心理学报 ›› 2018, Vol. 50 ›› Issue (6): 622-636.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2018.00622
收稿日期:
2016-11-28
发布日期:
2018-04-28
出版日期:
2018-06-01
通讯作者:
汪亚珉
E-mail:wangym@cnu.edu.cn
基金资助:
YUAN Xiaojun, CUI Xiaoxia, CAO Zhengcao, KAN Hong, WANG Xiao, WANG Yamin()
Received:
2016-11-28
Online:
2018-04-28
Published:
2018-06-01
Contact:
WANG Yamin
E-mail:wangym@cnu.edu.cn
摘要:
自?hman,
中图分类号:
袁小钧, 崔晓霞, 曹正操, 阚红, 王晓, 汪亚珉. (2018). 虚拟仿真场景中威胁性视觉刺激搜索的注意偏向效应 *. 心理学报, 50(6), 622-636.
YUAN Xiaojun, CUI Xiaoxia, CAO Zhengcao, KAN Hong, WANG Xiao, WANG Yamin. (2018). Attentional bias towards threatening visual stimuli in a virtual reality-based visual search task. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 50(6), 622-636.
因子 | 丛林搜索场景 | Tomb Raider | Half Life |
---|---|---|---|
存在感 | 3.83 | 3.00 | 3.93 |
空间存在感 | 3.83 | 3.06 | 3.99 |
沉浸感 | 3.31 | 2.40 | 3.26 |
真实感体验 | 3.04 | 1.92 | 2.34 |
表1 丛林搜索场景与国外虚拟现实游戏场景IGROUP评定结果比较
因子 | 丛林搜索场景 | Tomb Raider | Half Life |
---|---|---|---|
存在感 | 3.83 | 3.00 | 3.93 |
空间存在感 | 3.83 | 3.06 | 3.99 |
沉浸感 | 3.31 | 2.40 | 3.26 |
真实感体验 | 3.04 | 1.92 | 2.34 |
位置 | 平均数(ms) | 标准误 |
---|---|---|
位置5 | 5306.80 | 122.41 |
位置6 | 5997.55 | 138.11 |
位置7 | 6912.65 | 119.33 |
位置15 | 4935.95 | 109.64 |
位置16 | 5983.80 | 101.94 |
位置17 | 6612.70 | 141.75 |
表2 不同位置反应时最小值(平均数、标准误)
位置 | 平均数(ms) | 标准误 |
---|---|---|
位置5 | 5306.80 | 122.41 |
位置6 | 5997.55 | 138.11 |
位置7 | 6912.65 | 119.33 |
位置15 | 4935.95 | 109.64 |
位置16 | 5983.80 | 101.94 |
位置17 | 6612.70 | 141.75 |
实验 | 蛇 | 蜘蛛 | 花 | 蘑菇 | 松鼠 | 蝉 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
实验1 | 0.605 (0.002) | 0.606 (0.002) | 0.605 (0.002) | 0.602 (0.002) | ||
实验2 | 0.605 (0.002) | 0.606 (0.002) | 0.611 (0.002) | 0.605 (0.002) |
表3 图片物理显著性的明度值平均数(括号内为标准误)
实验 | 蛇 | 蜘蛛 | 花 | 蘑菇 | 松鼠 | 蝉 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
实验1 | 0.605 (0.002) | 0.606 (0.002) | 0.605 (0.002) | 0.602 (0.002) | ||
实验2 | 0.605 (0.002) | 0.606 (0.002) | 0.611 (0.002) | 0.605 (0.002) |
[1] | Buss, D. M . ( 1999). Evolutionary psychology: The new science of the mind. Boston: Allyn and Bacon. |
[2] |
Cisler J. M., Bacon A. K., & Williams N. L . ( 2009). Phenomenological characteristics of attentional biases towards threat: A critical review. Cognitive Therapy & Research, 33(2), 221-234.
doi: 10.1007/s10608-007-9161-y URL pmid: 2901130 |
[3] |
Cisler J. M., & Koster E. H. W . ( 2010). Mechanisms of attentional biases towards threat in anxiety disorders: An integrative review. Clinical Psychology Review, 30(2), 203-216.
doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.003 URL pmid: 2814889 |
[4] |
Dudeney J., Sharpe L., & Hunt C . ( 2015). Attentional bias towards threatening stimuli in children with anxiety: A meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 40, 66-75.
doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2015.05.007 URL pmid: 26071667 |
[5] |
Felnhofer A., Kothgassner O. D., Schmidt M., Heinzle A. K., Beutl L., Hlavacs H., & Kryspin-Exner I . ( 2015). Is virtual reality emotionally arousing? Investigating five emotion inducing virtual park scenarios. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 82, 48-56.
doi: 10.1016/j.ijhcs.2015.05.004 URL |
[6] |
Gibson, J. J . ( 1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.
doi: 10.1080/00071773.1982.11007570 URL |
[7] |
Hansen C. H., & Hansen R. D . ( 1988). Finding the face in the crowd: An anger superiority effect. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 54(6), 917-924.
doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.917 URL pmid: 3397866 |
[8] |
Itti L., & Koch C . ( 2000). A saliency-based search mechanism for overt and covert shifts of visual attention. Vision Research, 40(10-12), 1489-1506.
doi: 10.1002/jid.1310 URL pmid: 1078865422 |
[9] |
Janczyk M., Augst S., & Kunde W . ( 2014). The locus of the emotional Stroop effect: A study with the PRP paradigm. Acta Psychologica, 151, 8-15.
doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.05.011 URL pmid: 24904999 |
[10] |
Juth P., Lundqvist D., Karlsson A., & Öhman A . ( 2005). Looking for foes and friends: Perceptual and emotional factors when finding a face in the crowd. Emotion, 5(4), 379-395.
doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.5.4.379 URL pmid: 16366743 |
[11] | Ledoux, J. E . ( 2000). Emotion circuits in the brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 23, 155-184. |
[12] | Lee D. N., & Kalmus H . ( 1980). The optic flow field: The foundation of vision [and discussion]. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 290(1038), 169-179. |
[13] | Lindsay P. H. , & Norman, D. A.( 1977) . Human information processing: An introduction to psychology New York: Academic Press An introduction to psychology. New York: Academic Press. |
[14] |
LoBue, V. ( 2010). And along came a spider: An attentional bias for the detection of spiders in young children and adults. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 107(1), 59-66.
doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2010.04.005 URL pmid: 20529694 |
[15] |
LoBue V., & Rakison D. H . ( 2013). What we fear most: A developmental advantage for threat-relevant stimuli. Developmental Review, 33(4), 285-303.
doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2013.07.005 URL |
[16] |
Matsumoto, E. ( 2010). Bias in attending to emotional facial expressions: Anxiety and visual search efficiency. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 24(3), 414-424.
doi: 10.1002/acp.1686 URL |
[17] |
Mogg K., & Bradley B. P . ( 2006). Time course of attentional bias for fear-relevant pictures in spider-fearful individuals. Behaviour Research & Therapy, 44(9), 1241-1250.
doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2006.05.003 URL pmid: 16870133 |
[18] |
Mogg K., Bradley B. P., & Hallowell N . ( 1994). Attentional bias to threat: Roles of trait anxiety, stressful events, and awareness. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A: Human Experimental Psychology, 47(4), 841-864.
doi: 10.1080/14640749408401099 URL pmid: 7809399 |
[19] | Öhman, A. ( 2009). Of snakes and faces: An evolutionary perspective on the psychology of fear. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 50(6), 543-552.Öhman, A., Flykt, A., & Esteves, F. (2001). Emotion drives attention: Detecting the snake in the grass. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130(3), 466-478. |
[20] | Öhman A., Lundqvist D., & Esteves F . ( 2001). The face in the crowd revisited: A threat advantage with schematic stimuli. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 80(3), 381-396. |
[21] | Öhman A., & Mineka S . ( 2001). Fears, phobias, and preparedness: Toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning. Psychological Review, 108, 483-522. |
[22] |
Öhman A., Soares S. C., Juth P., Lindström B., & Esteves F . ( 2012). Evolutionary derived modulations of attention to two common fear stimuli: Serpents and hostile humans. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 24(1), 17-32.
doi: 10.1080/20445911.2011.629603 URL |
[23] |
Peira N., Golkar A., Öhman A., Anders S., & Wiens S . ( 2012). Emotional responses in spider fear are closely related to picture awareness. Cognition and Emotion, 26(2), 252-260.
doi: 10.1080/02699931.2011.579087 URL pmid: 21973031 |
[24] | Peng D. L. ( 2012). General psychology (4th ed.) . Beijing, China: Beijing Normal University Press. |
[ 彭聃龄 . ( 2012). 普通心理学 (第4版). 北京: 北京师范大学出版社.] | |
[25] |
Savage R. A., Lipp O. V., Craig B. M., Becker S. I., & Horstmann G . ( 2013). In search of the emotional face: Anger versus happiness superiority in visual search. Emotion, 13(4), 758-768.
doi: 10.1037/a0031970 URL pmid: 23527503 |
[26] |
Schubert T., Friedmann F., & Regenbrecht H . ( 2001). The experience of presence: Factor analytic insights. Presence, 10(3), 266-281.
doi: 10.1162/105474601300343603 URL |
[27] |
Soares, S. C . ( 2012). The lurking snake in the grass: Interference of snake stimuli in visually taxing conditions. Evolutionary Psychology, 10(2), 187-197.
doi: 10.1177/147470491201000202 URL pmid: 22947633 |
[28] |
Soares S. C., Esteves F., Lundqvist D., & Öhman A . ( 2009). Some animal specific fears are more specific than others: Evidence from attention and emotion measures. Behaviour Research & Therapy, 47(12), 1032-1042.
doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2009.07.022 URL pmid: 19695561 |
[29] |
Soares S. C., Lindström B., Esteves F., & Öhman A . ( 2014). The hidden snake in the grass: Superior detection of snakes in challenging attentional conditions. PLoS One, 9(12), e114724.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0114724 URL pmid: 4262429 |
[30] |
Stormark K. M., & Hugdahl K . ( 1996). Peripheral cuing of covert spatial attention before and after emotional conditioning of the cue. International Journal of Neuroscience, 86(3-4), 225-240.
doi: 10.3109/00207459608986713 URL pmid: 8884393 |
[31] |
Stormark K. M., Nordby H., & Hugdahl K . ( 1995). Attentional shifts to emotionally charged cues: Behavioural and ERP data. Cognition & Emotion, 9(5), 507-523.
doi: 10.1080/02699939508408978 URL |
[32] |
Sun H. J., Carey D. P., & Goodale M. A . ( 1992). A mammalian model of optic-flow utilization in the control of locomotion. Experimental Brain Research, 91(1), 171-175.
doi: 10.1007/BF00230026 URL pmid: 1301371 |
[33] |
Theeuwes, J. ( 2010). Top-down and bottom-up control of visual selection. Acta Psychologica, 135(2), 77-99.
doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.02.006 URL pmid: 20507828 |
[34] |
Thrasher C., & LoBue V . ( 2016). Do infants find snakes aversive? Infants' physiological responses to "fear-relevant" stimuli. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 142, 382-389.
doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2015.09.013 URL pmid: 26483161 |
[35] |
Tooby J., & Cosmides L . ( 1990). The past explains the present: Emotional adaptations and the structure of ancestral environments. Ethology & Sociobiology, 11(4-5), 375-424.
doi: 10.1016/0162-3095(90)90017-Z URL |
[36] | Torralba A., Oliva A., Castelhano M. S., & Henderson J. M . ( 2006). Contextual guidance of eye movements and attention in real-world scenes: The role of global features in object search. Psychological Review, 113(4), 766-786. |
[37] |
Tsotsos J. K., Culhane S. M., Kei W. W. Y., Lai Y. Z., Davis N., & Nuflo F . ( 1995). Modeling visual attention via selective tuning. Artificial Intelligence, 78(1-2), 507-545.
doi: 10.1016/0004-3702(95)00025-9 URL |
[38] |
Urech A., Krieger T., Chesham A., Mast F. W., & Berger T . ( 2015). Virtual reality-based attention bias modification training for social anxiety: A feasibility and proof of concept study. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 6, 154.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2015.00154 URL pmid: 4623392 |
[39] |
van Le Q., Isbell L. A., Matsumoto J., Nguyen M., Hori E., Maior R. S., … Nishijo H . ( 2013). Pulvinar neurons reveal neurobiological evidence of past selection for rapid detection of snakes. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 110(47), 19000-19005.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1312648110 URL pmid: 24167268 |
[40] |
Vuilleumier P., Armony J. L., Driver J., & Dolan R. J . ( 2003). Distinct spatial frequency sensitivities for processing faces and emotional expressions. Nature Neuroscience, 6(6), 624-631.
doi: 10.1016/S0021-9924(02)00092-8 URL pmid: 12740580 |
[41] | Wang F. X., Li W. J., Yan Z. Q., Duan Z. H., & Li H . ( 2015). Children's attention detection to snakes: Evidence from eye movements. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 47(6), 774-786. |
[ 王福兴, 李文静, 颜志强, 段朝辉, 李卉 . ( 2015). 幼儿对威胁性刺激蛇的注意觉察: 来自眼动证据. 心理学报, 47(6), 774-786.] | |
[42] |
Yiend, J. ( 2010). The effects of emotion on attention: A review of attentional processing of emotional information. Cognition & Emotion, 24(1), 3-47.
doi: 10.1080/02699930903205698 URL |
[43] | Zhang Y., Luo Y., Zhao S. Y., Chen W., & Li H . ( 2014). Attentional bias towards threat: Facilitated attentional orienting or impaired attentional disengagement. Advances in Psychological Science, 22(7), 1129-1138. |
[ 张禹, 罗禹, 赵守盈, 陈维, 李红 . ( 2014). 对威胁刺激的注意偏向: 注意定向加速还是注意解除困难?. 心理科学进展, 22(7), 1129-1138.]
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2014.01129 URL |
[1] | 尹华站, 张丽, 刘鹏玉, 李丹. 负性情绪的动机维度对时距知觉的影响:注意控制和注意偏向的中介作用[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(12): 1917-1931. |
[2] | 胡晓斐, 汪嘉维, 刘涵宇, 宋晓蕾. 听觉刺激对虚拟环境中空间压缩的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(1): 1-8. |
[3] | 彭宇彬, 宛小昂. 视觉搜索中风味引发对关联颜色的注意偏向[J]. 心理学报, 2022, 54(7): 736-747. |
[4] | 周子暖, 陈颜璋, 傅世敏. 预期对注意的影响受制于被预期主体是目标还是分心物[J]. 心理学报, 2022, 54(3): 221-235. |
[5] | 陈晓宇, 杜媛媛, 刘强. 积极情绪提高背景线索学习的适应性[J]. 心理学报, 2022, 54(12): 1481-1490. |
[6] | 唐晓雨, 崔鑫忠, 高敏, 袁梦莹. 视听时、空一致性对Pip-and-Pop效应的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2022, 54(11): 1310-1324. |
[7] | 张妮, 刘文, 刘方, 郭鑫. 8~12岁儿童抑郁与认知重评的关系:悲伤面孔注意偏向的中介作用[J]. 心理学报, 2022, 54(1): 25-39. |
[8] | 侯娟, 朱英格, 方晓义. 手机成瘾与抑郁:社交焦虑和负性情绪信息注意偏向的多重中介作用[J]. 心理学报, 2021, 53(4): 362-373. |
[9] | 柯晓晓, 齐惠紫, 梁家辉, 金欣园, 高婕, 张明霞, 汪亚珉. 中国人整体性思维特征的情境评估法及其应用[J]. 心理学报, 2021, 53(12): 1299-1309. |
[10] | 华艳, 李明霞, 王巧婷, 冯彩霞, 张晶. 左侧眶额皮层在自动情绪调节下注意选择中的作用:来自经颅直流电刺激的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(9): 1048-1056. |
[11] | 雷怡, 夏琦, 莫志凤, 李红. 面孔可爱度和客观熟悉度对婴儿面孔注意偏向效应的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(7): 811-822. |
[12] | 任志洪, 赵子仪, 余香莲, 赵春晓, 张琳, 林羽中, 张微. 睾酮素与反社会倾向未成年犯的攻击行为:敌意注意偏向的中介和皮质醇的调节作用[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(11): 1288-1300. |
[13] | 刘静远, 李虹. 状态焦虑对时距知觉的影响:认知评价和注意偏向有调节的中介作用[J]. 心理学报, 2019, 51(7): 747-758. |
[14] | 孙俊才, 寻凤娇, 刘萍, 张文海. 高善良特质在情绪调节行动控制中的内隐优势[J]. 心理学报, 2019, 51(7): 781-794. |
[15] | 过继成思, 黄建平, 宛小昂. 目标预知对路径整合的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2019, 51(2): 188-195. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||