[1] |
Bagnis A., Celeghin A., Mosso C. O., & Tamietto M. (2019). Toward an integrative science of social vision in intergroup bias. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 102, 318-326.
doi: S0149-7634(18)30907-2
pmid: 31042557
|
[2] |
Barrouillet P., & Camos V. (2014). Working memory: Loss and reconstruction. Psychology Press.
|
[3] |
Bascandziev I., & Harris P. L. (2014). In beauty we trust: Children prefer information from more attractive informants. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 32(1), 94-99.
doi: 10.1111/bjdp.12022
pmid: 24164592
|
[4] |
Bliss-Moreau E., Barrett L. F., & Wright C. I. (2008). Individual differences in learning the affective value of others under minimal conditions. Emotion, 8(4), 479-493.
doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.8.4.479
pmid: 18729580
|
[5] |
Bonnefon J. F., Hopfensitz A., & de Neys W. (2017). Can we detect cooperators by looking at their face? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 26(3), 276-281.
doi: 10.1177/0963721417693352
URL
|
[6] |
Buchan N. R., Croson R. T. A., & Solnick S. (2008). Trust and gender: An examination of behavior and beliefs in the Investment Game. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 68(3), 466-476.
doi: 10.1016/j.jebo.2007.10.006
URL
|
[7] |
Bzdok D., Langner R., Caspers S., Kurth F., Habel U., Zilles K., Laird A., & Eickhoff S. B. (2011). ALE meta-analysis on facial judgments of trustworthiness and attractiveness. Brain Structure & Function, 215(3-4), 209-223.
|
[8] |
Chauvin A., Worsley K. J., Schyns P. G., Arguin M., & Gosselin F. (2005). Accurate statistical tests for smooth classification images. Journal of Vision, 5(9), 659-667.
pmid: 16356076
|
[9] |
Chen F. F., Jing Y., & Lee J. M. (2014). The looks of a leader: Competent and trustworthy, but not dominant. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 51, 27-33.
doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2013.10.008
URL
|
[10] |
Diedenhofen B., & Musch J. (2015). Cocor: A comprehensive solution for the statistical comparison of correlations. Plos One, 10(3), e0121945.
|
[11] |
Dion K., Walster E., & Berscheid E. (1972). What is beautiful is good. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 24(3), 285-290.
doi: 10.1037/h0033731
pmid: 4655540
|
[12] |
Dion K. K. (1972). Physical attractiveness and evaluation of children's transgressions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 24(2), 207-213.
doi: 10.1037/h0033372
pmid: 5081193
|
[13] |
Dong Y., Liu Y., Jia Y., Li Y., & Li C. (2018). Effects of facial expression and facial gender on judgment of trustworthiness: The modulating effect of cooperative and Competitive settings. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, e2022.
|
[14] |
Dotsch R., & Todorov A. (2012). Reverse correlating social face perception. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 3(5), 562-571.
doi: 10.1177/1948550611430272
URL
|
[15] |
Dotsch R., Wigboldus D. H., Langner O., & van Knippenberg A. (2008). Ethnic out-group faces are biased in the prejudiced mind. Psychological Science, 19(10), 978-980.
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02186.x
pmid: 19000205
|
[16] |
Dotsch R., Wigboldus D. H. J., & van Knippenberg A. (2011). Biased allocation of faces to social categories. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100(6), 999-1014.
doi: 10.1037/a0023026
pmid: 21443368
|
[17] |
Engell A. D., Haxby J. V., & Todorov A. (2007). Implicit trustworthiness decisions: Automatic coding of face properties in the human amygdala. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(9), 1508-1519.
doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.9.1508
pmid: 17714012
|
[18] |
Freeman J. B., & Ambady N. (2011). A dynamic interactive theory of person construal. Psychological Review, 118(2), 247-279.
doi: 10.1037/a0022327
pmid: 21355661
|
[19] |
Freeman J. B., & Johnson K. L. (2016). More than meets the eye: Split-second social perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(5), 362-374.
doi: S1364-6613(16)00050-4
pmid: 27050834
|
[20] |
Gosselin F., & Schyns P. G. (2003). Superstitious perceptions reveal properties of internal representations. Psychological Science, 14(5), 505-509.
pmid: 12930484
|
[21] |
Gutierrez-Garcia A., Beltran D., & Calvo M. G. (2019). Facial attractiveness impressions precede trustworthiness inferences: Lower detection thresholds and faster decision latencies. Cognition & Emotion, 33(2), 378-385.
|
[22] |
Hehman E., Ingbretsen Z. A., & Freeman J. B. (2014). The neural basis of stereotypic impact on multiple social categorization. Neuroimage, 101, 704-711.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.07.056
pmid: 25094016
|
[23] |
Karremans J. C., Dotsch R., & Corneille O. (2011). Romantic relationship status biases memory of faces of attractive opposite-sex others: Evidence from a reverse-correlation paradigm. Cognition, 121(3), 422-426.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2011.07.008
pmid: 21903209
|
[24] |
Krosch A. R., & Amodio D. M. (2014). Economic scarcity alters the perception of race. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 111(25), 9079-9084.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1404448111
pmid: 24927595
|
[25] |
Langlois J. H., Kalakanis L., Rubenstein A. J., Larson A., Hallam M., & Smoot M. (2000). Maxims or myths of beauty? A meta-analytic and theoretical review. Psychological Bulletin, 126(3), 390-423.
doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.126.3.390
pmid: 10825783
|
[26] |
Lin C. J., Keles U., & Adolphs R. (2021). Four dimensions characterize attributions from faces using a representative set of english trait words. Nature Communications, 12(1), 5168.
|
[27] |
Lloyd E. P., Sim M., Smalley E., Bernstein M. J., & Hugenberg K. (2020). Good cop, bad cop: Race-based differences in mental representations of police. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 46(8), 1205-1218.
doi: 10.1177/0146167219898562
pmid: 31948382
|
[28] |
Loftus E. F. (1975). Reconstructing memory: The incredible eyewitness. Jurimetrics Journal, 15(3), 188-193.
|
[29] |
Maister L., de Beukelaer S., Longo M. R., & Tsakiris M. (2021). The self in the mind's eye: Revealing how we truly see ourselves through reverse correlation. Psychological Science, 32(12), 1965-1978.
doi: 10.1177/09567976211018618
URL
|
[30] |
Mayer R. C., Davis J. H., & Schoorman F. D. (1995). An integrative model of organizational trust. Academy of Management Review, 20(3), 709-734.
doi: 10.2307/258792
URL
|
[31] |
Mecklinger A., Rosburg T., & Johansson M. (2016). Reconstructing the past: The late posterior negativity (LPN) in episodic memory studies. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 68, 621-638.
doi: S0149-7634(16)30172-5
pmid: 27365154
|
[32] |
Mende-Siedlecki P., Said C. P., & Todorov A. (2013). The social evaluation of faces: A meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8(3), 285-299.
doi: 10.1093/scan/nsr090
pmid: 22287188
|
[33] |
Moon K., Kim S., Kim J., Kim H., & Ko Y. G. (2020). The mirror of mind: Visualizing mental representations of self through reverse correlation. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 8.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00008
URL
|
[34] |
Oosterhof N. N., & Todorov A. (2008). The functional basis of face evaluation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 105(32), 11087-11092.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.0805664105
pmid: 18685089
|
[35] |
Paunonen S. V. (2006). You are honest, therefore I like you and find you attractive. Journal of Research in Personality, 40(3), 237-249.
doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2004.12.003
URL
|
[36] |
Ratner K. G., Dotsch R., Wigboldus D. H. J., van Knippenberg A., & Amodio D. M. (2014). Visualizing minimal ingroup and outgroup faces: Implications for impressions, attitudes, and behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 106(6), 897-911.
doi: 10.1037/a0036498
pmid: 24841095
|
[37] |
Sutherland C. A. M., & Young A. W. (2022). Understanding trait impressions from faces. British Journal of Psychology, 113(4), 1056-1078.
doi: 10.1111/bjop.12583
pmid: 35880691
|
[38] |
Todorov A., Olivola C. Y., Dotsch R., & Mende-Siedlecki P. (2015). Social attributions from faces: Determinants, consequences, accuracy, and functional significance. Annual Review of Psychology, 66, 519-545.
doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143831
pmid: 25196277
|
[39] |
Todorov A., Pakrashi M., & Oosterhof N. N. (2009). Evaluating faces on trustworthiness after minimal time exposure. Social Cognition, 27(6), 813-833.
doi: 10.1521/soco.2009.27.6.813
URL
|
[40] |
Todorov A., & Uleman J. S. (2002). Spontaneous trait inferences are bound to actors' faces: Evidence from a false recognition paradigm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(5), 1051-1065.
pmid: 12416911
|
[41] |
Todorov A., & Uleman J. S. (2003). The efficiency of binding spontaneous trait inferences to actors' faces. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39(6), 549-562.
doi: 10.1016/S0022-1031(03)00059-3
URL
|
[42] |
Todorov A., & Uleman J. S. (2004). The person reference process in spontaneous trait inferences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87(4), 482-493.
pmid: 15491273
|
[43] |
Wang Z., Bovik A. C., Sheikh H. R., & Simoncelli E. P. (2004). Image quality assessment: From error visibility to structural similarity. IEEE Transactions on Image Processing, 13(4), 600-612.
doi: 10.1109/tip.2003.819861
pmid: 15376593
|
[44] |
Willis J., & Todorov A. (2006). First impressions: Making up your mind after a 100-ms exposure to a face. Psychological Science, 17(7), 592-598.
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01750.x
pmid: 16866745
|
[45] |
Xu F., Wu D. C., Toriyama R., Ma F. L., Itakura S., & Lee K. (2012). Similarities and differences in chinese and caucasian adults' use of facial cues for trustworthiness judgments. Plos One, 7(4), e34859.
|
[46] |
Young A. I., Ratner K. G., & Fazio R. H. (2014). Political attitudes bias the mental representation of a presidential candidate's face. Psychological Science, 25(2), 503-510.
doi: 10.1177/0956797613510717
pmid: 24367060
|
[47] |
Zou G. Y. (2007). Toward using confidence intervals to compare correlations. Psychological Methods, 12(4), 399-413.
doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.12.4.399
pmid: 18179351
|