心理学报 ›› 2023, Vol. 55 ›› Issue (6): 920-929.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2023.00920
张丹丹1,2(), 李宜伟1, 于文汶2, 莫李澄1,2, 彭程3, 刘黎黎3
收稿日期:
2022-10-24
发布日期:
2023-03-06
出版日期:
2023-06-25
通讯作者:
张丹丹
E-mail:zhangdd05@gmail.com
基金资助:
ZHANG Dandan1,2(), LI Yiwei1, YU Wenwen2, MO Licheng1,2, PENG Cheng3, LIU Lili3
Received:
2022-10-24
Online:
2023-03-06
Published:
2023-06-25
摘要:
“负性偏向”指人们往往优先感知、注意和记忆负性(而非正性)信息, 负性偏向是情绪领域能观察到的最稳定的情绪加工现象, 普遍存在于幼儿、儿童、青少年和成人之中。但是在婴儿中如何呢?本研究采用近红外成像技术, 考察了45名0岁新生儿(日龄5.2 ± 1.7天, 23名男孩)和45名1周岁婴儿(月龄12.7 ± 1.4月, 21名男孩)对快乐和愤怒语音韵律的大脑反应。结果表明, 在大脑右半球的额叶、颞叶和顶叶的脑功能连接中出现了情绪和组别的交互作用:在新生儿组, 快乐语音条件下的脑功能连接强度高于愤怒语音条件; 在婴儿组, 愤怒语音条件下的脑功能连接强于快乐语音条件。此结果证实了我们的假设:人类刚出生时情绪加工偏向是正性的, 之后才转变成为负性。因此, “负性偏向”不是与生俱来的, 情绪加工偏向在个体发展的早期阶段会发生转变。本研究首次从发展的角度揭示了情绪偏向的变化, 该发展规律不但有利于及早发现自闭症等情绪和认知发育障碍, 还能指导家长运用恰当的交流和抚养方式科学育儿。
中图分类号:
张丹丹, 李宜伟, 于文汶, 莫李澄, 彭程, 刘黎黎. (2023). 0~1岁婴儿情绪偏向的发展:近红外成像研究. 心理学报, 55(6), 920-929.
ZHANG Dandan, LI Yiwei, YU Wenwen, MO Licheng, PENG Cheng, LIU Lili. (2023). Development of emotional bias in infants aged from 0 to 1 year old: A near-infrared spectroscopy study. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 55(6), 920-929.
通道编号 | 解剖位置(AAL标签) | 通道编号 | 解剖位置(AAL标签) |
---|---|---|---|
1 | Inferior frontal gyrus (triangular) left | 8 | Inferior frontal gyrus (triangular) right |
2 | Inferior frontal gyrus (triangular) left | 9 | Inferior frontal gyrus (triangular) right |
3 | Superior temporal gyrus left | 10 | Superior temporal gyrus right |
4 | Postcentral gyrus left | 11 | Postcentral gyrus right |
5 | Superior temporal gyrus left | 12 | Superior temporal gyrus right |
6 | Supramarginal gyrus left | 13 | Supramarginal gyrus right |
7 | Angular gyrus left | 14 | Angular gyrus right |
表1 近红外通道的空间定位(婴儿头模)
通道编号 | 解剖位置(AAL标签) | 通道编号 | 解剖位置(AAL标签) |
---|---|---|---|
1 | Inferior frontal gyrus (triangular) left | 8 | Inferior frontal gyrus (triangular) right |
2 | Inferior frontal gyrus (triangular) left | 9 | Inferior frontal gyrus (triangular) right |
3 | Superior temporal gyrus left | 10 | Superior temporal gyrus right |
4 | Postcentral gyrus left | 11 | Postcentral gyrus right |
5 | Superior temporal gyrus left | 12 | Superior temporal gyrus right |
6 | Supramarginal gyrus left | 13 | Supramarginal gyrus right |
7 | Angular gyrus left | 14 | Angular gyrus right |
图2 情绪类别对脑功能连接的主效应。第一行图:愤怒语音比中性语音引起的更强的脑功能连接(愤怒 > 中性)。第二行图:快乐语音比中性语音引起的更强的脑功能连接(快乐 > 中性)。对主效应显著(FWE矫正91次)的脑功能连接, 在愤怒、快乐、中性三种条件间进行两两比较(Bonferroni矫正), 图中为情绪间差异显著的脑功能连接(FWE矫正91次)。脑功能连接的颜色(见colorbar)和粗细(越粗值越大)均反映t值。
通道编号 | 组别×情绪 | 新生儿组 快乐 > 愤怒 | 婴儿组 愤怒 > 快乐 | 快乐语音 新生儿 > 婴儿 | 愤怒语音 婴儿 > 新生儿 | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
F | p | |||||
2-9 | 9.73 | 0.011 | t = 4.59, p = 0.004 | |||
2-13 | 12.18 | 0.001 | t = 4.08, p = 0.027 | |||
3-9 | 8.48 | 0.031 | t = 3.91, p = 0.016 | |||
6-13 | 10.47 | 0.007 | t = 3.61, p = 0.046 | |||
7-8 | 12.15 | 0.001 | t = 4.06, p = 0.029 | t = 3.66, p = 0.039 | ||
7-10 | 9.81 | 0.012 | t = 3.96, p = 0.041 | t = 4.16, p = 0.007 | ||
7-12 | 8.36 | 0.040 | t = 3.92, p = 0.016 | |||
8-12 | 16.00 | 0.000 | t = 5.69, p = 0.000 | |||
8-13 | 10.03 | 0.015 | t = 4.05, p = 0.030 | |||
8-14 | 18.21 | 0.000 | t = 4.74, p = 0.002 | t = 5.17, p = 0.000 | t = 5.09, p = 0.000 | |
9-10 | 15.10 | 0.000 | t = 4.59, p = 0.004 | t = 4.68, p = 0.003 | t = 5.14, p = 0.000 | |
9-11 | 17.04 | 0.000 | t = 3.93, p = 0.046 | t = 5.41, p = 0.000 | t = 4.39, p = 0.003 | |
9-12 | 15.50 | 0.000 | t = 4.80, p = 0.002 | t = 4.07, p = 0.028 | t = 3.63, p = 0044 | |
9-13 | 14.74 | 0.000 | t = 3.96, p = 0.041 | t = 4.59, p = 0.004 | t = 4.45, p = 0.002 | |
9-14 | 13.26 | 0.001 | t = 4.65, p = 0.003 | t = 4.23, p = 0.005 | ||
10-12 | 9.67 | 0.040 | t = 5.46, p = 0.000 | t = 3.82, p = 0.022 | ||
10-13 | 14.01 | 0.001 | t = 4.01, p = 0.034 | t = 5.20, p = 0.000 | t = 4.00, p = 0.012 | |
10-14 | 17.93 | 0.000 | t = 4.30, p = 0.012 | t = 5.29, p = 0.000 | t = 5.66, p = 0.000 | |
11-13 | 9.46 | 0.016 | t = 4.38, p = 0.009 | |||
11-14 | 11.76 | 0.002 | t = 4.63, p = 0.003 | t = 3.59, p = 0.049 | ||
12-14 | 9.39 | 0.025 | t = 4.27, p = 0.004 |
表2 情绪和组别对脑功能连接的交互作用
通道编号 | 组别×情绪 | 新生儿组 快乐 > 愤怒 | 婴儿组 愤怒 > 快乐 | 快乐语音 新生儿 > 婴儿 | 愤怒语音 婴儿 > 新生儿 | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
F | p | |||||
2-9 | 9.73 | 0.011 | t = 4.59, p = 0.004 | |||
2-13 | 12.18 | 0.001 | t = 4.08, p = 0.027 | |||
3-9 | 8.48 | 0.031 | t = 3.91, p = 0.016 | |||
6-13 | 10.47 | 0.007 | t = 3.61, p = 0.046 | |||
7-8 | 12.15 | 0.001 | t = 4.06, p = 0.029 | t = 3.66, p = 0.039 | ||
7-10 | 9.81 | 0.012 | t = 3.96, p = 0.041 | t = 4.16, p = 0.007 | ||
7-12 | 8.36 | 0.040 | t = 3.92, p = 0.016 | |||
8-12 | 16.00 | 0.000 | t = 5.69, p = 0.000 | |||
8-13 | 10.03 | 0.015 | t = 4.05, p = 0.030 | |||
8-14 | 18.21 | 0.000 | t = 4.74, p = 0.002 | t = 5.17, p = 0.000 | t = 5.09, p = 0.000 | |
9-10 | 15.10 | 0.000 | t = 4.59, p = 0.004 | t = 4.68, p = 0.003 | t = 5.14, p = 0.000 | |
9-11 | 17.04 | 0.000 | t = 3.93, p = 0.046 | t = 5.41, p = 0.000 | t = 4.39, p = 0.003 | |
9-12 | 15.50 | 0.000 | t = 4.80, p = 0.002 | t = 4.07, p = 0.028 | t = 3.63, p = 0044 | |
9-13 | 14.74 | 0.000 | t = 3.96, p = 0.041 | t = 4.59, p = 0.004 | t = 4.45, p = 0.002 | |
9-14 | 13.26 | 0.001 | t = 4.65, p = 0.003 | t = 4.23, p = 0.005 | ||
10-12 | 9.67 | 0.040 | t = 5.46, p = 0.000 | t = 3.82, p = 0.022 | ||
10-13 | 14.01 | 0.001 | t = 4.01, p = 0.034 | t = 5.20, p = 0.000 | t = 4.00, p = 0.012 | |
10-14 | 17.93 | 0.000 | t = 4.30, p = 0.012 | t = 5.29, p = 0.000 | t = 5.66, p = 0.000 | |
11-13 | 9.46 | 0.016 | t = 4.38, p = 0.009 | |||
11-14 | 11.76 | 0.002 | t = 4.63, p = 0.003 | t = 3.59, p = 0.049 | ||
12-14 | 9.39 | 0.025 | t = 4.27, p = 0.004 |
图3 情绪类别和组别对脑功能连接的交互作用(简单效应检验结果)。第一行图:新生儿组的正性偏向(快乐 > 愤怒)和婴儿组的负性偏向(愤怒 > 快乐)。第二行图:加工正性韵律语音的脑功能连接在两个年龄组中的差异(新生儿 > 婴儿)和加工负性韵律语音的脑功能连接在两个年龄组中的差异(新生儿 < 婴儿)。对交互作用显著(FWE矫正)的脑功能连接, 在愤怒、快乐、中性三种条件间进行两两比较(Bonferroni矫正), 图中为情绪间差异显著的脑功能连接(FWE矫正)。
[1] |
Adolphs R., Damasio H., Tranel D., Cooper G., & Damasio A. R. (2000). A role for somatosensory cortices in the visual recognition of emotion as revealed by three-dimensional lesion mapping. Journal of Neuroscience, 20(7), 2683-2690.
pmid: 10729349 |
[2] |
Arimitsu T., Uchida-Ota M., Yagihashi T., Kojima S., Watanabe S., Hokuto I., … Minagawa-Kawai Y. (2011). Functional hemispheric specialization in processing phonemic and prosodic auditory changes in neonates. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 202.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00202 pmid: 21954386 |
[3] | Aryani A., Hsu C. T., & Jacobs A. M. (2018). The sound of words evokes affective brain responses. Brain Sciences, 8(6), 94. |
[4] |
Baumeister R. F., Bratslavsky E., Finkenauer C., & Vohs K. D. (2001). Bad is stronger than good. Review of General Psychology, 5(4), 323-370.
doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.5.4.323 URL |
[5] | Bayley N. (2006). Bayley scales of infant and toddler development (3 ed.). San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment. |
[6] |
Belyk M., & Brown S. (2014). Perception of affective and linguistic prosody: An ALE meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9(9), 1395-1403.
doi: 10.1093/scan/nst124 pmid: 23934416 |
[7] |
Blasi A., Lloyd-Fox S., Sethna V., Brammer M. J., Mercure E., Murray L., … Johnson M. H. (2015). Atypical processing of voice sounds in infants at risk for autism spectrum disorder. Cortex, 71, 122-133.
doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2015.06.015 pmid: 26200892 |
[8] |
Bowen H. J., Kark S. M., & Kensinger E. A. (2018). Never forget: Negative emotional valence enhances recapitulation. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25(3), 870-891.
doi: 10.3758/s13423-017-1313-9 URL |
[9] |
Brigadoi S., Aljabar P., Kuklisova-Murgasova M., Arridge S. R., & Cooper R. J. (2014). A 4D neonatal head model for diffuse optical imaging of pre-term to term infants. Neuroimage, 100, 385-394.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.06.028 pmid: 24954280 |
[10] |
Brück C., Kreifelts B., & Wildgruber D. (2011). Emotional voices in context: A neurobiological model of multimodal affective information processing. Physics of Life Reviews, 8(4), 383-403.
doi: 10.1016/j.plrev.2011.10.002 pmid: 22035772 |
[11] | Cacioppo J. T., Cacioppo S., & Gollan J. K. (2014). The negativity bias: Conceptualization, quantification, and individual differences. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 37(3), 309. |
[12] |
Cheng Y.W., Lee S. Y., Chen H. Y., Wang P. Y., & Decety J. (2012). Voice and emotion processing in the human neonatal brain. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24(6), 1411-1419.
doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00214 pmid: 22360593 |
[13] |
Cope M., & Delpy D. T. (1988). System for long-term measurement of cerebral blood and tissue oxygenation on newborn infants by near infra-red transillumination. Medical and Biological Engineering and Computing, 26(3), 289-294.
pmid: 2855531 |
[14] |
Ethofer T., Bretscher J., Gschwind M., Kreifelts B., Wildgruber D., & Vuilleumier P. (2012). Emotional voice areas: Anatomic location, functional properties, and structural connections revealed by combined fMRI/DTI. Cerebral Cortex, 22(1), 191-200.
doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr113 URL |
[15] |
Farroni T., Menon E., Rigato S., & Johnson M. H. (2007). The perception of facial expressions in newborns. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 4(1), 2-13.
pmid: 20228970 |
[16] |
Fernald A. (1993). Approval and disapproval: Infant responsiveness to vocal affect in familiar and unfamiliar languages. Child Development, 64(3), 657-674.
pmid: 8339687 |
[17] |
Filippa M., Monaci M. G., & Grandjean D. (2019). Emotion attribution in nonverbal vocal communication directed to preterm infants. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 43(1), 91-104.
doi: 10.1007/s10919-018-0288-1 |
[18] |
Fivush R. (1991). Gender and emotion in mother-child conversations about the past. Journal of Narrative and Life History, 1(4), 325-341.
doi: 10.1075/jnlh URL |
[19] | Gerhardt S. (2004). Why love matters: How affection shapes a baby's brain. Routledge. |
[20] |
Gómez D. M., Berent I., Benavides-Varela S., Bion R. A., Cattarossi L., Nespor M., & Mehler J. (2014). Language universals at birth. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(16), 5837-5841.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1318261111 URL |
[21] |
Grandjean D., Sander D., Pourtois G., Schwartz S., Seghier M. L., Scherer K. R., & Vuilleumier P. (2005). The voices of wrath: Brain responses to angry prosody in meaningless speech. Nature Neuroscience, 8(2), 145-146.
doi: 10.1038/nn1392 pmid: 15665880 |
[22] | Griffiths R. (1970). The abilities of young children: A comprehensive system of mental measurement for the first eight years of life. Association for Research in Infant and Child Development. |
[23] |
Grossmann T., Oberecker R., Koch S. P., & Friederici A. D. (2010). The developmental origins of voice processing in the human brain. Neuron, 65(6), 852-858.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.03.001 pmid: 20346760 |
[24] |
Hartwigsen G., Baumgaertner A., Price C. J., Koehnke M., Ulmer S., & Siebner H. R. (2010). Phonological decisions require both the left and right supramarginal gyri. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(38), 16494-16499.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1008121107 URL |
[25] |
Hoehl S., & Striano T. (2010). The development of emotional face and eye gaze processing. Developmental Science, 13(6), 813-825.
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00944.x pmid: 20977553 |
[26] |
Hutman T., Rozga A., DeLaurentis A. D., Barnwell J. M., Sugar C. A., & Sigman M. (2010). Response to distress in infants at risk for autism: A prospective longitudinal study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51(9), 1010-1020.
doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02270.x URL |
[27] |
Ito T. A., Larsen J. T., Smith N. K. &, Cacioppo J. T. (1998). Negative information weighs more heavily on the brain: The negativity bias in evaluative categorizations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75(4), 887-900.
doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.75.4.887 pmid: 9825526 |
[28] | Jahromi L. B., Putnam S. P., & Stifter C. A. (2004). Maternal regulation of infant reactivity from 2 to 6 months. Developmental Psychology, 40(4), 477. |
[29] |
Key A. P., Ibanez L. V., Henderson H. A., Warren Z., Messinger D. S., & Stone W. L. (2015). Positive affect processing and joint attention in infants at high risk for autism: An exploratory study. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45(12), 4051-4062.
doi: 10.1007/s10803-014-2191-x pmid: 25056131 |
[30] |
Kirby L. A., & Robinson J. L. (2017). Affective mapping: An activation likelihood estimation (ALE) meta-analysis. Brain and Cognition, 118, 137-148.
doi: S0278-2626(15)00046-9 pmid: 26074298 |
[31] |
Köchel A., Schöngassner F., & Schienle A. (2013). Cortical activation during auditory elicitation of fear and disgust: A near-infrared spectroscopy (NIRS) study. Neuroscience Letters, 549, 197-200.
doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2013.06.062 pmid: 23831343 |
[32] |
Kotz S. A., Kalberlah C., Bahlmann J., Friederici A. D., & Haynes J. D. (2013). Predicting vocal emotion expressions from the human brain. Human Brain Mapping, 34(8), 1971-1981.
doi: 10.1002/hbm.22041 pmid: 22371367 |
[33] |
LaBarbera J. D., Izard C. E., Vietze P., & Parisi S. A. (1976). Four- and six-month-old infants’ visual response to joy, anger and neutral expressions. Child Development, 47(2), 535-538.
pmid: 1269322 |
[34] | Liu J., Tsang T., Ponting C., Jackson L., Jeste S. S., Bookheimer S. Y., & Dapretto M. (2021). Lack of neural evidence for implicit language learning in 9-month-old infants at high risk for autism. Developmental Science,24(4), e13078. |
[35] | Liu P., & Pell M. D. (2012). Recognizing vocal emotions in Mandarin Chinese: A validated database of Chinese vocal emotional stimuli. Behavior Research Methods, 44(4), 1042-1051. |
[36] |
LoBue V., & DeLoache J. S. (2010). Superior detection of threat-relevant stimuli in infancy. Developmental Science, 13(1), 221-228.
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00872.x pmid: 20121878 |
[37] |
Marusak H. A., Zundel C. G., Brown S., Rabinak C. A., & Thomason M. E. (2017). Convergent behavioral and corticolimbic connectivity evidence of a negativity bias in children and adolescents. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12(4), 517-525.
doi: 10.1093/scan/nsw182 pmid: 28175919 |
[38] |
Mastropieri D., & Turkewitz G. (1999). Prenatal experience and neonatal responsiveness to vocal expressions of emotion. Developmental Psychobiology, 35(3), 204-214.
pmid: 10531533 |
[39] |
Paley J., & Alpert J. (2003). Memory of infant trauma. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 20(2), 329-347.
doi: 10.1037/0736-9735.20.2.329 URL |
[40] |
Peltola M. J., Leppänen J. M., Mäki S., & Hietanen J. K. (2009). Emergence of enhanced attention to fearful faces between 5 and 7 months of age. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 4(2), 134-142.
doi: 10.1093/scan/nsn046 pmid: 19174536 |
[41] |
Rigato S., Farroni T., & Johnson M. H. (2010). The shared signal hypothesis and neural responses to expressions and gaze in infants and adults. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 5(1), 88-97.
doi: 10.1093/scan/nsp037 pmid: 19858107 |
[42] | Saito Y., Aoyama S., Kondo T., Fukumoto R., Konishi N., Nakamura K., … Toshima T. (2007). Frontal cerebral blood flow change associated with infant-directed speech. Archives of Disease in Childhood. Fetal and Neonatal Edition, 92( 2), F113-F116. |
[43] |
Schirmer A., & Kotz S. A. (2006). Beyond the right hemisphere: Brain mechanisms mediating vocal emotional processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(1), 24-30.
pmid: 16321562 |
[44] | Shi F., Yap P. T., Wu G., Jia H., Gilmore J. H., Lin W., & Shen D. (2011). Infant brain atlases from neonates to 1-and 2-year-olds. PloS One, 6(4), e18746. |
[45] |
Singh L., Morgan J. L., & Best C. T. (2002). Infants' listening preferences: Baby talk or happy talk? Infancy, 3(3), 365-394.
doi: 10.1207/S15327078IN0303_5 pmid: 33451217 |
[46] |
Soroka S., Fournier P., & Nir L. (2019). Cross-national evidence of a negativity bias in psychophysiological reactions to news. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 116(38), 18888-18892.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1908369116 URL |
[47] |
Telkemeyer S., Rossi S., Koch S. P., Nierhaus T., Steinbrink J., Poeppel D., … Wartenburger l. (2009). Sensitivity of newborn auditory cortex to the temporal structure of sounds. Journal of Neuroscience, 29(47), 14726-14733.
doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1246-09.2009 pmid: 19940167 |
[48] |
Tzourio-Mazoyer N., Landeau B., Papathanassiou D., Crivello F., Etard O., Delcroix N.,... Joliot M. (2002). Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. Neuroimage, 15(1), 273-289.
doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0978 pmid: 11771995 |
[49] |
Vaish A., Grossmann T., & Woodward A. (2008). Not all emotions are created equal: The negativity bias in social- emotional development. Psychological Bulletin, 134(3), 383-403.
doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.134.3.383 URL |
[50] |
Wu Y. J., Hou X., Peng C., Yu W., Oppenheim G. M., Thierry G., & Zhang D. (2022). Rapid learning of a phonemic discrimination in the first hours of life. Nature Human Behaviour, 6, 1169-1179.
doi: 10.1038/s41562-022-01355-1 pmid: 35654965 |
[51] | Xia M., Wang J., & He Y. (2013). BrainNet Viewer: A network visualization tool for human brain connectomics. PloS One, 8(7), e68910. |
[52] |
Zhang D., Chen Y., Ao X., Sun G., Liu L., Hou X., & Chen Y. (2019). Early preference for positive over negative prosody in neonates: Evidence based on event-related potentials. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 51(4), 462-470.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2019.00462 |
[张丹丹, 陈钰, 敖翔, 孙国玉, 刘黎黎, 侯新琳, 陈玉明. (2019). 新生儿情绪性语音加工的正性偏向——来自事件相关电位的证据. 心理学报, 51(4), 462-470.]
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2019.00462 |
|
[53] |
Zhang D., Chen Y., Hou X., & Wu Y. J. (2019). Near‐ infrared spectroscopy reveals neural perception of vocal emotions in human neonates. Human Brain Mapping, 40(8), 2434-2448.
doi: 10.1002/hbm.v40.8 URL |
[54] |
Zhang D., Liu Y., Hou X., Sun G., Cheng Y., & Luo Y. (2014). Discrimination of fearful and angry emotional voices in sleeping human neonates: A study of the mismatch brain responses. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 8, 422.
doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00422 pmid: 25538587 |
[1] | 张梅, 丁书恒, 刘国芳, 徐亚珍, 傅鑫媛, 张巍, 辛自强. 网络突发事件中的负性偏向:产生与表现[J]. 心理学报, 2021, 53(12): 1361-1375. |
[2] | 雷震, 毕蓉, 莫李澄, 于文汶, 张丹丹. 外显和内隐情绪韵律加工的脑机制:近红外成像研究[J]. 心理学报, 2021, 53(1): 15-25. |
[3] | 雷怡, 夏琦, 莫志凤, 李红. 面孔可爱度和客观熟悉度对婴儿面孔注意偏向效应的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(7): 811-822. |
[4] | 张丹丹, 陈钰, 敖翔, 孙国玉, 刘黎黎, 侯新琳, 陈玉明. 新生儿情绪性语音加工的正性偏向——来自事件相关电位的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2019, 51(4): 462-470. |
[5] | 魏华;汪涛;周宗奎;冯文婷;丁倩. 叠音品牌名称对消费者知觉和偏好的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2016, 48(11): 1479-1488. |
[6] | 魏萍;康冠兰;丁锦红;郭春彦. 奖赏预期对面孔情绪加工的影响:一项事件相关电位研究[J]. 心理学报, 2014, 46(4): 437-449. |
[7] | 陶冶;徐琴美. 12个月中国婴儿熟悉词表征中声调的语义特性[J]. 心理学报, 2013, 45(10): 1111-1118. |
[8] | 陶冶,徐琴美,im Plunkett. 不同母语环境下16个月婴儿词汇表征中声调的语义特性[J]. 心理学报, 2012, 44(8): 1066-1074. |
[9] | 黄宇霞,罗跃嘉. 负性情绪刺激是否总是优先得到加工: ERP研究[J]. 心理学报, 2009, 41(09): 822-831. |
[10] | 董奇,陶沙,张华,刘玉新,李蓓蕾. 婴儿问题解决行为的特点与发展[J]. 心理学报, 2002, 34(01): 62-67. |
[11] | 陶沙,董奇,王雁平,J.J.Campos. 爬行经验对婴儿迂回行为发展的影响[J]. 心理学报, 1999, 31(1): 69-75. |
[12] | 曾琦,董奇,陶沙. 婴儿客体永久性发展机制的研究[J]. 心理学报, 1997, 29(4): 393-399. |
[13] | 董奇,陶沙,曾琦,芦咏莉,王雁萍. 8─11个月婴儿迂回行为发展特点与学习能力的研究[J]. 心理学报, 1997, 29(3): 286-293. |
[14] | 王垒,张岚,李黎. 婴儿识别面部表情的能力及其发展模式[J]. 心理学报, 1994, 26(2): 169-175. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||