[1] |
Barsalou L. W., Santos A., Simmons W. K., & Wilson C. D . (2008). Language and simulation in conceptual processing. In M. de Vega, A. M. Glenberg, & A. C. Graesser (Eds.), Symbols, embodiment, and meaning (pp. 245-283). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
|
[2] |
Binder J. R., Westbury C. F., Mckiernan K. A., Possing E. T., & Medler D. A . (2005). Distinct brain systems for processing concrete and abstract concepts. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(6), 905-917.
|
[3] |
Borghi A. M., Binkofski F., Castelfranchi C., Cimatti F., Scorolli C., & Tummolini L . (2017). The challenge of abstract concepts. Psychological Bulletin, 143(3), 263-292.
|
[4] |
Cohen J . (2013). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Routledge.
|
[5] |
Dove G . (2009). Beyond perceptual symbols: A call for representational pluralism. Cognition, 110(3), 412-431.
|
[6] |
Dove G . (2010). On the need for embodied and dis-embodied cognition. Frontiers in Psychology, 1(6), 242.
|
[7] |
Estes Z., Verges M., & Barsalou L. W . (2008). Head up, foot down: Object words orient attention to the objects' typical location. Psychological Science, 19(2), 93-97.
|
[8] |
Folstein J. R., & van Petten C . (2008). Influence of cognitive control and mismatch on the N2 component of the ERP: A review. Psychophysiology, 45(1), 152-170.
|
[9] |
Gullick M. M., Mitra P., & Coch D . (2013). Imagining the truth and the moon: An electrophysiological study of abstract and concrete word processing. Psychophysiology, 50(5), 431-440.
|
[10] |
Hirschfeld G., Zwitserlood P., & Dobel C . (2011). Effects of language comprehension on visual processing - MEG dissociates early perceptual and late N400 effects. Brain and Language, 116(2), 91-96.
|
[11] |
Holcomb P. J., Kounios J., Anderson J. E., & West W. C . (1999). Dual-coding, context-availability, and concreteness effects in sentence comprehension: An electrophysiological investigation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25(3), 721-742.
|
[12] |
Hubbard T. M., Magne C., & Langston W . (2014). Spatial configuration of vertically related word pairs modulates the N400 component. Neuroreport, 25(18), 1424-1428.
|
[13] |
Kutas M., & Federmeier K. D . (2011). Thirty years and counting: Finding meaning in the N400 component of the event-related brain potential (ERP). Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 621-647.
|
[14] |
Lakens D . (2012). Polarity correspondence in metaphor congruency effects: Structural overlap predicts categorization times for bipolar concepts presented in vertical space. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38(3), 726-736.
|
[15] |
Lakoff G., & Johnson M . (1980). Metaphors we live by. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
|
[16] |
Lakoff G., & Johnson M . (1999). Philosophy in the flesh: The embodied mind and its challenge to western thought. New York: Basic Books.
|
[17] |
Laszlo S., & Federmeier K. D . (2011). The N400 as a snapshot of interactive processing: Evidence from regression analyses of orthographic neighbor and lexical associate effects. Psychophysiology, 48(2), 176-186.
|
[18] |
Louwerse M. M . (2008). Embodied relations are encoded in language. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15(4), 838-844.
|
[19] |
Louwerse M. M., & Connell L . (2011). A taste of words: Linguistic context and perceptual simulation predict the modality of words. Cognitive Science, 35(2), 381-398.
|
[20] |
Louwerse M. M., & Hutchinson S . (2012). Neurological evidence linguistic processes precede perceptual simulation in conceptual processing. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 385.
|
[21] |
Louwerse M. M., & Jeuniaux P . (2010). The linguistic and embodied nature of conceptual processing. Cognition, 114(1), 96-104.
|
[22] |
Louwerse M. M., He X. Y., Deng Y. M., Zhao X. R., & Xue Y . (2017). Symbol interdependency in language processing: Interactions between language statistics and perceptual simulation. Journal of South China Normal University (Social Science Edition), (2), 51-60.
|
|
[ Louwerse, M. M., 何先友, 邓玉梅, 赵雪汝, 薛颖 . (2017). 语言加工中的符号相互依存: 语言统计和知觉模拟的交互作用. 华南师范大学学报: 社会科学版(2), 51-60.]
|
[23] |
Lund K., & Burgess C . (1996). Producing high-dimensional semantic spaces from lexical co-occurrence. Behavior Research Methods Instruments & Computers, 28(2), 203-208.
|
[24] |
Malhi S. K., & Buchanan L . (2018). A test of the symbol interdependency hypothesis with both concrete and abstract stimuli. Plos One, 13(3), e0192719.
|
[25] |
Markman A. B., & Dietrich E . (2000). Extending the classical view of representation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(12), 470-475.
|
[26] |
Meier B. P., Hauser D. J., Robinson M. D., Friesen C. K., & Schjeldahl K . (2007). What's "up" with God? Vertical space as a representation of the divine. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 93(5), 699-710.
|
[27] |
Patel S. H., & Azzam P. N . (2005). Characterization of N200 and P300: Selected studies of the event-related potential. International Journal of Medical Sciences, 2(4), 147-154.
|
[28] |
Schubert T. W . (2005). Your highness: Vertical positions as perceptual symbols of power. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89(1), 1-21.
|
[29] |
Song Y. Q., & Zhang J. J . (2014). Temporal-spatial metaphor in conceptual representation: Can spatial information be activated when processing the temporal reference which implied in the changing shape of objects? Acta Psychologica Sinica, 46(2), 216-226.
|
|
[ 宋宜琪, 张积家 . (2014). 空间隐喻和形状变化对物体内隐时间概念加工的影响. 心理学报, 46(2), 216-226.]
|
[30] |
Stefanucci J. K., & Storbeck J . (2009). Don't look down: Emotional arousal elevates height perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 138(1), 131-145.
|
[31] |
Tse C. S., Kurby C. A., & Du F . (2010). Perceptual simulations and linguistic representations have differential effects on speeded relatedness judgements and recognition memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63(5), 928-941.
|
[32] |
Wang J., Conder J. A., Blitzer D. N., & Shinkareva S. V . (2010). Neural representation of abstract and concrete concepts: A meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Human Brain Mapping, 31(10), 1459-1468.
|
[33] |
Wang, R. M, Mo, L., Wang R. M., Mo L., Li L., Wang S. P., & Wu J . (2005). Perceptual symbol representation and propositional symbol representation in language comprehension. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 37(2), 143-150.
|
|
[ 王瑞明, 莫雷, 李利, 王穗苹, 吴俊 . (2005). 言语理解中的知觉符号表征与命题符号表征. 心理学报, 37(2), 143-150.]
|
[34] |
Wang X. X., Jiang S., & Zhang J. J . (2018). Effect of the spatial linguistic symbol on the container metaphor of seniority rules. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 50(09), 953-964.
|
|
[ 汪新筱, 江珊, 张积家 . (2018). 空间语言标记影响亲属关系的容器隐喻. 心理学报, 50(09), 953-964.]
|
[35] |
West W. C., & Holcomb P. J . (2000). Imaginal, semantic, and surface-level processing of concrete and abstract words: An electrophysiological investigation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(6), 1024-1037.
|
[36] |
Zwaan R. A., & Yaxley R. H . (2003). Spatial iconicity affects semantic relatedness judgments. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10(4), 954-958.
|