心理学报 ›› 2023, Vol. 55 ›› Issue (12): 1903-1916.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2023.01903
• 研究报告 • 下一篇
收稿日期:
2022-01-14
发布日期:
2023-10-16
出版日期:
2023-12-25
通讯作者:
周吉帆, E-mail: 基金资助:
ZHU Yiming, ZHAO Yang, TANG Ning, ZHOU Jifan(), SHEN Mowei()
Received:
2022-01-14
Online:
2023-10-16
Published:
2023-12-25
摘要:
产生式理论认为, 视觉图形的识别是对其产生过程的逆推理。汉字是笔画按正字法规则交错连接构成的象形文字, 手写体汉字识别可以认为是对汉字产生过程的反向推理。基于典型的产生式模型——贝叶斯规划学习模型, 汉字的产生式识别过程从识别字的笔画开始, 先基于线段交点提取出节点, 再枚举能产生该节点的所有笔画组合方式, 从而获得汉字的产生方式。据此预测, 节点数量和节点复杂度是手写汉字识别过程的重要影响因素。本研究通过三个实验考察了节点在汉字识别中的作用。结果显示, 含有较多节点的汉字具有更好的识别绩效(节点数量效应), 掩盖由较多笔画构成的高复杂度节点会对汉字识别产生更大干扰(节点复杂度效应)。本研究增进了对汉字识别早期过程的认识, 为字形识别的产生式反向推理过程提供了证据。
中图分类号:
朱一鸣, 赵阳, 唐宁, 周吉帆, 沈模卫. (2023). 笔画节点在手写体汉字识别中的作用. 心理学报, 55(12), 1903-1916.
ZHU Yiming, ZHAO Yang, TANG Ning, ZHOU Jifan, SHEN Mowei. (2023). The role of stroke nodes in the recognition of handwritten Chinese characters. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 55(12), 1903-1916.
条件 | 第1点复杂 | 第5点复杂 | 第1点简单 | 第5点简单 |
---|---|---|---|---|
笔画数 | 10.85 | 10.70 | 10.25 | 10.30 |
字频(lg) | 4.16 | 4.35 | 4.19 | 4.31 |
部件数 | 2.45 | 2.65 | 2.55 | 2.45 |
节点数 | 5.75 | 5.75 | 5.80 | 6.00 |
整字结构 | 均包含6个上下结构合体字和14个左右结构合体字 |
表1 实验2材料的各种属性
条件 | 第1点复杂 | 第5点复杂 | 第1点简单 | 第5点简单 |
---|---|---|---|---|
笔画数 | 10.85 | 10.70 | 10.25 | 10.30 |
字频(lg) | 4.16 | 4.35 | 4.19 | 4.31 |
部件数 | 2.45 | 2.65 | 2.55 | 2.45 |
节点数 | 5.75 | 5.75 | 5.80 | 6.00 |
整字结构 | 均包含6个上下结构合体字和14个左右结构合体字 |
条件 | CIP | CJP | CIS | CJS | SIP | SJP | SIS | SJS |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
整字笔画数 | 11.27 | 11.47 | 11.27 | 10.80 | 11.67 | 11.47 | 11.00 | 11.73 |
整字频率(lg) | 2.77 | 2.93 | 2.76 | 2.69 | 2.81 | 2.68 | 2.86 | 2.92 |
形旁笔画数 | 4.87 | 4.60 | 4.60 | 4.73 | 4.60 | 4.13 | 4.13 | 4.57 |
形旁频率(lg) | 4.91 | 5.00 | 4.62 | 4.95 | 5.03 | 5.02 | 5.06 | 4.84 |
形旁结合律(lg) | 2.38 | 2.44 | 2.37 | 2.09 | 2.28 | 2.18 | 2.32 | 2.13 |
声旁笔画数 | 6.40 | 6.87 | 6.93 | 6.73 | 7.06 | 6.73 | 6.87 | 7.27 |
声旁频率(lg) | 4.90 | 4.66 | 5.03 | 4.93 | 4.67 | 4.84 | 4.93 | 4.85 |
声旁结合率(lg) | 1.98 | 1.87 | 1.95 | 1.73 | 1.68 | 1.72 | 2.00 | 1.89 |
表2 实验3材料的各种属性
条件 | CIP | CJP | CIS | CJS | SIP | SJP | SIS | SJS |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
整字笔画数 | 11.27 | 11.47 | 11.27 | 10.80 | 11.67 | 11.47 | 11.00 | 11.73 |
整字频率(lg) | 2.77 | 2.93 | 2.76 | 2.69 | 2.81 | 2.68 | 2.86 | 2.92 |
形旁笔画数 | 4.87 | 4.60 | 4.60 | 4.73 | 4.60 | 4.13 | 4.13 | 4.57 |
形旁频率(lg) | 4.91 | 5.00 | 4.62 | 4.95 | 5.03 | 5.02 | 5.06 | 4.84 |
形旁结合律(lg) | 2.38 | 2.44 | 2.37 | 2.09 | 2.28 | 2.18 | 2.32 | 2.13 |
声旁笔画数 | 6.40 | 6.87 | 6.93 | 6.73 | 7.06 | 6.73 | 6.87 | 7.27 |
声旁频率(lg) | 4.90 | 4.66 | 5.03 | 4.93 | 4.67 | 4.84 | 4.93 | 4.85 |
声旁结合率(lg) | 1.98 | 1.87 | 1.95 | 1.73 | 1.68 | 1.72 | 2.00 | 1.89 |
[1] |
Biederman, I. (1987). Recognition-by-components: a theory of human image understanding. Psychological Review, 94(2), 115-47.
doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.94.2.115 pmid: 3575582 |
[2] |
Brincat, S. L., & Connor, C. E. (2004). Underlying principles of visual shape selectivity in posterior inferotemporal cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 7(8), 880-886.
doi: 10.1038/nn1278 pmid: 15235606 |
[3] |
Cao, F., Vu, M., Chan, D. H. L., Lawrence, J. M., Harris, L. N., Guan, Q., … Perfetti, C. A. (2013). Writing affects the brain network of reading in Chinese: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Human Brain Mapping, 34(7), 1670-1684.
doi: 10.1002/hbm.22017 pmid: 22378588 |
[4] |
Cave, K. R., & Wolfe, J. M. (1990). Modeling the role of parallel processing in visual search. Cognitive Psychology, 22(2), 225-271.
pmid: 2331857 |
[5] | Changizi, M. A., Zhang, Q., Ye, H., & Shimojo, S. (2006). The structures of letters and symbols throughout human history are selected to match those found in objects in natural scenes. American Naturalist, 167(5), E117-E139. |
[6] | Chen, C. F., & Huang, X. T. (2004). Structural symmetry of Chinese character cognition: Research and application. Beijing: Xinhua Publishing House. |
[陈传锋, 黄希庭. (2004). 结构对称性汉字认知: 研究与应用. 北京: 新华出版社.] | |
[7] | Chen, T. Q. (1983). Chinese orthography. Wuhan, China: Hubei People’S Press. |
[陈天泉. (1983). 汉字正字法. 武汉: 湖北人民出版社.] | |
[8] | Chen, Y. C., & Yeh, S. L. (2017). Examining radical position and function in Chinese character recognition using the repetition blindness paradigm. Language Cognition & Neuroscience, 32(1), 37-54. |
[9] | Dehaene, S. (2009). Reading in the brain. New York, America: Penguin Press. |
[10] | Dehaene, S., Duhamel, J.-R., Hauser, M. D., & Rizzolatti, G. (2005). From monkey brain to human brain. Cambridge, America: MIT Press. |
[11] |
Gershman, J., Vul, E., & Tenenbaum, B. (2012). Multistability and perceptual inference. Neural Computation, 24(1), 1-24.
doi: 10.1162/NECO_a_00226 pmid: 22023198 |
[12] |
Giovanni, F. B. D. (1994). Order of strokes writing as a cue for retrieval in reading Chinese characters. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 6(4), 337-355.
doi: 10.1080/09541449408406519 URL |
[13] | Grenander, U. (1976). Pattern synthesis: Lectures in pattern theory I. New York, Springer-Verlag. |
[14] | Han, B. X. (1998). The frequency effect of character constituents in the recognition of Chinese characters. Journal of Psychological Science, 21(3), 193-195+286. |
[韩布新. (1998). 汉字识别中部件的频率效应. 心理科学, 21(3), 193-195+286.] | |
[15] | Hou, W. K., & Qin, Q. G. (1996). A preliminary study of the implicit learning of Chinese characters. Journal of Psychological Science, 19(6), 351-354+384. |
[侯伟康, 奏启庚. (1996). 汉字特征内隐学习的初步实验研究. 心理科学, 19(6), 351-354+384.] | |
[16] | Huang, J. T., & Wang, M. Y. (1992). From unit to gestalt: perceptual dynamics in recognizing Chinese characters. Advances in Psychology, 90, 3-35. |
[17] |
Kok, P., Brouwer, G., van Gerven, M., & de Lange, F. (2013). Prior expectations bias sensory representations in visual cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 33(41), 16275-16284.
doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0742-13.2013 pmid: 24107959 |
[18] | Krizhevsky, A., Sutskever, I., & Hinton, G. (2012). Imagenet classification with deep convolutional neural networks. Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems, 25(2), 1097-1105. |
[19] |
Laeng, B., Bloem, I. M., D’Ascenzo, S., & Tommasi, L. (2014). Scrutinizing visual images: The role of gaze in mental imagery and memory. Cognition, 131(2), 263-283.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.01.003 pmid: 24561190 |
[20] |
Lake, B. M., Salakhutdinov, R., & Tenenbaum, J. B. (2015). Human-level concept learning through probabilistic program induction. Science, 350(6266), 1332-1338.
doi: 10.1126/science.aab3050 pmid: 26659050 |
[21] |
Lanthier, S. N., Risko, E. F., Stolz, J. A., & Besner, D. (2009). Not all visual features are created equal: Early processing in letter and word recognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16(1), 67-73.
doi: 10.3758/PBR.16.1.67 URL |
[22] |
Lee, C. Y., Tsai, J. L., Huang, H. W., Hung, D. L., & Tzeng, O. J. L. (2006). The temporal signatures of semantic and phonological activations for Chinese sublexical processing: An event-related potential study. Brain Research, 1121(1), 150-159.
doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.08.117 URL |
[23] |
Li, Z., Wu, Q., Xiao, Y., Jin, M., & Lu, H. (2020). Deep matching network for handwritten Chinese character recognition. Pattern Recognition, 107, 107471.
doi: 10.1016/j.patcog.2020.107471 URL |
[24] | Lockhead, G. R., & Pomerantz, J. R. (1991). The perception of structure. Washington, DC, America: American psychological Association. |
[25] | Luo, F. F. (2020). Potent features for identification of Chinese character strokes (Unpublished master’s thesis). Hangzhou Normal University, China. |
[骆非凡. (2020). 汉字笔画认知的关键性特征 (硕士学位论文). 杭州师范大学.] | |
[26] | Luo, Y. L., Chen, M., & Peng, D. L. (2008). The influence of the whole words、radicals and strokes in stroke-form- character. Psychological Exploration, 28(1), 28-32. |
[罗艳琳, 陈墨, 彭聃龄. (2008). 整字、部件与笔画对笔画构成汉字的影响. 心理学探新, 28(1), 28-32.] | |
[27] | Meila, M. (2006). Machine learning: Discriminative and generative. The Mathematical Intelligencer, 28(1), 67-69. |
[28] | Peng, D. L., & Wang, C. M. (1997). Basic processing unit of Chinese character recognition: Evidence from stroke number effect and radical number effect. Acta Psychological Sinica, 29(1), 9-17. |
[彭聃龄, 王春茂. (1997). 汉字加工的基本单元: 来自笔画数效应和部件数效应的证据. 心理学报, 29(1), 9-17.] | |
[29] |
Petit, J. P., & Grainger, J. (2002). Masked partial priming of letter perception. Visual Cognition, 9(3), 337-353.
doi: 10.1080/13506280042000207 URL |
[30] | Qian, Y., Zhang, W. Y., & Bi, H. Y. (2015). The effect of semantic-radical family in Chinese character recognition. Chinese Journal of Ergonomics, 21(3), 25-30. |
[钱怡, 张逸玮, 毕鸿燕. (2015). 汉字识别中的形旁家族效应. 人类工效学, 21(3), 25-30. ] | |
[31] |
Ren, X. Q., Fang, X., Sui, X., & Wu, Y. (2018). Characteristics and neural mechanisms of handwritten character recognition. Advances in Psychological Science, 26(7), 1174-1185.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2018.01174 |
[任晓倩, 方娴, 隋雪, 吴岩. (2018). 手写体文字识别的特点及神经机制. 心理科学进展, 26(7), 1174-1185.]
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2018.01174 |
|
[32] | Shen, M. W., Pan, S. H., & Chen, X. (1998). A study on the cognitive process of identification of two-radical Chinese characters. Chinese Journal of Applied Psychology, 4(1), 27-32. |
[沈模卫, 潘善会, 陈新. (1998). 合体汉字字形识别过程探索. 应用心理学, 4(1), 27-32. ] | |
[33] | Shen, M. W., Pan, S. H., & Li, Z. P. (1997). The effects of global perception of Chinese character morphology on recognition of its radical. Chinese Journal of Applied Psychology, 3(1), 47-51. |
[沈模卫, 潘善会, 李忠平. (1997). 整体字形对部件识别的影响. 应用心理学, 3(1), 47-51. ] | |
[34] | Shen, M. W., & Zhu, Z. X. (1997). Experimental tests of the hierarchical model of two-radical Chinese character perception. Acta Psychological Sinica, 29(4), 350-356. |
[沈模卫, 朱祖祥. (1997). 对汉字字形识别层次模型的实验验证. 心理学报, 29(4), 350-356.] | |
[35] |
Su, I., & Weekes, B. (2007). Effects of frequency and semantic radical combinability on reading in Chinese: An ERP study. Brain and Language, 103(1-2), 111-112.
doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2007.07.069 URL |
[36] |
Szwed, M., Dehaene, S., Kleinschmidt, A., Eger, E., Valabrègue, R., Amadon, A., & Cohen, L. (2011). Specialization for written words over objects in the visual cortex. Neuroimage, 56(1), 330-344.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.01.073 pmid: 21296170 |
[37] | Taft, M., & Zhu, X. (1997). Submorphemic processing in reading Chinese. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 23(3), 761-775. |
[38] |
Tan, L. H., Spinks, J. A., Eden, G. F., Perfetti, C. A., & Siok, W. T. (2005). Reading depends on writing, in Chinese. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 102(24), 8781-8785.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.0503523102 pmid: 15939871 |
[39] | Tang, N., An, W., Xu, H. K., Zhou, J. F., Gao, T., & Shen, M. W. (2018). From “data” to “Representation”: How human cognition can inspire artificial intelligence. Chinese Journal of Applied Psychology, 24(1), 3-14 |
[唐宁, 安玮, 徐昊骙, 周吉帆, 高涛, 沈模卫. (2018). 从数据到表征: 人类认知对人工智能的启发. 应用心理学, 24(1), 3-14. ] | |
[40] | Treisman, A. M. (1982). Perceptual grouping and attention in visual search for features and for objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 8(2), 194-214. |
[41] |
Treisman, A. M., & Gelade, G. (1980). A feature-integration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology, 12(1), 97-136.
doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(80)90005-5 pmid: 7351125 |
[42] | Tse, P. U., & Cavanagh, P. (2000). Chinese and Americans see opposite apparent motions in a Chinese character. Cognition, 74(3), B27-B32. |
[43] | Wang, F. (2015). The implict learning research of Chinese words character and form-meaning rules for learning difficulty students (Unpublished master’s thesis). Zhengzhou University, China. |
[王菲. (2015). 学困生汉语字词特征及形义规则的内隐学习研究 (硕士学位论文). 郑州大学.] | |
[44] | Yan, G. L., Chi, H., Bian, Q., Xu, Z. J., & Cui, L. (2013). Effects of phonetic radicals and pattern of stroke removal on identification of Chinese phonograms. Journal of Psychological Science, 36(1), 14-19. |
[闫国利, 迟慧, 卞迁, 徐子珺, 崔磊. (2013). 声旁与笔画省略方式对形声字识别的影响. 心理科学, 36(1), 14-19.] | |
[45] |
Yu, L., Xiong, J., Zhang, Q., Drieghe, D., & Reichle, E. D. (2018). Eye-movement evidence For the mental representation of strokes in Chinese characters. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 45(3), 544-551.
doi: 10.1037/xlm0000598 URL |
[46] |
Yuille, A., & Kersten, D. (2006). Vision as Bayesian inference: Analysis by synthesis? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(7), 301-308.
pmid: 16784882 |
[47] |
Zhang, J., Du, J., & Dai, L. (2020). Radical analysis network for learning hierarchies of Chinese characters. Pattern Recognition, 103, 107305.
doi: 10.1016/j.patcog.2020.107305 URL |
[48] |
Zhang, J. J., & Jiang, M. M. (2008). The effects of the radical family size and high frequency phonetic radical family member on phonogram recognition. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 40(9), 947-960.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2008.00947 URL |
[张积家, 姜敏敏. (2008). 形旁家族、声旁家族和高频同声旁字对形声字识别的影响. 心理学报, 40(9), 947-960.] | |
[49] | Zhang, J. J., Wang, H. P., Zhang, M., & Zhang, H. C. (2002). The effect of the complexity and repetition of the strokes on the cognition of the strokes and the Chinese characters. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 34(5), 7-11. |
[张积家, 王惠萍, 张萌, 张厚粲. (2002). 笔画复杂性和重复性对笔画和汉字认知的影响. 心理学报, 34(5), 7-11.] | |
[50] | Zhang, R. (2017). The role of hierarchical organization of Chinese character components in vocabulary recognition (Unpublished master’s thesis). Shaanxi Normal University, China. |
[张瑞. (2017). 汉字部件的层级组织在词汇识别中的作用 (硕士学位论文). 陕西师范大学.] | |
[51] | Zhang, W. T., & Feng, L. (1992). A study on the unit of processing in recognition of Chinese characters. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 24(4), 45-51. |
[张武田, 冯玲. (1992). 关于汉字识别加工单位的研究. 心理学报, 24(4), 45-51.] | |
[52] | Zheng, C. M., & Gao, S. R. (1982). Psychological research on Chinese language. Taipei, China: Wenhe Publishing Co., Ltd. |
[郑昭明, 高尚仁. (1982). 中国语文的心理学研究. 台北: 文鹤出版有限公司.] | |
[53] | Zhou, J. F., Xu, H. K., Tang, N., Shi, B. H., Zhao, Y., Gao, T., & Shen, M. W. (2016). The “strong-cognition” psychology research: Implications of AlphaGo. Chinese Journal of Applied Psychology, 22(1), 3-11. |
[周吉帆, 徐昊骙, 唐宁, 史博皓, 赵阳, 高涛, 沈模卫. (2016). "强认知"的心理学研究: 来自alphago的启示. 应用心理学, 22(1), 3-11.] | |
[54] |
Zhu, Z. X., Liu, L., Cui, L., & Peng, D. L. (2019). The influence of writing on reading: Evidence from the contrast between traditional writing and typing. Advances in Psychological Science, 27(5), 796-803.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2019.00796 |
[朱朝霞, 刘丽, 崔磊, 彭聃龄. (2019). 书写对阅读的影响——来自传统书写与电脑打字的证据. 心理科学进展, 27(5), 796-803.]
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2019.00796 |
[1] | 赵雪汝, 李婷, 李金惠, 何先友, 张维, 陈广耀. 动态性对简笔画动物审美的影响及其神经机制[J]. 心理学报, 2021, 53(6): 575-586. |
[2] | 肖涵敏,杜文久,张婷婷. 基于项目节点的多级评分模型的统一[J]. 心理学报, 2011, 43(12): 1462-1467. |
[3] | 杨双,宁宁,刘翔平,潘益中,卢佳. 听写困难儿童在笔画加工中的整体干扰效应[J]. 心理学报, 2009, 41(02): 127-134. |
[4] | 王永丽,时勘. 文化价值取向与自我调节点对反馈效果的调节作用[J]. 心理学报, 2004, 36(06): 738-743. |
[5] | 王惠萍,张积家,张厚粲. 汉字整体和笔画频率对笔画认知的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2003, 35(01): 17-22. |
[6] | 张积家,王惠萍,张萌,张厚粲. 笔画复杂性和重复性对笔画和汉字认知的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2002, 34(05): 7-11. |
[7] | 曾捷英,周新林,喻柏林. 变形汉字的结构方式和笔画数效应[J]. 心理学报, 2001, 33(3): 13-17. |
[8] | 陈传锋,黄希庭. 结构对称性汉字视觉识别特点的实验研究[J]. 心理学报, 1999, 31(2): 154-161. |
[9] | 彭聃龄,王春茂. 汉字加工的基本单元:来自笔画数效应和部件数效应的证据[J]. 心理学报, 1997, 29(1): 9-17. |
[10] | 喻柏林,张蜀林,潘玉进. 笔画类型对直立和倾斜汉字识别的影响[J]. 心理学报, 1997, 29(1): 24-29. |
[11] | 喻柏林,曹河圻. 汉字识别中的笔画数效应新探——兼论字频效应[J]. 心理学报, 1992, 24(2): 10-16. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||