心理学报 ›› 2019, Vol. 51 ›› Issue (2): 177-187.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2019.00177 cstr: 32110.14.2019.00177
收稿日期:
2018-02-09
发布日期:
2018-12-24
出版日期:
2019-02-25
基金资助:
Received:
2018-02-09
Online:
2018-12-24
Published:
2019-02-25
摘要:
采用情感错误归因范式(AMP)和经典情感启动范式(BFP), 在汉语双音节中性名词的前面加上否定或肯定词, 构成“没有/有XX”短语, 作为启动材料, 以中性俄语词或汉语情绪词为目标材料, 探讨了汉语否定加工的情绪表征。在实验1的AMP范式中, 刺激反应不同步(SOA)时间为200 ms和500 ms, 结果显示, 否定短语比肯定短语有显著高的消极反应概率; 在实验2的BFP范式中, 否定短语比肯定短语同样启动了更高的消极反应概率。实验结果表明, 否定本身具有消极效价, 否定加工通过消极情绪实现具身化。
中图分类号:
高志华, 鲁忠义. (2019). “没有”为什么隐含着“消极情绪”?——否定加工中的情绪表征. 心理学报, 51(2), 177-187.
GAO Zhihua, LU Zhongyi. (2019). Why “no” implies “negative emotion”? Emotional representation in negation processing. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 51(2), 177-187.
启动项 | SOA: 200 ms (N = 38) | SOA: 500 ms (N = 40) |
---|---|---|
否定短语 | 0.64 ± 0.20 | 0.63 ± 0.19 |
肯定短语 | 0.30 ± 0.19 | 0.30 ± 0.20 |
表1 实验1两种SOA条件下中性短语启动的消极反应概率(M ± SD)
启动项 | SOA: 200 ms (N = 38) | SOA: 500 ms (N = 40) |
---|---|---|
否定短语 | 0.64 ± 0.20 | 0.63 ± 0.19 |
肯定短语 | 0.30 ± 0.19 | 0.30 ± 0.20 |
情绪词指标 | 消极效价 | 积极效价 | F | p |
---|---|---|---|---|
词频 | 50.06 ± 48.09 | 49.21 ± 48.98 | 0.01 | 0.912 |
效价 | 3.12 ± 0.24 | 6.80 ± 0.20 | 10937.67 | < 0.001 |
唤醒水平 | 4.86 ± 0.63 | 4.69 ± 0.60 | 3.01 | 0.085 |
优势水平 | 5.08 ± 0.54 | 5.78 ± 0.56 | 64.78 | < 0.001 |
熟悉程度 | 4.91 ± 0.44 | 5.63 ± 0.45 | 104.35 | < 0.001 |
表2 实验2目标项的词频和情绪维度各指标分析
情绪词指标 | 消极效价 | 积极效价 | F | p |
---|---|---|---|---|
词频 | 50.06 ± 48.09 | 49.21 ± 48.98 | 0.01 | 0.912 |
效价 | 3.12 ± 0.24 | 6.80 ± 0.20 | 10937.67 | < 0.001 |
唤醒水平 | 4.86 ± 0.63 | 4.69 ± 0.60 | 3.01 | 0.085 |
优势水平 | 5.08 ± 0.54 | 5.78 ± 0.56 | 64.78 | < 0.001 |
熟悉程度 | 4.91 ± 0.44 | 5.63 ± 0.45 | 104.35 | < 0.001 |
启动 | 目标 | SOA: 200 ms (N = 43) | 启动 | 目标 | SOA: 500 ms (N = 46) | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
消极反应 | 正确率 | 消极反应 | 正确率 | ||||
否定中性 | 积极 | 0.11 ± 0.15 | 0.89 ± 0.15 | 否定中性 | 积极 | 0.09 ± 0.16 | 0.91 ± 0.16 |
消极 | 0.93 ± 0.09 | 0.93 ± 0.09 | 消极 | 0.96 ± 0.18 | 0.96 ± 0.18 | ||
肯定中性 | 积极 | 0.09 ± 0.09 | 0.91 ± 0.87 | 肯定中性 | 积极 | 0.05 ± 0.05 | 0.95 ± 0.05 |
消极 | 0.89 ± 0.15 | 0.89 ± 0.15 | 消极 | 0.94 ± 0.22 | 0.94 ± 0.22 |
表3 实验2不同条件下消极反应概率与反应正确率(M ± SD)
启动 | 目标 | SOA: 200 ms (N = 43) | 启动 | 目标 | SOA: 500 ms (N = 46) | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
消极反应 | 正确率 | 消极反应 | 正确率 | ||||
否定中性 | 积极 | 0.11 ± 0.15 | 0.89 ± 0.15 | 否定中性 | 积极 | 0.09 ± 0.16 | 0.91 ± 0.16 |
消极 | 0.93 ± 0.09 | 0.93 ± 0.09 | 消极 | 0.96 ± 0.18 | 0.96 ± 0.18 | ||
肯定中性 | 积极 | 0.09 ± 0.09 | 0.91 ± 0.87 | 肯定中性 | 积极 | 0.05 ± 0.05 | 0.95 ± 0.05 |
消极 | 0.89 ± 0.15 | 0.89 ± 0.15 | 消极 | 0.94 ± 0.22 | 0.94 ± 0.22 |
1 |
Alter A. L., Oppenheimer D. M., Epley N., & Eyre R. N . ( 2007). Overcoming intuition: Metacognitive difficulty activates analytic reasoning. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 136( 4), 569-576.
doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.136.4.569 URL |
2 | Anderson S. E., Huette S., Matlock T., & Spivey M . ( 2010). On the temporal dynamics of negated perceptual simulations. In Parrill, F., Tobin, V., & Turner, M.( Eds.), Meaning, form and body (pp. 1-20). Stanford, CA: CSLI Publication. |
3 |
Armstrong, A.M., & Dienes Z. ( 2013). Subliminal understanding of negation: Unconscious control by subliminal processing of word pairs. Consciousness and Cognition, 22( 3), 1022-1040.
doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2013.06.010 URL |
4 |
Autry, K.S., & Levine W.H, . ( 2014). Presupposition processing and the (re)activation of negated concepts. Discourse Processes, 51( 7), 535-564.
doi: 10.1080/0163853X.2013.871192 URL |
5 |
Bar-Anan, Y., & Nosek B.A, . ( 2014). A comparative investigation of seven indirect attitude measures. Behavior Research Methods, 46( 3), 668-688.
doi: 10.3758/s13428-013-0410-6 URL |
6 | Chen G. Y., Wu M. Y., Wei X. P., Zhou M., He X. Y., & Mo L . ( 2014). The processing of isolated negative sentences with uncertain states. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 46( 2), 204-215. |
[ 陈广耀, 吴洺仪, 魏小平, 周苗, 何先友, 莫雷 . ( 2014). 状态不确定独立否定句的加工机制. 心理学报, 46( 2), 204-215.] | |
7 | Cui R. X., Gao Z.H., Tang Y. L., He H. P., & Lu Z. Y . ( 2016). The two-steps mental simulation process of Chinese definite unbounded negative sentences. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 48( 6), 607-616. |
[ 崔如霞, 高志华, 唐艺琳, 何皓璠, 鲁忠义 . ( 2016). 汉语确定性无界否定句模拟加工的时间进程. 心理学报, 48( 6), 607-616.] | |
8 |
Deutsch, R., & Gawronski B. ( 2009). When the method makes a difference: Antagonistic effects on “automatic evaluations” as a function of task characteristics of the measure. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45( 1), 101-114.
doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2008.09.001 URL |
9 |
Deutsch R., Kordts-Freudinger R., Gawronski B., & Strack F . ( 2009). Fast and fragile: A new look at the automaticity of negation processing. Experimental Psychology, 56( 6):434-446.
doi: 10.1027/1618-3169.56.6.434 URL |
10 |
Eddy, M.D., & Holcomb P.J, . ( 2010). The temporal dynamics of masked repetition picture priming effects: Manipulations of stimulus-onset asynchrony (SOA) and prime duration. Brain Research, 1340( 9), 24-39.
doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.04.024 URL |
11 | Fazio R. H., Sanbonmatsu D. M., Powell M. C., & Kardes F. R . ( 1986). On the automatic activation of attitudes. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 50( 2), 229-238. |
12 | Flaisch T., Imhof M., Schmälzle R., Wentz K. U., Ibach B., & Schupp H. T . ( 2015). Implicit and explicit attention to pictures and words: An fMRI-study of concurrent emotional stimulus processing. Frontiers in Psychology, 6( 1351), 1861. |
13 | Foroni, F., & Semin G.R, . ( 2013). Comprehension of action negation involves inhibitory simulation. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7( 7), 62-77. |
14 | Gao Z. H., Lu Z. Y., & Cui X. Y . ( 2017). What is the mechanism of negation processes? Introduction and commentary on psychological theories on negation. Advances in Psychological Science, 25( 3), 413-423. |
[ 高志华, 鲁忠义, 崔新颖 . ( 2017). 否定加工的机制到底是什么?——否定加工的心理学理论述评. 心理科学进展, 25( 3), 413-423.] | |
15 | Gao Z. H., Lu Z. Y., & Ma H. X . ( 2011). The simulation process of Chinese simple negative declarative sentence. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 43( 12), 1380-1387. |
[ 高志华, 鲁忠义, 马红霞 . ( 2011). 汉语简单否定陈述句理解的心理模拟过程. 心理学报, 43( 12), 1380-1387.] | |
16 | Gawronski B., & Houwer, J. D .( 2014) . Implicit measures in social and personality psychology. In H. T. Reis & C. M. Judd (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in social and personality psychology (2nd edition). New York: Cambridge University Press. |
17 | Gawronski B., & Strack, F . (Eds.).( 2012). Cognitive consistency: A fundamental principle in social cognition New York, NY: Guilford Press. |
18 | Gawronski, B., & Yang Y. ( 2015). Prevention of intention invention in the affect misattribution procedure. Social Psychological & Personality Science, 6( 1), 101-108. |
19 |
Haran D., Mor N., & Mayo R . ( 2011). Negating in order to be negative: The relationship between depressive rumination, message content and negation processing. Emotion, 11( 5), 1105-1111.
doi: 10.1037/a0025301 URL |
20 | Havas D. A., Glenberg A. M., & Rinck M . ( 2007). Emotion simulation during language comprehension. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14( 3), 436-441. |
21 |
Herbert C., Deutsch R., Platte P., & Pauli P . ( 2013). No fear, no panic: Probing negation as a means for emotion regulation. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience Advance, 8( 6), 654-661.
doi: 10.1093/scan/nss043 URL |
22 |
Herbert C., Deutsch R., Sütterlin S., Kübler A., & Pauli P . ( 2011). Negation as a means for emotion regulation? Startle reflex modulation during processing of negated emotional words. Cognitive Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience, 11( 2), 199-206.
doi: 10.3758/s13415-011-0026-1 URL |
23 |
Hermans D., Spruyt A., & Eelen P . ( 2003). Automatic affective priming of recently acquired stimulus valence: Priming at SOA 300 but not at SOA 1000. Cognition and Emotion, 17( 1), 83-99.
doi: 10.1080/02699930302276 URL |
24 |
Hill H., Ott F., & Weisbrod M . ( 2005). SOA-dependent N400 and P300 semantic priming effects using pseudoword primes and a delayed lexical decision. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 56( 3), 209-221.
doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2004.12.004 URL |
25 |
Jiang Z. Q., Li W. H., Liu Y., Luo Y. J., Luu P., & Tucker D. M . ( 2014). When affective word valence meets linguistic polarity: Behavioral and ERP evidence. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 28, 19-30.
doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2013.11.001 URL |
26 |
Jordan, M.P . ( 1998). The power of negation in English: Text, context, and relevance. Journal of Pragmatics, 29( 6), 705-752.
doi: 10.1016/S0378-2166(97)00086-6 URL |
27 |
Kaup B., Lüdtke J., Zwaan R.A . ( 2006). Processing negated sentences with contradictory predicates: Is a door that is not open mentally closed? Journal of Pragmatics, 38( 7), 1033-1050.
doi: 10.1016/j.pragma.2005.09.012 URL |
28 |
Kaup B., Yaxley R. H., Madden C. J., Zwaan R. A., & Lüdtke J . ( 2007). Experiential simulations of negated text information. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60( 7), 976-990.
doi: 10.1080/17470210600823512 URL |
29 |
Khemlani S., Orenes I., Johnson-Laird P. N . ( 2012). Negation: A theory of its meaning, representation, and use. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 24( 5), 541-559.
doi: 10.1080/20445911.2012.660913 URL |
30 |
Kousta S. T., Vigliocco G., Vinson D. P., Andrews M., & Del Campo E . ( 2011). The representation of abstract words: why emotion matters. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 140( 1), 14-34.
doi: 10.1037/a0021446 URL |
31 |
Kousta S. T., Vinson D. P., & Vigliocco G . ( 2009). Emotion words, regardless of polarity, have a processing advantage over neutral words. Cognition, 112( 3), 473-481.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.06.007 URL |
32 | Li F., Bai X., & Shen D . ( 2007). The reverse priming effect of high-frequency words' affective priming. Psychological Science, 30( 6), 1287-1289. |
[ 李芳, 白学军, 沈德立 . ( 2007). 高频靶词情绪启动的反转效应. 心理科学, 30( 6), 1287-1289.] | |
33 | Lü, J. M. & Lu, Z.Y . ( 2013). The vertical-spatial metaphor of emotions in text reading. Journal of Psychological Science, 36( 2), 328-334. |
[ 吕军梅, 鲁忠义 . ( 2013). 为什么快乐在“上”, 悲伤在“下”——语篇阅读中情绪的垂直空间隐喻. 心理科学, 36( 2), 328-334.] | |
34 |
Mayo R., Schul Y., & Burnstein E . ( 2004). “I am not guilty” vs. “I am innocent”: Successful negation may depend on the schema used for its encoding. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40( 4), 433-449.
doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2003.07.008 URL |
35 |
Mendes W. B., Blascovich J., Hunter S. B., Lickel B., & Jost J. T . ( 2007). Threatened by the unexpected: Physiological responses during social interactions with expectancy-violation partners. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92( 4), 698-716.
doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.92.4.698 URL |
36 |
Mitchell D. G. V., Nakic M., Fridberg D., Kamel N., Pine D. S., & Blair, R. J. R. ( 2007). The impact of processing load on emotion . Neuroimage, 34( 3), 1299-1309.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.10.012 URL |
37 | Moffat M., Siakaluk P. D., Sidhu D. M., & Pexman P. M . ( 2015). Situated conceptualization and semantic processing: effects of emotional experience and context availability in semantic categorization and naming tasks. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22( 2), 408-419. |
38 |
Nosek B. A., Hawkins C. B., & Frazier R. S . ( 2011). Implicit social cognition: from measures to mechanisms. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15( 4), 152-159.
doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.01.005 URL |
39 |
Oosterwijk S., Mackey S., Wilson-Mendenhall C., Winkielman P., & Paulus M. P . ( 2015). Concepts in context: Processing mental state concepts with internal or external focus involves different neural systems. Social Neuroscience, 10( 3), 294-307.
doi: 10.1080/17470919.2014.998840 URL |
40 | Oosterwijk S., Winkielman P., Pecher D., Zeelenberg R., Rotteveel M., & Fischer A. H . ( 2012). Mental states inside out: switching costs for emotional and nonemotional sentences that differ in internal and external focus. Memory & Cognition, 40( 1), 93-100. |
41 | Payne B. K., Cheng C. M., Govorun O., & Stewart B. D . ( 2005). An inkblot for attitudes: affect misattribution as implicit measurement. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 89( 3), 277-293. |
42 |
Siakaluk P. D., Knol N., & Pexman P. M . ( 2014). Effects of emotional experience for abstract words in the stroop task. Cognitive Science, 38( 8), 1698-1717.
doi: 10.1111/cogs.2014.38.issue-8 URL |
43 |
Topolinski, S. ( 2014). A processing fluency-account of funniness: Running gags and spoiling punchlines. Cognition and Emotion, 28( 5), 811-820.
doi: 10.1080/02699931.2013.863180 URL |
44 | van Gaal S., Naccache L., Meuwese J. D. I., van Loon A. M., Leighton A. H., Cohen L., & Dehaene S . ( 2014). Can the meaning of multiple words be integrated unconsciously? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B, 369( 1641), 58-68. |
45 |
Vermeulen N., Niedenthal P. M., & Luminet O . ( 2007). Switching between sensory and affective systems incurs processing costs. Cognitive Science, 31( 1), 183-192.
doi: 10.1080/03640210709336990 URL |
46 |
Vigliocco G., Kousta S. T., Della Rosa P. A., Vinson D. P., Tettamanti M., Devlin J. T., & Cappa S. F . ( 2014). The neural representation of abstract words: the role of emotion. Cerebral Cortex, 24( 7), 1767-1777.
doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht025 URL |
47 | Wang Y. N., Zhou L. M., Luo Y. J . ( 2008). The pilot establishment and evaluation of Chinese Affective Words System. Chinese Mental Health Journal, 22( 8), 608-612. |
[ 王一牛, 周立明, 罗跃嘉 . ( 2008). 汉语情感词系统的初步编制及评定. 中国心理卫生杂志, 22( 8), 608-612.] | |
48 |
Williams, L.E., & Bargh J.A, . ( 2008 a). Keeping one’s distance: The influence of spatial distance cues on affect and evaluation. Psychological Science, 19( 3), 302-308.
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02084.x URL |
49 |
Williams, L.E., & Bargh J.A, . ( 2008 b). Experiencing physical warmth promotes interpersonal warmth. Science, 322, 606-607.
doi: 10.1126/science.1162548 URL |
50 |
Yao Z., Yu D. S., Wang L. L., Zhu X. G., Guo J. J., & Wang Z. H . ( 2016). Effects of valence and arousal on emotional word processing are modulated by concreteness: Behavioral and ERP evidence from a lexical decision task. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 110, 231-242.
doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2016.07.499 URL |
51 | Zwaan, R.A .( 2008). Experiential traces and mental simulations in language comprehension. In M. DeVega, A. M. Glenberg, & A. C.Graesser (Eds.), Symbols and Embodiment: Debates on meaning and cognition (pp.165-180). Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK. |
[1] | 张环, 秦锡权, 刘雨, 林琳, 吴捷. 不同强度趋近动机积极情绪对基于语义关联性错误记忆的影响及其神经机制[J]. 心理学报, 2025, 57(3): 349-362. |
[2] | 夏莲香, 刘凯歌, 李新宇, 叶群. 编码方式与叙事连贯性调节情绪对时间顺序记忆的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2025, 57(1): 1-17. |
[3] | 姚雨佳, 颜之悦, 林慧慧, 陈静全, 宣雨阳. 人际距离和策略创造性对人际情绪调节的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2025, 57(1): 125-134. |
[4] | 张环, 王晨, 李俊霞, 林琳, 吴捷. 情绪效价和动机强度对社会分享型提取诱发遗忘的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2024, 56(8): 999-1014. |
[5] | 王丹, 付雨佳, 陈文锋. 社会情境对情绪感染的影响:一项基于EMG的超扫描研究[J]. 心理学报, 2024, 56(8): 1047-1060. |
[6] | 张明杨, 杨盈, 包寒吴霜, 蔡华俭. 中国人的积极理想情绪:近几十年来的变迁[J]. 心理学报, 2024, 56(7): 847-858. |
[7] | 黄顺森, 来枭雄, 张彩, 赵心媚, 代欣然, 祁梦迪, 王欢蕾, 王文荣, 王耘. 青少年手机压力与心理健康的关系:基于多元宇宙样分析和密集追踪方法[J]. 心理学报, 2024, 56(6): 745-758. |
[8] | 孟现鑫, 罗怡, 韩晨媛, 吴国伟, 常姣, 袁加锦, 千坤, 傅小兰. 社会接纳调节社会排斥对情绪冲突适应的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2024, 56(5): 577-593. |
[9] | 童廷婷, 白幼玲, 冯廷勇. 情绪调节改善拖延行为的认知机制:任务厌恶中介作用[J]. 心理学报, 2024, 56(4): 458-468. |
[10] | 王伟晗, 曹斐臻, 余林伟, 曾珂, 杨鑫超, 徐强. 群体信息对面部表情识别的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2024, 56(3): 268-280. |
[11] | 尹华站, 肖春花, 夏安妮, 袁中静, 崔晓冰, 李丹. 基本情绪对时距知觉的影响: 来自三水平元分析和网络元分析的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2024, 56(12): 1676-1690. |
[12] | 张瑾, 许销冰, 庄晓涵. 产品包装的医药符号线索对产品偏好的影响: 感知有效性和规避情绪的中介作用[J]. 心理学报, 2024, 56(12): 1836-1850. |
[13] | 肖程元, 赵世瑞, 袁加锦. 积极认知重评对负性信息传播的调控及多维证据[J]. 心理学报, 2024, 56(11): 1471-1487. |
[14] | 李其容, 王春淼, 孙明慧. 创业激情的“错位”对创业努力和创业成瘾的影响机制[J]. 心理学报, 2024, 56(11): 1568-1584. |
[15] | 田杨阳, 李东, 闫向博, 李曌, 崔倩, 蒋重清. 动态高兴表情评价中的表征动量效应和参照依赖效应[J]. 心理学报, 2024, 56(1): 29-43. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||