心理学报 ›› 2018, Vol. 50 ›› Issue (10): 1071-1082.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2018.01071
• 研究报告 • 下一篇
收稿日期:
2017-10-12
发布日期:
2018-08-23
出版日期:
2018-10-15
基金资助:
WANG Hui-Yuan1, SUI Jie2,3(), ZHANG Ming4()
Received:
2017-10-12
Online:
2018-08-23
Published:
2018-10-15
摘要:
采用线索化范式, 操作性地建立线索和靶子的语义关联, 考察语义关联对注意捕获的影响。实验1采用特征线索意义靶子的形式, 建立某一特定语义水平的注意控制定势, 发现线索靶子语义一致时线索捕获注意, 线索靶子语义不一致时同样的线索失去捕获注意的能力。实验2随机呈现线索颜色和靶子汉字, 建立两种语义概念的注意控制定势, 发现所有的线索都出现了捕获效应, 而不论其在某一具体试次中是否与靶子意义一致。实验3交换了线索与靶子的属性及概念, 采用意义线索特征靶子的形式, 结果与实验1一致。这些结果表明, (1)语义关联的注意捕获符合关联性的无意注意定向假说, 刺激的捕获能力受当前的注意控制定势调节; (2)由语义概念激活的知觉表征在调节空间注意分配上与其自身相比形式相同, 但程度有所下降; (3)知觉特征水平的注意控制定势能够激活与其相应的语义概念, 使其吸引注意并调节空间注意分配; (4)知觉表征和语义概念的激活可能是双向的, 激活后在指导注意转移上表现出相同的特性。
中图分类号:
王慧媛, 隋洁, 张明. (2018). 语义关联的注意捕获——来自线索化范式的证据. 心理学报, 50(10), 1071-1082.
WANG Hui-Yuan, SUI Jie, ZHANG Ming. (2018). Attentional capture is contingent on attentional control setting for semantic meaning: Evidence from modified spatial cueing paradigm. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 50(10), 1071-1082.
图1 实验1流程图 注:首先呈现注视屏500 ms, 然后呈现线索屏100 ms。存在两种颜色线索, 分别为红色和绿色, 线索位置随机。随后再次呈现注视屏100 ms, 最后呈现靶子屏1000 ms。靶子为白色的“红”字或“绿”字, 4个汉字位置随机。根据线索与靶子的组合情况, 实验共有4种条件, 对于每种条件线索颜色和靶子汉字均已确定。实验采用Block设计, 被试的任务是既快又准地判断当前Block内特定靶子字所在方框缺口的位置, 缺口左右随机。
因变量指标/线索有效性 | “红”字靶子 | “绿”字靶子 | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
语义一致 | 语义不一致 | 语义一致 | 语义不一致 | ||
反应时(ms) | 线索无效 | 816.5 (83.9) | 784.6 (86.7) | 840.3 (80.2) | 827.9 (73.7) |
线索有效 | 768.3 (83.8) | 772.2 (69.3) | 806.5 (70.1) | 822.8 (84.2) | |
错误率(%) | 线索无效 | 6.5 (4.9) | 5.6 (4.2) | 8.1 (7.3) | 8.6 (9.1) |
线索有效 | 5.3 (5.7) | 5.4 (4.7) | 5.7 (4.9) | 7.1 (5.7) |
表1 实验1中线索有效性、线索靶子语义一致性和靶子身份间各处理下的平均反应时及错误率(标准差)
因变量指标/线索有效性 | “红”字靶子 | “绿”字靶子 | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
语义一致 | 语义不一致 | 语义一致 | 语义不一致 | ||
反应时(ms) | 线索无效 | 816.5 (83.9) | 784.6 (86.7) | 840.3 (80.2) | 827.9 (73.7) |
线索有效 | 768.3 (83.8) | 772.2 (69.3) | 806.5 (70.1) | 822.8 (84.2) | |
错误率(%) | 线索无效 | 6.5 (4.9) | 5.6 (4.2) | 8.1 (7.3) | 8.6 (9.1) |
线索有效 | 5.3 (5.7) | 5.4 (4.7) | 5.7 (4.9) | 7.1 (5.7) |
因变量指标/线索有效性 | “红”字靶子 | “绿”字靶子 | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
语义一致 | 语义不一致 | 语义一致 | 语义不一致 | ||
反应时(ms) | 线索无效 | 665.8 (106.2) | 662.1 (102.8) | 718.9 (120.6) | 723.1 (123.0) |
线索有效 | 636.7 (104.1) | 641.6 (99.9) | 692.4 (119.1) | 698.4 (115.2) | |
错误率(%) | 线索无效 | 8.5 (4.5) | 6.9 (5.2) | 5.9 (5.5) | 6.4 (6.3) |
线索有效 | 4.3 (4.3) | 5.9 (6.3) | 5.3 (6.2) | 5.1 (6.2) |
表2 实验2中有效性、线索靶子语义一致性和靶子身份间各处理下的平均反应时及错误率(标准差)
因变量指标/线索有效性 | “红”字靶子 | “绿”字靶子 | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
语义一致 | 语义不一致 | 语义一致 | 语义不一致 | ||
反应时(ms) | 线索无效 | 665.8 (106.2) | 662.1 (102.8) | 718.9 (120.6) | 723.1 (123.0) |
线索有效 | 636.7 (104.1) | 641.6 (99.9) | 692.4 (119.1) | 698.4 (115.2) | |
错误率(%) | 线索无效 | 8.5 (4.5) | 6.9 (5.2) | 5.9 (5.5) | 6.4 (6.3) |
线索有效 | 4.3 (4.3) | 5.9 (6.3) | 5.3 (6.2) | 5.1 (6.2) |
图3 实验3流程图 注:首先呈现注视屏500 ms, 然后呈现线索屏100 ms。线索是“红”字、“绿”字或“纱”字, 位置随机。随后再次呈现注视屏100 ms, 最后呈现靶子屏500 ms, 靶子为红色方框或绿色方框。根据线索与靶子的组合情况, 实验采用Block设计, 共有6种条件, 对于每种条件线索字和靶子颜色均已确定。被试的任务是既快又准地判断当前Block内特定颜色靶子框缺口的位置, 缺口左右随机。
因变量指标/线索有效性 | 红色靶子 | 绿色靶子 | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
线索无效 | 线索有效 | 线索无效 | 线索有效 | ||
反应时(ms) | 语义一致 | 509.0 (38.1) | 484.0 (41.7) | 534.7 (40.7) | 511.9( 44.2) |
语义不一致 | 500.5 (44.1) | 498.1 (47.0) | 526.1 (37.5) | 528.1 (39.0) | |
语义无关 | 499.8 (42.9) | 495.8 (51.4) | 516.7 (33.0) | 515.8 (35.4) | |
错误率(%) | 语义一致 | 4.0 (2.8) | 2.1 (2.8) | 8.0 (6.3) | 5.7 (7.5) |
语义不一致 | 4.8 (4.7) | 4.0 (4.4) | 5.6 (4.5) | 5.7 (5.8) | |
语义无关 | 4.1 (3.1) | 5.0 (4.7) | 5.8 (4.8) | 7.1 (7.6) |
表3 实验3中有效性、线索靶子语义一致性和靶子颜色间各处理下的平均反应时及错误率(标准差)
因变量指标/线索有效性 | 红色靶子 | 绿色靶子 | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
线索无效 | 线索有效 | 线索无效 | 线索有效 | ||
反应时(ms) | 语义一致 | 509.0 (38.1) | 484.0 (41.7) | 534.7 (40.7) | 511.9( 44.2) |
语义不一致 | 500.5 (44.1) | 498.1 (47.0) | 526.1 (37.5) | 528.1 (39.0) | |
语义无关 | 499.8 (42.9) | 495.8 (51.4) | 516.7 (33.0) | 515.8 (35.4) | |
错误率(%) | 语义一致 | 4.0 (2.8) | 2.1 (2.8) | 8.0 (6.3) | 5.7 (7.5) |
语义不一致 | 4.8 (4.7) | 4.0 (4.4) | 5.6 (4.5) | 5.7 (5.8) | |
语义无关 | 4.1 (3.1) | 5.0 (4.7) | 5.8 (4.8) | 7.1 (7.6) |
[1] |
Arexis M., Maquestiaux F., Gaspelin N., Ruthruff E., & Didierjean A . ( 2017). Attentional capture in driving displays. British Journal of Psychology, 108( 2), 259-275.
doi: 10.1111/bjop.12197 URL pmid: 28369841 |
[2] |
Barratt D., & Bundesen. C . ( 2012). Attentional capture by emotional faces is contingent on attentional control settings. Cognition and Emotion, 26( 7), 1223-1237.
doi: 10.1080/02699931.2011.645279 URL pmid: 22416914 |
[3] |
Binder J. R., Conant L. L., Humphries C. J., Fernandino L., Simons S. B., & Aguilar M., & Desai R. H . ( 2016). Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 33( 3-4), 130-174.
doi: 10.1080/02643294.2016.1147426 URL pmid: 27310469 |
[4] |
Bottini R., Bucur M., & Crepaldi D . ( 2016). The nature of semantic priming by subliminal spatial words: Embodied or disembodied? Journal of Experimental Psychology General, 145( 9), 1160-1176.
doi: 10.1037/xge0000197 URL pmid: 27359130 |
[5] | Broadbent D. E. ( 1958). Perception and communication. Elmsford, NY, US: Pergamon Press. |
[6] |
Burra N., Barras C., Coll S. Y., & Kerzel D . ( 2016). Electrophysiological evidence for attentional capture by irrelevant angry facial expressions. Biological Psychology, 120, 69-80.
doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2016.08.008 URL pmid: 27568328 |
[7] |
Chee M. W. L., Weekes B., Lee K. M., Soon C. S., Schreiber A., & Hoon J. J., & Chee M . ( 2000). Overlap and dissociation of semantic processing of Chinese characters, English words, and pictures: Evidence from fMRI. Neuroimage, 12( 4), 392-403.
doi: 10.1006/nimg.2000.0631 URL |
[8] |
Eimer M. &Kiss M. . ( 2008). Involuntary attentional capture is determined by task set: Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20( 8), 1423-1433.
doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20099 URL |
[9] | Eimer M., & Kiss M. , ( 2010). Top-down search strategies determine attentional capture in visual search: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72( 4), 951-962. |
[10] |
Folk C. L., Remington R. W., & Johnston J. C . ( 1992). Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18( 4), 1030-1044.
doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.18.4.1030 URL pmid: 1431742 |
[11] |
Folk C. L., Remington R. W., & Wright J. H . ( 1994). The structure of attentional control: Contingent attentional capture by apparent motion, abrupt onset, and color. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20( 2), 317-329.
doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.20.2.317 URL |
[12] | Folk C.L., &Remington R. ,( 1998). Selectivity in distraction by irrelevant featural singletons: Evidence for two forms of attentional capture. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception & Performance, 24( 3), 847-858. |
[13] |
Folk, C. L., &Remington R.W, . ( 2008). Bottom-up priming of top-down attentional control settings. Visual Cognition, 16( 2-3), 215-231.
doi: 10.1080/13506280701458804 URL |
[14] | Folk C.L., &Anderson B.A . ( 2010). Target-uncertainty effects in attentional capture: Color-singleton set or multiple attentional control settings? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17( 3), 421-426. |
[15] |
Gallese V., &Lakoff G. , ( 2005). The brain's concepts: The role of the sensory-motor system in conceptual knowledge. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 22( 3-4), 455-479.
doi: 10.1080/02643290442000310 URL pmid: 21038261 |
[16] |
Giammarco M., Paoletti A., Guild E. B., &Al-Aidroos N . ( 2014). Contingent attentional capture by stimuli that match long-term memory representations. Journal of Vision, 14( 10), 645.
doi: 10.1167/14.10.645 URL |
[17] |
Giammarco M., Paoletti A., Guild E. B., &Al-Aidroos N . ( 2016). Attentional capture by items that match episodic long-term memory representations. Visual Cognition, 24( 1), 78-101
doi: 10.1080/13506285.2016.1195470 URL |
[18] | Glickman M., &Lamy D. ,( 2018). Attentional capture by irrelevant emotional distractor faces is contingent on implicit attentional settings. Cognition and Emotion, 32( 2), 303-314 |
[19] |
Gluckman M., &Johnson S.P, . ( 2013). Attentional capture by social stimuli in young infants. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 527.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00527 URL pmid: 3744870 |
[20] |
Goodhew S. C., Kendall W., Ferber S., & Pratt J . ( 2014). Setting semantics: Conceptual set can determine the physical properties that capture attention. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 76( 6), 1577-1589.
doi: 10.3758/s13414-014-0686-3 URL pmid: 24824982 |
[21] |
Guo Y. N., You X. Q., & Li Y . ( 2016). The role of stimulus type and semantic category-level attentional set in sustained inattentional blindness. Japanese Psychological Research, 58( 4), 332-341.
doi: 10.1111/jpr.2016.58.issue-4 URL |
[22] |
Hill R.A., & Barton R.A . ( 2005). Psychology: Red enhances human performance in contests. Nature, 435, 293.
doi: 10.1038/435293a URL |
[23] |
Huang W. Y., Su Y. L., Zhen Y. F., & Zhe Q . ( 2016). The role of top-down spatial attention in contingent attentional capture. Psychophysiology,53( 5), 650-662.
doi: 10.1111/psyp.12615 URL pmid: 26879628 |
[24] | Huang X. T., Huang W., & Li X. R . ( 1991). The symbolic implication of colors to the Chinese. Journal of Psychological Science, ( 6), 1-7. |
[ 黄希庭, 黄巍, 李小融 . ( 1991). 关于中国人颜色情调的研究. 心理科学, ( 6), 1-7.] | |
[25] |
Huang Y. M., Baddeley A., & Young A. W . ( 2008). Attentional capture by emotional stimuli is modulated by semantic processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception and Performance, 34( 2), 328-339.
doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.34.2.328 URL pmid: 18377174 |
[26] | Institute of Language Teaching and Research. ( 1986). A frequency dictionary of Modern Chinese. Beijing, China: Beijing Language Institute Press. |
[ 北京语言学院语言教学研究所. ( 1986). 现代汉语频率词典. 北京: 北京语言学院出版社.] | |
[27] | Inukai T., &Kawahara J.I . ( 2012). Sex differences in attentional capture (summary of awarded presentation at the 30th annual meeting). The Japanese Journal of Psychonomic Science, 31( 1), 89-90. |
[28] |
Kiss M., &Eimer M. , ( 2011). Attentional capture by size singletons is determined by top-down search goals. Psychophysiology, 48( 6), 784-787.
doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.01145.x URL pmid: 21539576 |
[29] |
Koivisto M. &Revonsuo A. , ( 2007). How meaning shapes seeing. Psychological Science, 18( 10), 845-849.
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01989.x URL |
[30] | Li X. M., He P., & Liu L. Y . ( 2016). Red graphical effect in risk avoidance behavior. Advances in Psychological Science, 24( 3), 351-355. |
[ 李晓明, 和平, 刘林英 . ( 2016). 风险回避行为中的“红色图形效应”. 心理科学进展, 24( 3), 351-355.] | |
[31] | Liu L., & Bai X.J . ( 2016). The effects of attentional control setting and types of cues on attentional capture. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 48( 9), 1093-1104. |
[ 刘丽, 白学军 . ( 2016). 注意控制定势和线索类型在注意捕获中的作用. 心理学报, 48( 9), 1093-1104.] | |
[32] |
Malcolm G. L., Rattinger M., & Shomstein S . ( 2016). Intrusive effects of semantic information on visual selective attention. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 78( 7), 2066-2078.
doi: 10.3758/s13414-016-1156-x URL pmid: 5013139 |
[33] | Preston S.D., &Stansfield R.B . ( 2008). I know how you feel: Task-irrelevant facial expressions are spontaneously processed at a semantic level. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 8( 1), 54-64. |
[34] | Reeder R. R., van Zoest W., & Peelen M. V . ( 2015). Involuntary attentional capture by task-irrelevant objects that match the search template for category detection in natural scenes. Attention Perception & Psychophysics, 77( 4), 1070-1080. |
[35] |
Remington R. W., Folk C. L., & McLean J. P . ( 2001). Contingent attentional capture or delayed allocation of attention? Perception & Psychophysics, 63( 2), 298-307.
doi: 10.3758/BF03194470 URL pmid: 11281104 |
[36] | Schmidt J.R . ( 2014). Contingencies and attentional capture: The importance of matching stimulus informativeness in the item-specific proportion congruent task. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 540. |
[37] |
Schmidt L. J., Belopolsky A. V., & Theeuwes J . ( 2015). Attentional capture by signals of threat. Cognition and Emotion, 29( 4), 687-694.
doi: 10.1080/02699931.2014.924484 URL pmid: 24899117 |
[38] | Su L., Bowman H., & Barnard P . ( 2007) Attentional capture by meaning: A multi-level modelling study. Proceedings of the 29th Meeting of the Annual Cognitive Science Society. Nashville Tennesse, 1521-1526. |
[39] |
Sun S. Z., Shen J., Shaw M., Cant J. S., & Ferber S . ( 2015). Automatic capture of attention by conceptually generated working memory templates. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics,77( 6), 1841-1847.
doi: 10.3758/s13414-015-0918-1 URL pmid: 25944451 |
[40] | Theeuwes J . ( 1991). Cross-dimensional perceptual selectivity. Perception & Psychophysics, 50( 2), 184-193. |
[41] | Theeuwes J .( 2004). Top-down search strategies cannot override attentional capture. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11( 1), 65-70. |
[42] | Wang H.Y . ( 2009). The influence of semantic relevancy on attentional capture (Unpublished Master’s Thesis). China:Northeast Normal University. |
[ 王慧媛 . ( 2009). 语义关联对注意捕获的影响(硕士学位论文). 东北师范大学.] | |
[43] | Wang H. Y., Zhang M., & Sui J . ( 2014). The Effect of Cue-Target Relevance and Search Strategies on Attentional Capture. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 46( 2), 185-195. |
[ 王慧媛, 张明, 隋洁 . ( 2014). 线索靶子关联和搜索策略对注意捕获的作用. 心理学报, 46( 2), 185-195.] | |
[44] | Wang H. Y., Sui J., & Zhang M . ( 2016). The effect of cue-target relevance and search strategies on attentional capture: Evidence from meaning cues. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 48( 7), 783-793. |
[ 王慧媛, 隋洁, 张明 . ( 2016). 线索靶子关联和搜索策略对注意捕获的作用——来自意义线索的证据. 心理学报, 48( 7), 783-793.]
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2016.00783 URL |
|
[45] | Ye C. X., Lu J. C., Zhang J. F., Wang J., Tian L. L., & Liu Q . ( 2017). Strategic inhibition of distractors with visual working memory contents after involuntary attention capture. Scientific Reports. |
[46] | Zhang T.X., & Han B.X . ( 2017). Red is more sexy: Factors and evolutional process. Advances in Psychological Science, 25( 6), 1069-1076. |
[ 张腾霄, 韩布新 . ( 2017). 红色更性感:影响因素与进化基础. 心理科学进展, 25( 6), 1069-1076.] |
[1] | 车晓玮, 徐慧云, 王凯旋, 张倩, 李寿欣. 工作记忆表征精度加工需求对注意引导的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2021, 53(7): 694-713. |
[2] | 黄月胜, 张豹, 范兴华, 黄杰. 无关工作记忆表征的负性情绪信息能否捕获视觉注意?一项眼动研究[J]. 心理学报, 2021, 53(1): 26-37. |
[3] | 郑旭涛, 郭文姣, 陈满, 金佳, 尹军. 社会行为的效价信息对注意捕获的影响:基于帮助和阻碍行为的探讨[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(5): 584-596. |
[4] | 李毕琴, 李玲, 王爱君, 张明. 言语工作记忆内容在语义水平的注意捕获[J]. 心理学报, 2018, 50(5): 483-493. |
[5] | 刘丽;白学军. 注意控制定势和线索类型在注意捕获中的作用[J]. 心理学报, 2016, 48(9): 1093-1104. |
[6] | 王慧媛; 隋洁; 张明. 线索靶子关联和搜索策略对注意捕获的作用 ——来自意义线索的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2016, 48(7): 783-793. |
[7] | 白学军; 刘丽; 宋娟; 郭志英. 特征和位置信息在价值驱动注意捕获中的作用[J]. 心理学报, 2016, 48(11): 1357-1369. |
[8] | 胡艳梅;张明. 基于记忆的注意捕获和注意抑制效应:ERP证据[J]. 心理学报, 2016, 48(1): 12-21. |
[9] | 吴诗玉;马拯. 二语词汇阅读的跨语言语音干扰:语义关联判断任务的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2015, 47(11): 1318-1327. |
[10] | 张微;周兵平;臧玲;莫书亮. 网络成瘾倾向者在视觉工作记忆引导下的注意捕获[J]. 心理学报, 2015, 47(10): 1223-1234. |
[11] | 王慧媛;张明;隋洁. 线索靶子关联和搜索策略对注意捕获的作用[J]. 心理学报, 2014, 46(2): 185-195. |
[12] | 刘丽;李运;李力红;白学军. 自上而下的注意控制定势在注意捕获中的作用[J]. 心理学报, 2014, 46(10): 1442-1453. |
[13] | 马利军;张积家;杜凯. 语义分解性在惯用语理解中的作用[J]. 心理学报, 2013, 45(4): 391-405. |
[14] | 胡中华,赵光,刘强,李红. 视觉搜索任务中直视探测优势的眼动研究[J]. 心理学报, 2012, 44(4): 435-445. |
[15] | 魏萍;康冠兰. 注意捕获效应受知觉负载及目标与干扰项距离的共同影响[J]. 心理学报, 2012, 44(11): 1454-1462. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||