[1] Beech, J. R. (2002). Individual differences in mature readers in reading, spelling, and grapheme-phoneme conversion. Current Psychology, 21(2), 121-132. [2] Blau V., Reithler J., van Atteveldt N, Seitz, J., Gerretsen P., Goebel R., & Blomert L. (2010). Deviant processing of letters and speech sounds as proximate cause of reading failure: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study of dyslexic children. Brain, 133(3), 868-879. [3] Blau V., van Atteveldt N., Ekkebus M., Goebel R., & Blomert L. (2009). Reduced neural integration of letters and speech sounds links phonological and reading deficits in adult dyslexia. Current Biology, 19(6), 503-508. [4] Blomert, L. (2011). The neural signature of orthographic- phonological binding in successful and failing reading development. Neuroimage, 57(3), 695-703. [5] Chung K. K. H., McBride-Chang C., Wong S. W. L., Cheung H., Penney T. B., & Ho, C. S. H. (2008). The role of visual and auditory temporal processing for Chinese children with developmental dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 58, 15-35. [6] Clayton, F. J., & Hulme, C. (2018). Automatic activation of sounds by letters occurs early in development but is not impaired in children with dyslexia. Scientific Studies of Reading, 22(2), 137-151. [7] Colavita, F. B. (1974). Human sensory dominance. Perception & Psychophysics, 16(2), 409-412. [8] Evans K. K.(2020). The role of selective attention in cross- modal interactions between auditory and visual features. Cognition, 196, Article 104119. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2019.104119 [9] Faul F., Erdfelder E., Lang A. G., & Buchner A. (2007). G*power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175-191. [10] Francisco A. A., Jesse A., Groen M. A., & McQueen J. M. (2017). A general audiovisual temporal processing deficit in adult readers with dyslexia. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 60(1), 144-158. [11] Froyen D., Willems G., & Blomert L. (2011). Evidence for a specific cross-modal association deficit in dyslexia: An electrophysiological study of letter-speech sound processing. Developmetal Science, 14(4), 635-648. [12] Hairston W. D., Burdette J. H., Flowers D. L., Wood F. B., & Wallace M. T. (2005). Altered temporal profile of visual-auditory multisensory interactions in dyslexia. Experimental Brain Research, 166(3-4), 474-480. [13] Kast M., Bezzola L., Jäncke L., & Meyer M. (2011). Multi- and unisensory decoding of words and nonwords result in differential brain responses in dyslexic and nondyslexic adults. Brain and Language, 119(3), 136-148. [14] Koelewijn T., Bronkhorst A. W., & Theeuwes J. (2010). Attention and the multiple stages of multisensory integration: A review of audiovisual studies. Acta Psychologica, 134(3), 372-384. [15] Kronschnabel J., Brem S., Maurer U., & Brandeis D. (2014). The level of audiovisual print-speech integration deficits in dyslexia. Neuropsychologia, 62, 245-261. [16] Law J. M.,De Vos, A., Vanderauwera, J., Wouters, J., Ghesquiere, P., & Vandermosten, M.(2018). Grapheme- phoneme learning in an unknown Orthography: A study in typical reading and dyslexic children. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, Article 1393. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01393 [17] Li D., Hu K. D., Chen J. P., Jin Y., & Li M. (1988). Report of Combined Raven Test (CRT) in Shanghai urban area. Journal of Psychological Science, 4(7), 29-33. [李丹, 胡克定, 陈国鹏, 金瑜, 李眉. (1988). 瑞文测验联合型(CRT)上海市区试测报告. 心理科学通讯, 4(7), 29-33.] [18] Lu, W. (2001). The disagreement on number (quantity) and composition distribution of modern Chinese syllables. Language Teaching and Linguistic Studies, (6), 28-34. [卢偓. (2001). 现代汉语音节的数量与构成分布. 语言教学与研究, (6), 28-34.] [19] Lyon G. R., Shaywitz S. E., & Shaywitz B. A. (2003). A definition of dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 53(1), 1-14. [20] Macoir J., Fossard M., Saint-Pierre M., & Auclair-Ouellet N. (2012). Phonological or procedural dyslexia: Specific deficit of complex grapheme-to-phoneme conversion. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 25(3), 163-177. [21] Mittag M., Thesleff P., Laasonen M., & Kujala T. (2013). The neurophysiological basis of the integration of written and heard syllables in dyslexic adults. Clinical Neurophysiology, 124(2), 315-326. [22] Nash H. M., Gooch D., Hulme C., Mahajan Y., McArthur G., Steinmetzger K., & Snowling M. J. (2016). Are the literacy difficulties that characterize developmental dyslexia associated with a failure to integrate letters and speech sounds? Developmental Science, 20(4), Article e12423. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12423 [23] Qian Y.,& Bi, H. Y.(2014). The visual magnocellular deficit in Chinese-speaking children with developmental dyslexia. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, Article 692. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00692 [24] Shaywitz S. M.D. (2019). Overcoming dyslexia. (Liu et al., Trans.). Random House Inc. (Original work publish 2003). [萨利·施维茨. (2019). 聪明的笨小孩. (刘丽等译). 北京: 北京师范大学出版社.] [25] Wang X. L.,& Tao, B. P. (1996). Chinese character recognition test battery and assessment scale for primary school children. Shanghai, China: Shanghai Education Press. [王孝玲, 陶保平. (1996). 小学生识字量测试题库及评价量表. 上海: 上海教育出版社.] [26] Wimmer, H., & Mayringer, H. (2002). Dysfluent reading in the absence of spelling difficulties: A specific disability in regular orthographies. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94(2), 272-277. [27] Yang Y., Yang Y. H., Li J. J., Xu M.,& Bi, H. Y.(2020). An audiovisual integration deficit underlies reading failure in nontransparent writing systems: An fMRI study of Chinese children with dyslexia. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 54, Article 100884. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2019.100884 |