心理学报 ›› 2021, Vol. 53 ›› Issue (11): 1203-1214.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.01203
收稿日期:
2021-02-04
发布日期:
2021-09-23
出版日期:
2021-11-25
通讯作者:
刘伟,陈宁
E-mail:liuwei@shnu.edu.cn;chenning@shnu.edu.cn
基金资助:
SUN Lin1,2, DUAN Tao1, LIU Wei1(), CHEN Ning1()
Received:
2021-02-04
Online:
2021-09-23
Published:
2021-11-25
Contact:
LIU Wei,CHEN Ning
E-mail:liuwei@shnu.edu.cn;chenning@shnu.edu.cn
摘要:
基于真实考试和实验室模拟情境, 考察特质正念对初中生学业情绪预测偏差的影响及其机制。结果表明:(1)学业领域存在情绪预测偏差; (2)特质正念影响情绪预测偏差, 特质正念水平高的初中生情绪预测偏差相对更小, 反之则更大; (3)注意聚焦在特质正念影响情绪预测偏差中存在中介作用。本研究初步提出正念的“扩大-联结”解释模型, 推进了学业领域情绪预测的研究, 具有良好的生态学效度和切实的教育实践价值。
中图分类号:
孙琳, 段涛, 刘伟, 陈宁. (2021). 特质正念对初中生学业情绪预测偏差的影响. 心理学报, 53(11), 1203-1214.
SUN Lin, DUAN Tao, LIU Wei, CHEN Ning. (2021). The influence of dispositional mindfulness on the academic affective forecasting biases of middle school students. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 53(11), 1203-1214.
情境 | 特质正念分组 | 情绪预测 | 情绪体验 | 相对偏差 | 绝对偏差 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
目标实现组 | 低特质正念组(n = 61) | 3.79 ± 1.11 | 3.13 ± 1.35 | -0.66 ± 1.31 | 1.16 ± 0.89 |
高特质正念组(n = 61) | 3.93 ± 1.09 | 3.35 ± 1.20 | -0.58 ± 0.93 | 0.82 ± 0.73 | |
目标未实现组 | 低特质正念组(n = 72) | 3.18 ± 1.25 | 2.72 ± 1.21 | -0.46 ± 1.42 | 1.10 ± 1.00 |
高特质正念组(n = 73) | 2.92 ± 1.10 | 2.84 ± 1.17 | -0.08 ± 1.05 | 0.74 ± 0.74 |
表1 研究1描述性统计结果(M ± SD)
情境 | 特质正念分组 | 情绪预测 | 情绪体验 | 相对偏差 | 绝对偏差 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
目标实现组 | 低特质正念组(n = 61) | 3.79 ± 1.11 | 3.13 ± 1.35 | -0.66 ± 1.31 | 1.16 ± 0.89 |
高特质正念组(n = 61) | 3.93 ± 1.09 | 3.35 ± 1.20 | -0.58 ± 0.93 | 0.82 ± 0.73 | |
目标未实现组 | 低特质正念组(n = 72) | 3.18 ± 1.25 | 2.72 ± 1.21 | -0.46 ± 1.42 | 1.10 ± 1.00 |
高特质正念组(n = 73) | 2.92 ± 1.10 | 2.84 ± 1.17 | -0.08 ± 1.05 | 0.74 ± 0.74 |
情境 | 特质正念分组 | 情绪预测 | 情绪体验 | 相对偏差 | 绝对偏差 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
成功反馈 | 低特质正念组(n = 36) | 3.07 ± 1.02 | 3.27 ± 1.05 | 0.19 ± 1.13 | 0.90 ± 0.70 |
高特质正念组(n = 34) | 3.44 ± 1.00 | 3.50 ± 1.10 | 0.06 ± 0.76 | 0.57 ± 0.50 | |
失败反馈 | 低特质正念组(n = 36) | 2.93 ± 1.17 | 2.58 ± 1.29 | -0.69 ± 1.52 | 1.33 ± 0.99 |
高特质正念组(n = 34) | 2.10 ± 1.19 | 2.76 ± 1.20 | -0.74 ± 0.91 | 0.91 ± 0.73 |
表2 研究2描述性统计结果(M ± SD)
情境 | 特质正念分组 | 情绪预测 | 情绪体验 | 相对偏差 | 绝对偏差 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
成功反馈 | 低特质正念组(n = 36) | 3.07 ± 1.02 | 3.27 ± 1.05 | 0.19 ± 1.13 | 0.90 ± 0.70 |
高特质正念组(n = 34) | 3.44 ± 1.00 | 3.50 ± 1.10 | 0.06 ± 0.76 | 0.57 ± 0.50 | |
失败反馈 | 低特质正念组(n = 36) | 2.93 ± 1.17 | 2.58 ± 1.29 | -0.69 ± 1.52 | 1.33 ± 0.99 |
高特质正念组(n = 34) | 2.10 ± 1.19 | 2.76 ± 1.20 | -0.74 ± 0.91 | 0.91 ± 0.73 |
情境 | 特质正念分组 | 情绪预测 | 情绪体验 | 相对偏差 | 绝对偏差 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
挑战成功 | 低特质正念组(n = 32) | 4.05 ± 0.46 | 3.80 ± 0.62 | -0.26 ± 0.57 | 0.46 ± 0.41 |
高特质正念组(n = 39) | 4.11 ± 0.57 | 4.08 ± 0.63 | -0.03 ± 0.31 | 0.21 ± 0.24 | |
挑战失败 | 低特质正念组(n = 33) | 2.21 ± 0.87 | 2.83 ± 0.78 | 0.62 ± 1.02 | 0.83 ± 0.85 |
高特质正念组(n = 32) | 2.14 ± 0.74 | 2.41 ± 0.89 | 0.27 ± 0.78 | 0.55 ± 0.61 |
表3 研究3描述性统计结果(M ± SD)
情境 | 特质正念分组 | 情绪预测 | 情绪体验 | 相对偏差 | 绝对偏差 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
挑战成功 | 低特质正念组(n = 32) | 4.05 ± 0.46 | 3.80 ± 0.62 | -0.26 ± 0.57 | 0.46 ± 0.41 |
高特质正念组(n = 39) | 4.11 ± 0.57 | 4.08 ± 0.63 | -0.03 ± 0.31 | 0.21 ± 0.24 | |
挑战失败 | 低特质正念组(n = 33) | 2.21 ± 0.87 | 2.83 ± 0.78 | 0.62 ± 1.02 | 0.83 ± 0.85 |
高特质正念组(n = 32) | 2.14 ± 0.74 | 2.41 ± 0.89 | 0.27 ± 0.78 | 0.55 ± 0.61 |
回归方程(n = 136) | 拟合指标 | 系数显著性 | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
结果变量 | 预测变量 | R | R2 | F | β | t | p |
绝对偏差 | 0.24 | 0.06 | 8.13 | ||||
特质正念 | -0.29 | -2.85 | 0.005 | ||||
注意聚焦 | 0.28 | 0.08 | 11.76 | ||||
特质正念 | 0.46 | 3.43 | 0.001 | ||||
绝对偏差 | 0.33 | 0.11 | 8.10 | ||||
特质正念 | -0.21 | -2.01 | 0.046 | ||||
注意聚焦 | -0.18 | -2.77 | 0.006 |
表4 注意聚焦的中介模型检验
回归方程(n = 136) | 拟合指标 | 系数显著性 | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
结果变量 | 预测变量 | R | R2 | F | β | t | p |
绝对偏差 | 0.24 | 0.06 | 8.13 | ||||
特质正念 | -0.29 | -2.85 | 0.005 | ||||
注意聚焦 | 0.28 | 0.08 | 11.76 | ||||
特质正念 | 0.46 | 3.43 | 0.001 | ||||
绝对偏差 | 0.33 | 0.11 | 8.10 | ||||
特质正念 | -0.21 | -2.01 | 0.046 | ||||
注意聚焦 | -0.18 | -2.77 | 0.006 |
效应 | 效应值 | Boot 标准误 | BootCI 下限 | BootCI 上限 | 效应占比 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
总效应 | -0.29 | 0.10 | -0.49 | -0.09 | |
直接效应 | -0.21 | 0.10 | -0.41 | -0.004 | 72.41% |
间接效应 | -0.08 | 0.04 | -0.17 | -0.02 | 27.59% |
表5 注意聚焦总效应、直接效应及中介效应分解表
效应 | 效应值 | Boot 标准误 | BootCI 下限 | BootCI 上限 | 效应占比 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
总效应 | -0.29 | 0.10 | -0.49 | -0.09 | |
直接效应 | -0.21 | 0.10 | -0.41 | -0.004 | 72.41% |
间接效应 | -0.08 | 0.04 | -0.17 | -0.02 | 27.59% |
[1] | Adam W. H. (2016). The mindful personality: Associations between dispositional mindfulness and the Five Factor Model of personality. Personality and Individual Differences, 91, 154-158. |
[2] | Baer R. A., Smith G. T., Hopkins J., Krietemeyer J., & Toney L. (2006). Using self-report assessment methods to explore facets of mindfulness. Assessment, 13(1), 27-45. |
[3] | Barsics C., van der Linden M., & D′Argembeau A. (2016). Frequency, characteristics, and perceived functions of emotional future thinking in daily life. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69(2), 217-233. |
[4] | Bishop S. R., Lau M., Shapiro S., Carlson L., Anderson N. D., Carmody J., … Devins D. (2004). Mindfulness: A proposed operational definition. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 11(3), 230-241. |
[5] | Brenner C. J., & Ben-Zeev D. (2014). Affective forecasting in schizophrenia: Comparing predictions to real-time Ecological Momentary Assessment (EMA) ratings. Psychiatric Rehabilitation Journal, 37(4), 316-320. |
[6] | Brown K. W., Ryan R. M., & Creswell J. D. (2007). Mindfulness: Theoretical foundations and evidence for its salutary effects. Psychological Inquiry, 18(4), 211-237. |
[7] | Chen, N. (2014). On teachers’ emotional forecasting accuracy and influencing factors (Unpublished doctorial dissertation). Shanghai Normal University. |
[ 陈宁. (2014). 教师的情绪预测准确性及其影响因素 (博士学位论文). 上海师范大学.] | |
[8] | Chen N., Lu J. M., & Wang H. B. (2013). Interpersonal empathy gap can be overcome: Taking teachers forecasting students’ emotion as an example. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 45(12), 1368-1380. |
[ 陈宁, 卢家楣, 汪海彬. (2013). 人际共情鸿沟可以跨越:以教师预测学生情绪为例. 心理学报, 45(12), 1368-1380.] | |
[9] | Chen N., Lu J. M., & Wang H. B. (2014). The example anchoring effect in the process of interpersonal emotional forecasting. Journal of Psychological Science, 37(4), 930-935. |
[ 陈宁, 卢家楣, 汪海彬. (2014). 人际间情绪预测过程中的样例锚定效应. 心理科学, 37(4), 930-935.] | |
[10] | Chen Y., Zhao X., Huang J. H., Chen S. Y., & Zhou R. L. (2011). Effects of mindfulness meditation on emotion regulation: Theories and neural mechanism. Advances in Psychological Science, 19(10), 1502-1510. |
[ 陈语, 赵鑫, 黄俊红, 陈思佚, 周仁来. (2011). 正念冥想对情绪的调节作用: 理论与神经机制. 心理科学进展, 19(10), 1502-1510.] | |
[11] | Cordonnier A., Barnier A. J., & Sutton J. (2016). Scripts and information units in future planning: Interactions between a past and a future planning task. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69(2), 324-338. |
[12] | Deng, Y. Q. (2009). Effect of mental awareness training on mental health of college students (Unpublished master’s thesis). Capital Normal University, Beijing. |
[ 邓玉琴. (2009). 心智觉知训练对大学生心理健康水平的干预效果 (硕士学位论文). 首都师范大学.] | |
[13] | Dong, Y., & Yu, G. L. (2007). The development and application of an academic emotions questionnaire. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 39(5), 852-860. |
[ 董妍, 俞国良. (2007). 青少年学业情绪问卷的编制及应用. 心理学报, 39(5), 852-860. ] | |
[14] | Dore B. P., Meksin R., Mather M., Hirst W., & Ochsner K. N. (2016). Highly accurate prediction of emotions surrounding the attacks of September 11, 2001 over 1-, 2-, and 7-year prediction intervals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145(6), 788-795. |
[15] | Dunn E. W., Brackett M. A., Ashton-James C., Schneiderman E., & Salovey P. (2007). On emotionally intelligent time travel:Individual differences in affective forecasting ability. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33(1), 85-93. |
[16] | Elwyn G., Stiel M., Durand M.-A., & Boivin J. (2011). The design of patient decision support interventions: Addressing the theory-practice gap. Journal of Evaluation in Clinical Practice, 17(4), 565-574. |
[17] | Emanuel A. S., Updegraff J. A., Kalmbach D. A., & Ciesla J. A. (2010). The role of mindfulness facets in affective forecasting. Personality and Individual Differences, 49(7), 815-818. |
[18] | Gautam S., Bulley A., von Hippel W., & Suddendorf T. (2017). Affective forecasting bias in preschool children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 159, 175-184. |
[19] | Geng X. W., Fan L. L., & Zhao L. N. (2019). The influence of learning agency beliefs on affective forecasting bias in middle school students’ learning behavior. Journal of Psychological Science, 42(1), 56-61. |
[ 耿晓伟, 范琳琳, 赵李娜. (2019). 学习能动性信念对初中生学习行为中情感预测偏差的影响. 心理科学, 42(1), 56-61.] | |
[20] | Geng, X. W., & Jiang, H. Y. (2017). Influence of regulatory focus and regulatory fit on impact biases in affective forecast. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 49(12), 1537-1547. |
[ 耿晓伟, 姜宏艺. (2017). 调节定向和调节匹配对情感预测中影响偏差的影响. 心理学报, 49(12), 1537-1547.] | |
[21] | Geng X. W., Liu D., & Niu Y. H. (2020). Analytical thinking reduces impact bias in affective forecast. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 52(10), 1168-1177. |
[ 耿晓伟, 刘丹, 牛燕华. (2020). 分析思维降低情感预测影响偏差. 心理学报, 52(10), 1168-1177. ] | |
[22] | Geng, X. W., & Zhang, F. (2015). The impact bias in affective forecasting: Focalism or unforeseen adaptation? Journal of Psychological Science, 38(5), 1201-1206. |
[ 耿晓伟, 张峰. (2015). 情感预测的影响偏差——聚焦错觉还是适应忽视? 心理科学, 38(5), 1201-1206.] | |
[23] | Gilbert D. T., & Wilson T.D. (2009). Why the brain talks to itself: Sources of error in emotional prediction. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 364(1521), 1335-1341. |
[24] | Green J. D., Davis J. L., Luchies L. B., Coy A. E., van Tongeren D. R., Reid C. A., & Finkel E. J. (2013). Victims versus perpetrators: Affective and empathic forecasting regarding transgressions in romantic relationships. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49(3), 329-333. |
[25] | Hair J. F., Black W. C., Babin B. J., & Anderson R. E. (2010). Multivariate Data Analysis: A global perspective. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. |
[26] | Hayes A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York: Guilford. |
[27] | Hezel D. M., Stewart S. E., Riemann B. C., & McNally R. J. (2019). Affective forecasting accuracy in obsessive compulsive disorder. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 47(5), 573-584. |
[28] | Hill C. L., & Updegraff J.A. (2012). Mindfulness and its relationship to emotional regulation. Emotion, 12(1), 81-0. |
[29] | Hoerger M., Chapman B., & Duberstein P. (2016). Realistic affective forecasting: The role of personality. Cognition and Emotion, 30(7), 1304-1316. |
[30] | Hoerger M., Chapman B. P., Epstein R. M., & Duberstein P. R. (2012). Emotional intelligence: A theoretical framework for individual differences in affective forecasting. Emotion, 12(4), 716-725. |
[31] | Hoerger M., & Quirk S. W. (2010). Affective forecasting and the Big Five. Personality and Individual Differences, 49(8), 972-976. |
[32] | Hoerger M., Scherer L. D., & Fagerlin A. (2016). Affective forecasting and medication decision making in breast- cancer prevention. Health Psychology, 35(6), 594-603. |
[33] | Hong P. Y., Lishner D. A., Vogels E. A., & Ebert A. R. (2016). The effect of a mindfulness practice and dispositional mindfulness on affective forecasting. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 38(3), 153-165. |
[34] | Kabat-Zinn J. (2003). Mindfulness-based interventions in context: Past, present, and future. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 10(2), 144-156. |
[35] | Kong D. T. (2015). The role of mindfulness and neuroticism in predicting acculturative anxiety forecasting error. Mindfulness, 6, 1387-1400. |
[36] | Kopp L., Atance C. M., & Pearce S. (2017). “Things aren’t so bad!”: Preschoolers overpredict the emotional intensity of negative outcomes. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 35, 623-627. |
[37] | Kumar A., & Epley N. (2018). Undervaluing gratitude: Expressers misunderstand the consequences of showing appreciation. Psychological Science, 29(9), 1423-1435. |
[38] | Lau T., Morewedge C. K., & Cikara M. (2016). Overcorrection for social-categorization information moderates impact bias in affective forecasting. Psychological Science, 27(10), 1340-1351. |
[39] | Lench H. C., Levine L. J., Perez K., Carpenter Z. K., Carlson S. J., Bench S. W., & Wan Y. (2019). When and why people misestimate future feelings: Identifying strengths and weaknesses in affective forecasting. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 116(5), 724-742. |
[40] | Lench H. C., Safer M. A., & Levine L. J. (2011). Focalism and the underestimation of future emotion: When it’s worse than imagine. Emotion, 11(2), 278-285. |
[41] | Levine E. E., & Cohen T.R. (2018). You can handle the truth: Mispredicting the consequences of honest communication. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 147(9), 1400-1429. |
[42] | Loehr V. G., & Baldwin A.S. (2014). Affective forecasting error in exercise: Differences between physically active and inactive individuals. Sport, Exercise, and Performance Psychology, 3(3), 177-183. |
[43] | Luo H. B., Xu F. M., Guo Y. Y., Zheng Q. Q., Li B., & Zhang H. (2013). Focusing illusion in affective forecasting. Advances in Psychological Science, 21(8), 1482-1492. |
[ 罗寒冰, 徐富明, 郭永玉, 郑秋强, 李彬, 张慧. (2013). 情感预测中的聚焦错觉. 心理科学进展, 21(8), 1482-1492.] | |
[44] | Mata A., & Simão C. (2019). Karmic forecasts: The role of justice in forecasts about self and others. Motivation Science, Online First Publication, December 19. |
[45] | Miloyan B., & Suddendorf T. (2015). Feelings of the future. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19(4), 196-200. |
[46] | Pauketat J. V. T., Moons W. G., Chen J. M., Mackie D. M., & Sherman D. K. (2016). Self-affirmation and affective forecasting: Affirmation reduces the anticipated impact of negative events. Motivation and Emotion, 40(5), 750-759. |
[47] | Peng, Y. Q., & Ju, M. Z. (2013). The “heart” of the working mechanism of mindfulness: Attention or attitude? Journal of Psychological Science, 36(4), 1009-1013. |
[ 彭彦琴, 居敏珠. (2013). 正念机制的核心: 注意还是态度? 心理科学, 36(4), 1009-1013.] | |
[48] | Pollmann M. M. H., & Finkenauer C. (2009). Empathic forecasting: How do we predict other people’s feelings? Cognition and Emotion, 23(5), 978-1001. |
[49] | Rasmussen K. W., & Berntsen D. (2014). “I can see clearly now”: The effect of cue imageability on mental time travel. Memory and Cognition, 42(7), 1063-1075. |
[50] | Robins C. J., Keng S.-L., Ekblad A. G., & Brantley J. G. (2012). Effects of mindfulness-based stress reduction on emotional experience and expression: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 68(1), 117-131. |
[51] | Ruby M. B., Dunn E. W., Perrino A., Gillis R., & Viel S. (2011). The invisible benefits of exercise. Health Psychology, 30, 67-74. |
[52] | Sevdalis N., & Harvey N. (2007). Biased forecasting of postdecisional affect. Psychological Science, 18(8), 678-681. |
[53] | Shapiro S. L., Carlson L. E., Astin J. A., & Freedman B. (2006). Mechanisms of mindfulness. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 62(3), 373-386. |
[54] | Sun L., Duan T., & Chen N. (2020). On the causes and interventions of affective forecasting bias. Advances in Psychological Science, 28(12), 2018-2026. |
[ 孙琳, 段涛, 陈宁. (2020). 情绪预测偏差的成因及干预. 心理科学进展, 28(12), 2018-2026. ] | |
[55] | Teper R., Segal Z. V., & Inzlicht M. (2013). Inside the mindful mind: How mindfulness enhances emotion regulation through improvements in executive control. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22(6), 449-454. |
[56] | Thompson R. J., Spectre A., Insel P. S., Mennin D., Gotlib I. H., & Gruber J. (2017). Positive and negative affective forecasting in remitted individuals with bipolar I disorder, and major depressive disorder, and healthy controls. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 41(5), 673-685. |
[57] | Watson D., Clark L. A., & Tellegen A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063-1070. |
[58] | Wilson T. D. , & Gilbert, D. T.(2003) . Affective forecasting. In P. Z. Mark (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology Vol. 35 (pp. 345-411). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. |
[59] | Wilson T. D., & Gilbert D.T. (2005). Affective forecasting: Knowing what to want. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14(3), 131-134. |
[60] | Wilson T. D., Meyers J., & Gilbert D. T. (2001). Lessons from the past: Do people learn from experience that emotional reactions are short-lived?. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27(12), 1648-1661. |
[61] | Wilson T. D., Wheatley T., Meyers J. M., Gilbert D. T., & Axsom D. (2000). Focalism: A source of durability bias in affective forecasting. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 821-836. |
[62] | Zelenski J. M., Whelan D. C., Nealis L. J., Besner C. M., Santoro M. S., & Wynn J. E. (2013). Personality and affective forecasting: Trait introverts underpredict the hedonic benefits of acting extraverted. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104(6), 1092-1108. |
[63] | Zhang P., Lu J. M., & Zhang M. (2012). The effect of mood on the probability judgment of future events. Journal of Psychological Science, 35(1), 100-104. |
[ 张萍, 卢家楣, 张敏. (2012). 心境对未来事件发生概率判断的影响. 心理科学, 35(1), 100-104.] | |
[64] | Zhu, P. Y. (2010). An experimental study on emotional states affecting high school students’ remote association (Unpublished master’s thesis). Anhui Normal University, China. |
[ 朱平原. (2010). 情绪状态影响高中生远距离联想的实验研究 (硕士学位论文). 安徽师范大学.] |
[1] | 张孟可, 李晴, 尹首航, 陈安涛. 冲突水平的变化诱发冲突适应[J]. 心理学报, 2021, 53(2): 128-138. |
[2] | 柯晓晓, 齐惠紫, 梁家辉, 金欣园, 高婕, 张明霞, 汪亚珉. 中国人整体性思维特征的情境评估法及其应用[J]. 心理学报, 2021, 53(12): 1299-1309. |
[3] | 孙莎莎, 李小兵, 李宝山, 刘承宜, 黄敏儿. 正念维持适应的机制:来自心率变异性自相似的初步证据[J]. 心理学报, 2018, 50(12): 1413-1427. |
[4] | 刘潞潞, 卢家楣, 和美, 周建设, 肖晶, 罗劲. 先苦后乐:英语乐学大学生在英语学习时情绪反应的脑认知特点[J]. 心理学报, 2017, 49(11): 1414-1427. |
[5] | 董妍,俞国良. 青少年学业情绪问卷的编制及应用[J]. 心理学报, 2007, 39(05): 852-860. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||