, health status, and psychological symptoms. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 50( 3), 571-579.
[15] Fox E., Russo R., Bowles R., & Dutton K . ( 2001). Do threatening stimuli draw or hold visual attention in subclinical anxiety? Journal of Experimental Psychology General, 130( 4), 681-700.
[16] Fraisse P. , (1984). Perception and estimation of time. Annual Review of Psychology, 35, 1-36.
[17] Gable P.A., & Poole B.D . ( 2012). Time flies when you're having approach-motivated fun: Effects of motivational intensity on time perception. Psychological Science, 23( 8), 879-886.
[18] Georgiou G. A., Bleakley C., Hayward J., Russo R., Dutton K., Eltiti S., & Fox E . ( 2005). Focusing on fear: Attentional disengagement from emotional faces. Visual Cognition, 12( 1), 145-158.
[19] Gil S., Niedenthal P. M., & Droit-Volet S . ( 2007). Anger and time perception in children. Emotion, 7( 1), 219-225.
[20] Grondin S. , (2010). Timing and time perception: A review of recent behavioral and neuroscience findings and theoretical directions. Attention Perception & Psychophysics, 72( 3), 561-582.
[21] Hajcak G., Moser J. S., & Simons R. F . ( 2006). Attending to affect: Appraisal strategies modulate the electrocortical response to arousing pictures. Emotion, 6( 3), 517-522.
[22] Hayes, A.F . ( 2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. Journal of Educational Measurement, 51( 3), 335-337.
[23] Hoehn-Saric R., & Mcleod D.R . (2000). Anxiety and arousal: physiological changes and their perception. Journal of Affective Disorders, 61( 3), 217-224.
[24] Huang S., Liu P. D., Li Q. Q., Chen Y. G., & Huang X. T . ( 2018). The influence of facial expressions of pain on subsecond and suprasecond time perception. Journal of Psychological Science, ( 2), 278-284.
[24] [ 黄顺航, 刘培朵, 李庆庆, 陈有国, 黄希庭 . ( 2018). 疼痛表情对秒下及秒上时距知觉的影响. 心理科学, ( 2), 278-284.]
[25] Huang X. T., Li B. Y., & Zhang Z. J . ( 2003). The research of the range-synthetic model of temporal cognition. Journal of Southwest University (Social Sciences Edition), 29( 2), 5-9.
[25] [ 黄希庭, 李伯约, 张志杰 . ( 2003). 时间认知分段综合模型的探讨. 西南大学学报(社会科学版), 29( 2), 5-9.]
[26] Jamieson J. P., Nock M. K., & Mendes W. B . ( 2012). Mind over matter: reappraising arousal improves cardiovascular and cognitive responses to stress. Journal of Experimental Psychology General, 141( 3), 417-422.
[27] Karademas E. C., Christopoulou S., Dimostheni A., & Pavlu F . ( 2008). Health anxiety and cognitive interference: Evidence from the application of a modified Stroop task in two studies. Personality and Individual Differences, 44( 5), 1138-1150.
[28] Kim S. A., Kim H., & Kim S. H . ( 2016). Reappraisal modulates attentional bias to angry faces. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1841.
[29] Koster E. H. W., Leyman L., Raedt R. D., & Crombez G . ( 2006). Cueing of visual attention by emotional facial expressions: The influence of individual differences in anxiety and depression. Personality & Individual Differences, 41( 2), 329-339.
[30] Lang P. J., Bradley M. M., & Cuthbert B. N . ( 2005). International affective picture system (IAPS): Affective ratings of pictures and instruction manual. Technical Report A-6. Gainesville, FL: University of Florida.
[31] Lozano L. M., Garcíacueto E., & Muñiz J . ( 2008). Effect of the number of response categories on the reliability and validity of rating scales. Methodology, 4( 2), 73-79.
[32] Lueck M.D . ( 2007). Anxiety levels: Do they influence the perception of time? Journal of Undergraduate Research, 10, 1-5.
[33] Lv C., Niu Q.Y., & Zhang X.M . ( 2014). Attentional bias to negative stimulus in anxiety individuals. Chinese Mental Health Journal, 28( 3), 208-214.
[33] [ 吕创, 牛青云, 张学民 . ( 2014). 焦虑个体对负性刺激的注意偏向特点. 中国心理卫生杂志, 28( 3), 208-214.]
[34] Maeers S. , (2010). Task switching and subjective duration. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63( 3), 531-543.
[35] Mccormack H. M., Horne D. J. D., & Sheather S . ( 1988). Clinical applications of visual analog scales: A critical review. Psychological Medicine, 18( 4), 1007-1019.
[36] Mioni G., Stablum F., Prunetti E., & Grondin S . ( 2016). Time perception in anxious and depressed patients: A comparison between time reproduction and time production tasks. Journal of Affective Disorders, 196, 154-163.
[37] Mogg K., Wilson K. A., Hayward C., Cunning D., & Bradley B. P . ( 2012). Attentional biases for threat in at-risk daughters and mothers with lifetime panic disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121( 4), 852-862.
[38] Montorio I., Nuevo R., Cabrera I., Márquez M., & Izal M . ( 2015). Differential effectiveness of two anxiety induction proceduresin youth and older adult populations. Anales De Psicología, 31( 1), 28-36.
[39] Smith C.A . ( 1989). Dimensions of appraisal and physiological response in emotion. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 56( 3), 339-353.
[40] Spielberger C.D . ( 1971). Notes and comments trait-state anxiety and motor behavior. Journal of Motor Behavior, 3( 3), 265-279.
[41] Tamm M., Uusberg A., Allik J., & Kreegipuu K . ( 2014). Emotional modulation of attention affects time perception: Evidence from event-related potentials. Acta Psychologica, 149, 148-156.
[42] Tomaka J., Blascovich J., Kelsey R. M., & Leitten C. L . ( 1993). Subjective, physiological, and behavioral effects of threat and challenge appraisal. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 65( 2), 248-260.
[43] Tse P. U., Intriligator J., Rivest J., & Cavanagh P . ( 2004). Attention and the subjective expansion of time. Percept Psychophys, 66( 7), 1171-1189.
[44] Uusberg A., Naar R., Tamm M., Kreegipuu K., & Gross J. J . ( 2018). Bending time: The role of affective appraisal in time perception. Emotion, 18( 8), 1177-1188.
[45] van Bockstaele B., Verschuere B., Tibboel H., de Houwer J., Crombez G & Koster, E. H. W. ., ( 2014). A review of current evidence for the causal impact of attentional bias on fear and anxiety. Psychological Bulletin, 140( 3), 682-721.
[46] van Reekum C. M., Johnstone T., Urry H. L., Thurow M. E., Schaefer H. S., Alexander A. L., & Davidson R. J . ( 2007). Gaze fixations predict brain activation during the voluntary regulation of picture-induced negative affect. Neuroimage, 36( 3), 1041-1055.
[47] Wang Y.M., & Mao R.J . ( 2016). The influence of cognitive reappraisal on attentional deployment. Psychological Exploration, 36( 5), 409-412.
[47] [ 王艳梅, 毛锐杰 . ( 2016). 认知重评策略对注意分配的影响. 心理学探新, 36( 5), 409-412.]
[48] Whyman A.D., & Moos R. . ( 1967). Time perception and anxiety. Perceptual & Motor Skills, 24( 2), 567-570.
[49] Yoo J.Y., & Lee J.H . ( 2015). The effects of valence and arousal on time perception in individuals with social anxiety. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1208.
[50] Zakay D. ,( 1993). Relative and absolute duration judgments under prospective and retrospective paradigms. Perception & Psychophysics, 54( 5), 656-664.
[51] Zakay D. , ( 2005). Attention et jugement temporel. Psychologie Francaise, 50( 1), 65-79.
[52] Zakay D., & Block R.A . ( 1997). Temporal Cognition. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 6( 1), 12-16.
[53] Zakay D., & Block R.A . ( 2004). Prospective and retrospective duration judgments: An executive-control perspective. Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis, 64( 3), 319-328.
[54] Zakay D., Nitzan D., & Glicksohn J . ( 1983). The influence of task difficulty and external tempo on subjective time estimation. Perception & Psychophysics, 34( 5), 451-456.
[55] Zhao X., Lynch J. G., & Chen Q . ( 2010). Reconsidering Baron and Kenny: Myths and truths about mediation analysis. Journal of Consumer Research, 37( 2), 197-206.
[1] 雷怡, 夏琦, 莫志凤, 李红. 面孔可爱度和客观熟悉度对婴儿面孔注意偏向效应的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(7): 811-822.
[2] 吕薇. 回避与趋近性负性人格特质对应激心血管反应模式的不同影响[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(6): 758-776.
[3] 王盼盼,何嘉梅. 情景预见对跨期决策的影响机制[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(1): 38-54.
[4] 袁小钧, 崔晓霞, 曹正操, 阚红, 王晓, 汪亚珉. 虚拟仿真场景中威胁性视觉刺激搜索的注意偏向效应[J]. 心理学报, 2018, 50(6): 622-636.
[5] 胡金生, 李骋诗, 王琦, 李松泽, 李涛涛, 刘淑清. 孤独症青少年的情绪韵律注意偏向缺陷:低效率的知觉模式[J]. 心理学报, 2018, 50(6): 637-646.
[6] 孙俊才; 石荣. 哭泣表情面孔的注意偏向:眼动的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2017, 49(2): 155-163.
[7] 徐亮;区诵宜;郑希付;陈婷;冯彪;闫沛. 状态焦虑对条件性恐惧泛化的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2016, 48(12): 1507-1518.
[8] 闫志英;卢家楣. 情境真实性对悲伤移情调节的ERP证据[J]. 心理学报, 2015, 47(8): 971-980.
[9] 寇慧;苏艳华;罗小春;陈红. 相貌负面身体自我女性对相貌词的注意偏向眼动的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2015, 47(10): 1213-1222.
[10] 张予贺;金艳;郑希付;闫柯;周晌昀. 状态焦虑对条件恐惧习得和消退的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2014, 46(3): 396-404.
[11] 丁如一;周晖;林玛. 感激情绪的认知评估体系[J]. 心理学报, 2014, 46(10): 1463-1475.
[12] 李宝林;陈有国;袁祥勇;Todd Jackson;黄希庭. 拓扑性质对视觉新异刺激时距知觉的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2013, 45(12): 1324-1333.
[13] 王敬欣;贾丽萍;白学军;罗跃嘉. 返回抑制过程中情绪面孔加工优先:ERPs研究[J]. 心理学报, 2013, 45(1): 1-10.
[14] 翁春燕,陈红,朱岚. 限制性饮食者对食物线索的注意偏向:基于目标矛盾理论模型[J]. 心理学报, 2012, 44(5): 680-689.
[15] 李海江,杨娟,贾磊,张庆林. 不同自尊水平者的注意偏向[J]. 心理学报, 2011, 43(08): 907-916.
Viewed
Full text


Abstract

Cited

  Shared   
  Discussed   
版权所有 © 《心理学报》编辑部
本系统由北京玛格泰克科技发展有限公司设计开发  技术支持:support@magtech.com.cn