心理学报 ›› 2025, Vol. 57 ›› Issue (4): 559-572.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2025.0559 cstr: 32110.14.2025.0559
收稿日期:
2023-09-30
发布日期:
2025-02-06
出版日期:
2025-04-25
通讯作者:
王争艳, E-mail: wangzhengyan@cnu.edu.cn作者简介:
第一联系人:吴楣和梁熙为共同第一作者。
基金资助:
WU Mei, LIANG Xi, SHI Jiahui, WANG Zhengyan()
Received:
2023-09-30
Online:
2025-02-06
Published:
2025-04-25
摘要:
2007年Hamlin等人在《自然》上发表的研究表明6个月婴儿具备社会评价能力, 对个体在社会互动中的“帮助”和“阻碍”行为产生不同反应, 并对帮助者表现出偏好。此后, 不断有学者对此结果的可复制性提出质疑。本研究采用Hamlin等人(
中图分类号:
吴楣, 梁熙, 时嘉惠, 王争艳. (2025). 6个月婴儿社会评价的个体差异初探. 心理学报, 57(4), 559-572.
WU Mei, LIANG Xi, SHI Jiahui, WANG Zhengyan. (2025). Individual differences in the social evaluation of 6-month-old infants. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 57(4), 559-572.
图1 实验程序 注:以习惯化阶段黄色三角形“帮助者”, 蓝色正方形“阻碍者”为例。图1a左图为“帮助者”试次, 箭头向上表示“帮助者”帮助“爬山者”爬上山坡; 右图为“阻碍者”试次, 箭头向下表示“阻碍者”将“爬山者”推下山坡。图1b左图为“爬山者”接近“帮助者”, 即预期内事件, 箭头向左表示“爬山者”接近“帮助者”; 右图为“爬山者”接近“阻碍者”, 即预期违背事件, 箭头向右表示“爬山者”接近“阻碍者”。彩图见电子版, 下同。
预测因素 | B | SE B | Wald χ² | p | OR | 95% CI OR |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
外向性 | 0.68 | 0.25 | 7.10 | 0.008** | 1.97 | [1.19, 3.24] |
消极情绪性 | 0.04 | 0.24 | 0.02 | 0.88 | 1.04 | [0.65, 1.65] |
调节性 | 0.11 | 0.34 | 0.10 | 0.75 | 1.11 | [0.57, 2.17] |
常量 | −0.08 | 0.16 | 0.24 | 0.62 | 0.92 |
表1 预测手动选择的二元Logistic回归分析
预测因素 | B | SE B | Wald χ² | p | OR | 95% CI OR |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
外向性 | 0.68 | 0.25 | 7.10 | 0.008** | 1.97 | [1.19, 3.24] |
消极情绪性 | 0.04 | 0.24 | 0.02 | 0.88 | 1.04 | [0.65, 1.65] |
调节性 | 0.11 | 0.34 | 0.10 | 0.75 | 1.11 | [0.57, 2.17] |
常量 | −0.08 | 0.16 | 0.24 | 0.62 | 0.92 |
[1] | Ambady, N., & Rosenthal, R. (1992). Thin slices of expressive behavior as predictors of interpersonal consequences: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 111(2), 256-274. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.111.2.256 |
[2] | Baker, M. (2016). 1, 500 Scientists lift the lid on reproducibility. Nature, 533(7604), 452-454. https://doi.org/10.1038/533452a |
[3] |
Berdan, L. E., Keane, S. P., & Calkins, S. D. (2008). Temperament and externalizing behavior: Social preference and perceived acceptance as protective factors. Developmental Psychology, 44(4), 957-968. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.44.4.957
doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.44.4.957 URL pmid: 18605827 |
[4] | Biro, S. (2023). Twelve months old infants’ evaluation of observed comforting behavior using a choice paradigm: The role of animacy cues and self-distress. Infancy, 28(4), 807-819. https://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12542 |
[5] | Bloom, P. (2013). Horrible children:The limits of natural morality. In M. R. Banaji & S. A. Gelman (Eds.), Navigating the social world: What infants, children, and other species can teach us (pp. 348-351). Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199890712.003.0064 |
[6] |
Brownell, C. A. (2013). Early development of prosocial behavior: Current perspectives. Infancy, 18(1), 1-9. https://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12004
URL pmid: 25632273 |
[7] | Brownell, C. A., & The early social development research lab. (2016). Prosocial behavior in infancy: The role of socialization. Child Development Perspectives, 10(4), 222-227. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12189 |
[8] |
Brownell, C. A., Svetlova, M., & Nichols, S. (2009). To share or not to share: When do toddlers respond to another’s needs? Infancy, 14(1), 117-130. https://doi.org/10.1080/15250000802569868
doi: 10.1080/15250000802569868 URL pmid: 22639549 |
[9] |
Canfield, C. F., & Saudino, K. J. (2016). The influence of infant characteristics and attention to social cues on early vocabulary. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 150, 112-129. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2016.05.005
doi: S0022-0965(16)30025-X URL pmid: 27280332 |
[10] | Chen, X. (2018). Culture, temperament, and social and psychological adjustment. Developmental Review, 50, 42-53. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2018.03.004 |
[11] | Chen, X., & Schmidt, L. A. (2015). Temperament and personality. In M. E. Lamb & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology and developmental science: Socioemotional processes (7th ed., pp. 152-200). John Wiley & Sons, Inc.. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118963418.childpsy305 |
[12] | Cowell, J. M., & Decety, J. (2015a). Precursors to morality in development as a complex interplay between neural, socioenvironmental, and behavioral facets. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112(41), 12657-12662. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1508832112 |
[13] | Cowell, J. M., & Decety, J. (2015b). The neuroscience of implicit moral evaluation and its relation to generosity in early childhood. Current Biology, 25(1), 93-97. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2014.11.002 |
[14] |
Cunningham, W. A., Johnson, M. K., Raye, C. L., Gatenby, J. C., Gore, J. C., & Banaji, M. R. (2004). Separable neural components in the processing of black and white faces. Psychological Science, 15(12), 806-813. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00760.x
doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00760.x URL pmid: 15563325 |
[15] |
Cushman, F., Sheketoff, R., Wharton, S., & Carey, S. (2013). The development of intent-based moral judgment. Cognition, 127(1), 6-21. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2012.11.008
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2012.11.008 URL pmid: 23318350 |
[16] | Dahl, A., Goeltz, M. T., & Brownell, C. A. (2022). Scaffolding the emergence of infant helping: A longitudinal experiment. Child Development, 93(3), 751-759. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13710 |
[17] |
Dahl, A., Satlof-Bedrick, E. S., Hammond, S. I., Drummond, J. K., Waugh, W. E., & Brownell, C. A. (2017). Explicit scaffolding increases simple helping in younger infants. Developmental Psychology, 53(3), 407-416. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000244
doi: 10.1037/dev0000244 URL pmid: 27854464 |
[18] |
Dahl, A., Schuck, R. K., & Campos, J. J. (2013). Do young toddlers act on their social preferences? Developmental Psychology, 49(10), 1964-1970. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031460
doi: 10.1037/a0031460 URL pmid: 23316770 |
[19] | Darwin, C. (1871). The descent of man, and Selection in relation to sex. (Vol 1, pp. viii, 423). John Murray. https://doi.org/10.1037/12293-000 |
[20] |
Decety, J., & Meyer, M. (2008). From emotion resonance to empathic understanding: A social developmental neuroscience account. Development and Psychopathology, 20(4), 1053-1080. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579408000503
doi: 10.1017/S0954579408000503 URL pmid: 18838031 |
[21] |
Duncan, G. J., Engel, M., Claessens, A., & Dowsett, C. J. (2014). Replication and robustness in developmental research. Developmental Psychology, 50(11), 2417-2425. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037996
doi: 10.1037/a0037996 URL pmid: 25243330 |
[22] |
Dunfield, K., Kuhlmeier, V. A., O’Connell, L., & Kelley, E. (2011). Examining the diversity of prosocial behavior: Helping, sharing, and comforting in infancy. Infancy, 16(3), 227-247. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-7078.2010.00041.x
doi: 10.1111/j.1532-7078.2010.00041.x URL pmid: 32693496 |
[23] |
Edwards, A., Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T. L., Reiser, M., Eggum-Wilkens, N. D., & Liew, J. (2015). Predicting sympathy and prosocial behavior from young children’s dispositional sadness. Social Development, 24(1), 76-94. https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12084
URL pmid: 25663753 |
[24] | Eisenberg, N., Damon, W., & Lerner, R. M. (Eds.). (2006). Handbook of child psychology: Social, emotional, and personality development (6th ed.). John Wiley & Sons, Inc.. |
[25] | Fox, N. A., & Henderson, H. A. (1999). Does infancy matter? Predicting social behavior from infant temperament. Infant Behavior & Development, 22(4), 445-455. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0163-6383(00)00018-7 |
[26] |
Gartstein, M. A., Bell, M. A., & Calkins, S. D. (2014). EEG asymmetry at 10 months of age: Are temperament trait predictors different for boys and girls? Developmental Psychobiology, 56(6), 1327-1340. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.21212
doi: 10.1002/dev.21212 URL pmid: 24634135 |
[27] | Gartstein, M. A., Gonzalez, C., Carranza, J. A., Ahadi, S. A., Ye, R., Rothbart, M. K., & Yang, S. W. (2006). Studying cross-cultural differences in the development of infant temperament: People’s Republic of China, the United States of America, and Spain. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 37(2), 145-161. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10578-006-0025-6 |
[28] | Gartstein, M. A., & Rothbart, M. K. (2003). Studying infant temperament via the revised infant behavior questionnaire. Infant Behavior & Development, 26(1), 64-86. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0163-6383(02)00169-8 |
[29] | Geraci, A., & Surian, L. (2011). The developmental roots of fairness: Infants’ reactions to equal and unequal distributions of resources. Developmental Science, 14(5), 1012-1020. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01048.x |
[30] | Hamlin, J. K. (2013a). Failed attempts to help and harm: Intention versus outcome in preverbal infants’ social evaluations. Cognition, 128(3), 451-474. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2013.04.004 |
[31] | Hamlin, J. K. (2013b). Moral judgment and action in preverbal infants and toddlers: Evidence for an innate moral core. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22(3), 186-193. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721412470687 |
[32] | Hamlin, J. K. (2014). Context-dependent social evaluation in 4.5-month-old human infants: The role of domain-general versus domain-specific processes in the development of social evaluation. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 614. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00614 |
[33] | Hamlin, J. K. (2015a). The infantile origins of our moral brains. In J. Decety & T. Wheatley (Eds.), The moral brain: A multidisciplinary perspective (pp. 105-122). Boston Review. |
[34] | Hamlin, J. K. (2015b). The case for social evaluation in preverbal infants: Gazing toward one’s goal drives infants’ preferences for Helpers over Hinderers in the hill paradigm. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1563. https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01563 |
[35] |
Hamlin, J. K., & Wynn, K. (2011). Young infants prefer prosocial to antisocial others. Cognitive Development, 26(1), 30-39. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2010.09.001
URL pmid: 21499550 |
[36] | Hamlin, J., Wynn, K. & Bloom, P. (2007). Social evaluation by preverbal infants. Nature, 450, 557-559. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature06288 |
[37] |
Hamlin, J. K., Wynn, K., & Bloom, P. (2010). Three-month- olds show a negativity bias in their social evaluations. Developmental Science, 13(6), 923-929. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00951.x
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00951.x URL pmid: 20977563 |
[38] | Hamlin, J. K., Wynn, K., Bloom, P., & Mahajan, N. (2011). How infants and toddlers react to antisocial others. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(50), 19931-19936. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1110306108 |
[39] | Hammond, S. I., & Carpendale, J. I. M. (2015). Helping children help: The relation between maternal scaffolding and children’s early help. Social Development, 24(2), 367-383. https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12104 |
[40] | Hoffman, M. L. (2000). Empathy and moral development: Implications for caring and justice. Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511805851 |
[41] |
Hoijtink, H., van Kooten, P., & Hulsker, K. (2016). Why Bayesian psychologists should change the way they use the bayes factor. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 51(1), 2-10. https://doi.org/10.1080/00273171.2014.969364
doi: 10.1080/00273171.2014.969364 URL pmid: 26881951 |
[42] | Holvoet, C., Scola, C., Arciszewski, T., & Picard, D. (2016). Infants’ preference for prosocial behaviors: A literature review. Infant Behavior and Development, 45, 125-139. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2016.10.008 |
[43] | Hu, C.-P., Kong, X., Wagenmakers, E.-J., Ly, A., & Peng, K. (2018). The Bayes factor and its implementation in JASP: A practical primer. Advances in Psychological Science, 26(6), 951-965. https://doi.org/10.3724/SP.J.1042.2018.00951 |
[44] | JASP, Team. (2023). JASP (Version 0.17.2) [Computer software]. |
[45] | Jeffreys, H. (1961). Theory of probability (3rd ed.). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. |
[46] | Jin, K., Houston, J. L., Baillargeon, R., Groh, A. M., & Roisman, G. I. (2018). Young infants expect an unfamiliar adult to comfort a crying baby: Evidence from a standard violation-of-expectation task and a novel infant-triggered- video task. Cognitive Psychology, 102, 1-20. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2017.12.004 |
[47] | Jones, V., Wang, Z., Yuan, S., Pham, C., Putnam, S. P., & Gartstein, M. A. (2021). Cross-cultural differences in temperament between the United States and the People’s Republic of China: A longitudinal comparison. Asian Journal of Pediatric Research, 7(3), 31-43. https://doi.org/10.9734/ajpr/2021/v7i330219 |
[48] | Killen, M., & Smetana, J. G. (2015). Origins and development of morality. In M. E. Lamb & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology and developmental science: Socioemotional processes (7th ed., pp. 701-749). John Wiley & Sons, Inc.. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118963418.childpsy317 |
[49] | Kohnstamm, G. A. (1989). Temperament in childhood:Cross- cultural and sex differences. In G. A. Kohnstamm, J. E. Bates, & M. K. Rothbart (Eds.), Temperament in childhood (pp. 483-508). John Wiley & Sons. |
[50] |
Kuhlmeier, V., Wynn, K., & Bloom, P. (2003). Attribution of dispositional states by 12-month-olds. Psychological Science, 14(5), 402-408. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.01454
URL pmid: 12930468 |
[51] |
Laible, D. (2004). Mother-child discourse in two contexts: Links with child temperament, attachment security, and socioemotional competence. Developmental Psychology, 40(6), 979-992. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.40.6.979
URL pmid: 15535752 |
[52] | Laible, D. J., Agalar, A. E., Norden, C. V., & Cruz, A. (2023). Temperament and prosocial behavior. In M. Davidov & T. Malti (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of prosociality: Development, mechanisms, promotion (pp. 300-319). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108876681.017 |
[53] | Laws, K. R. (2013). Negativland—A home for all findings in psychology. BMC Psychology, 1(2). https://doi.org/10.1186/2050-7283-1-2 |
[54] |
Li, H. H., Shang, S. Y., & Xie, X. F. (2022). The role of genes in altruistic behavior: Evidence from quantitative genetics and molecular genetics. Advances in Psychological Science, 30(7), 1574-1588.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2022.01574 |
[李海虹, 尚思源, 谢晓非. (2022). 利他行为的遗传基础: 来自定量遗传学和分子遗传学的证据. 心理科学进展, 30(7), 1574-1588.]
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2022.01574 |
|
[55] | Liang, Z. B., Sun, L., Zhang, G. Z., Chen, H. C., & Zhang, P. (2011). The relations between paternal emotional expressivity and children’s social adjustment: Moderate effect of child temperament. Psychological Development and Education, (4), 351-358. |
[梁宗保, 孙铃, 张光珍, 陈会昌, 张萍. (2011). 父亲情绪表达与儿童社会适应: 气质的调节作用. 心理发展与教育, (4), 351-358. doi:10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2011.04.007.] | |
[56] | Licata, M., Paulus, M., Thoermer, C., Kristen, S., Woodward, A. L., & Sodian, B. (2014). Mother-infant interaction quality and infants’ ability to encode actions as goal-directed. Social Development, 23(2), 340-356. https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12057 |
[57] | Lucca, K., Capelier-Mourguy, A., Byers-Heinlein, K., Cirelli, L., Dal Ben, R., Frank, M. C., … Hamlin, K. (2021, June 28). Infants’ social evaluation of helpers and hinderers: A large-scale, multi-lab, coordinated replication study. PsyArxiv Preprints. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/qhxkm |
[58] | Margoni, F., & Surian, L. (2017). Children’s intention-based moral judgments of helping agents. Cognitive Development, 41, 46-64. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2016.12.001 |
[59] | Margoni, F., & Surian, L. (2018). Infants’ evaluation of prosocial and antisocial agents: A meta-analysis. Developmental Psychology, 54(8), 1445-1455. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000538 |
[60] |
Masson, M. E. J. (2011). A tutorial on a practical Bayesian alternative to null-hypothesis significance testing. Behavior Research Methods, 43(3), 679-690. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-010-0049-5
doi: 10.3758/s13428-010-0049-5 URL pmid: 21302025 |
[61] | Mullen, B., Bryant, B., & Driskell, J. E. (1997). Presence of others and arousal: An integration. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice, 1(1), 52-64. https://doi.org/10.1037/1089-2699.1.1.52 |
[62] |
Muthukrishna, M., Bell, A. V., Henrich, J., Curtin, C. M., Gedranovich, A., McInerney, J., & Thue, B. (2020). Beyond Western, Educated, Industrial, Rich, and Democratic (WEIRD) psychology: Measuring and mapping scales of cultural and psychological distance. Psychological Science, 31(6), 678-701. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797620916782
doi: 10.1177/0956797620916782 URL pmid: 32437234 |
[63] |
Oakes, L. M. (2017). Sample size, statistical power, and false conclusions in infant looking-time research. Infancy, 22(4), 436-469. https://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12186
doi: 10.1111/infa.12186 URL pmid: 28966558 |
[64] |
Paulus, M. (2018). The multidimensional nature of early prosocial behavior: A motivational perspective. Current Opinion in Psychology, 20, 111-116. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.09.003
doi: S2352-250X(17)30174-4 URL pmid: 28988024 |
[65] |
Premack, D., & Premack, A. J. (1997). Infants attribute value ± to the goal-directed actions of self-propelled objects. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9(6), 848-856. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.1997.9.6.848
doi: 10.1162/jocn.1997.9.6.848 URL pmid: 23964604 |
[66] |
Putnam, S. P., Helbig, A. L., Gartstein, M. A., Rothbart, M. K., & Leerkes, E. (2014). Development and assessment of short and very short forms of the infant behavior questionnaire-revised. Journal of Personality Assessment, 96(4), 445-458. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223891.2013.841171
doi: 10.1080/00223891.2013.841171 URL pmid: 24206185 |
[67] | Raggio, E., Hendi, S. F.-F., Modesti, C., Presaghi, F., & Nicolais, G. (2015). Temperamento e attaccamento nell’acquisizione dei precursori morali: Alcuni dati preliminari [Temperament and attachment in the development of moral precursors: Preliminary data]. Infanzia e Adolescenza, 14(3), 197-217. |
[68] | Rothbart, M. K. (1981). Measurement of temperament in infancy. Child Development, 52(2), 569-578. https://doi.org/10.2307/1129176 |
[69] |
Salley, B., Miller, A., & Bell, M. A. (2013). Associations between temperament and social responsiveness in young children. Infant and Child Development, 22(3), 270-288. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.1785
URL pmid: 24068881 |
[70] | Salvadori, E., Blazsekova, T., Volein, A., Karap, Z., Tatone, D., Mascaro, O., & Csibra, G. (2015). Probing the strength of infants’ preference for helpers over hinderers: Two replication attempts of Hamlin and Wynn (2011). PLoS ONE, 10(11), e0140570. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0140570 |
[71] | Scarf, D., Imuta, K., Colombo, M., & Hayne, H. (2012). Social evaluation or simple association? Simple associations may explain moral reasoning in infants. PLoS ONE, 7(8), e42698. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0042698 |
[72] | Schlingloff, L., Csibra, G., & Tatone, D. (2020). Do 15-month-old infants prefer helpers? A replication of Hamlin et al. (2007). Royal Society Open Science, 7(4), 191795. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.191795 |
[73] | Shimizu, Y., Senzaki, S., & Uleman, J. S. (2018). The influence of maternal socialization on infants’ social evaluation in two cultures. Infancy, 23(5), 748-766. |
[74] | Shultz, T. R., & Wells, D. (1985). Judging the intentionality of action-outcomes. Developmental Psychology, 21(1), 83-89. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.21.1.83 |
[75] | Tomasello, M., Dweck, C. S., Silk, J. B., Skyrms, B., & Spelke, E. S. Collaborators. (2009). Why we cooperate. MIT Press.Vaish, A., & Tomasello, M. (2014). The early ontogeny of human cooperation and morality. In M. Killen & J. G. Smetana (Eds.), Handbook of moral development (2nd ed., pp. 279-298). Psychology Press. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203581957.ch13 |
[76] | Vaughan, A., Mundy, P., Block, J., Burnette, C., Delgado, C., Gomez, Y., … Pomares, Y. (2009). Child, caregiver, and temperament contributions to infant joint attention. Infancy. 4(4), 603-616. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327078IN0404_11 |
[77] | Vertsberger, D., Israel, S., & Knafo-Noam, A. (2019). Genetics, parenting, and moral development. In D. J. Laible, G. Carlo, & L. M. Padilla-Walker (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of parenting and moral development (pp. 107-127). Oxford University Press |
[78] |
Wagenmakers, E.-J., Lodewyckx, T., Kuriyal, H., & Grasman, R. (2010). Bayesian hypothesis testing for psychologists: A tutorial on the Savage-Dickey method. Cognitive Psychology, 60(3), 158-189. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2009.12.001
doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2009.12.001 URL pmid: 20064637 |
[79] | Wagenmakers, E.-J., Love, J., Marsman, M., Jamil, T., Ly, A., Verhagen, J., … Morey, R. D. (2018a). Bayesian inference for psychology. Part II: Example applications with JASP. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25(1), 58-76. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-017-1323-7 |
[80] | Wagenmakers, E.-J., Marsman, M., Jamil, T., Ly, A., Verhagen, J., Love, J., … Morey, R. D. (2018b). Bayesian inference for psychology. Part I: Theoretical advantages and practical ramifications. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25(1), 35-57. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-017-1343-3 |
[81] | Wang, M. T., & Kenny, S. (2014). Longitudinal links between fathers' and mothers' harsh verbal discipline and adolescents' conduct problems and depressive symptoms. Child development, 85(3), 908-923. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12143 |
[82] | Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2015). The developmental and evolutionary origins of human helping and sharing. In D. A. Schroeder & W. G. Graziano (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of prosocial behavior (pp. 100-113). Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780195399813.013.007 |
[83] |
Winter, L., & Uleman, J. S. (1984). When are social judgments made? Evidence for the spontaneousness of trait inferences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47(2), 237-252. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.47.2.237
URL pmid: 6481615 |
[84] | Wong, T. K. Y., Konishi, C., & Kong, X. (2021). Parenting and prosocial behaviors: A meta-analysis. Social Development, 30(2), 343-373. https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12481 |
[85] | Woo, B. M., Steckler, C. M., Le, D. T., & Hamlin, J. K. (2017). Social evaluation of intentional, truly accidental, and negligently accidental helpers and harmers by 10-month- old infants. Cognition, 168, 154-163. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2017.06.029 |
[86] | Wu, Y.-Y., Yang, Y., & Uzzi, B. (2023). A discipline-wide investigation of the replicability of psychology papers over the past two decades. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 120(6), e2208863120. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2208863120 |
[87] | Wynn, K. (2008). Some innate foundations of social and moral cognition. In P. Carruthers & S. Laurence (Eds.), The innate mind (Volume 3, pp. 330-347). Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195332834.003.0017 |
[88] | Wynn, K. (2009). Constraints on natural altruism. British Journal of Psychology, 100(3), 481-485. https://doi.org/10.1348/000712609X441312 |
[89] |
Zhang, J., Wang, H., Shi, S., Huang, X., Liu, G., Lian, G., & Shi, J. (2009). Reliability and validity of standardized Chinese version of urban infant-toddler social and emotional assessment. Early Human Development, 85(5), 331-336. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2008.12.012
doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2008.12.012 URL pmid: 19181462 |
[90] |
Zheng, X. T., Guo, W. J., Chen, M., Jin, J., & Yin, J. (2020). Influence of the valence of social actions on attentional capture: Focus on helping and hindering actions. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 52(5), 584-596. https://doi.org/10.3724/SP.J.1041.2020.00584
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2020.00584 URL |
[郑旭涛, 郭文姣, 陈满, 金佳, 尹军. (2020). 社会行为的效价信息对注意捕获的影响:基于帮助和阻碍行为的探讨. 心理学报, 52(5), 584-596.]
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2020.00584 |
|
[91] | Zhong, J. J., Zoltan, D., & Chen, Z. Y. (2017). Bayesian inference be introduced to the psychological research: Its necessity, ideas and field. Journal of Psychological Science, (6), 1477-1482. |
[钟建军, Zoltan, Dienese, 陈中永. (2017). 心理研究引入贝叶斯统计推断的必要性、应用思路与领域. 心理科学, (6), 1477-1482. doi:10.16719/j.cnki.1671-6981.20170630.] |
[1] | 张丹丹, 李宜伟, 于文汶, 莫李澄, 彭程, 刘黎黎. 0~1岁婴儿情绪偏向的发展:近红外成像研究[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(6): 920-929. |
[2] | 张青, 王争艳. 母亲敏感性与婴儿气质、注意对学步儿执行功能影响的交互作用:一项两年的追踪研究[J]. 心理学报, 2022, 54(2): 141-153. |
[3] | 雷怡, 夏琦, 莫志凤, 李红. 面孔可爱度和客观熟悉度对婴儿面孔注意偏向效应的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(7): 811-822. |
[4] | 刘湍丽, 白学军. 部分线索对记忆提取的影响:认知抑制能力的作用[J]. 心理学报, 2017, 49(9): 1158-1171. |
[5] | 朱祖德, 段懿行, 王穗苹. 个体差异对工作记忆训练迁移效果的调节[J]. 心理学报, 2017, 49(8): 1022-1030. |
[6] | 魏华;汪涛;周宗奎;冯文婷;丁倩. 叠音品牌名称对消费者知觉和偏好的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2016, 48(11): 1479-1488. |
[7] | 王雨晴;游旭群;焦健;谌鹏飞. 观点采择:基于自我的推理及其个体差异[J]. 心理学报, 2015, 47(8): 1039-1049. |
[8] | 贺琼;王争艳;王莉;蒋彩虹;上官芳芳. 新入园幼儿的皮质醇变化与上呼吸道感染的关系:气质的作用[J]. 心理学报, 2014, 46(4): 516-527. |
[9] | 陶冶;徐琴美. 12个月中国婴儿熟悉词表征中声调的语义特性[J]. 心理学报, 2013, 45(10): 1111-1118. |
[10] | 陶冶,徐琴美,im Plunkett. 不同母语环境下16个月婴儿词汇表征中声调的语义特性[J]. 心理学报, 2012, 44(8): 1066-1074. |
[11] | 刘玲,李荆广,宋宜颖,刘嘉. COMT基因对注意控制神经基础的调控效应:影像遗传学研究的元分析[J]. 心理学报, 2012, 44(4): 446-458. |
[12] | 李董平,张卫,李丹黎,王艳辉,甄霜菊. 教养方式、气质对青少年攻击的影响:独特、差别与中介效应检验[J]. 心理学报, 2012, 44(2): 211-225. |
[13] | 张晓. 童年早期社会能力的发展:一个潜变量增长模型[J]. 心理学报, 2011, 43(12): 1388-1397. |
[14] | 张璇,杨玉芳. 工作记忆广度对语篇理解中重读效应的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2010, 42(07): 727-734. |
[15] | 张晓,王晓艳,陈会昌. 气质与童年早期的师生关系:家庭情感环境的作用[J]. 心理学报, 2010, 42(07): 768-778. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||