1 |
Arnold A. E. G. F., Burles F., Bray S., Levy R. M., & Iaria G . ( 2014). Differential neural network configuration during human path integration. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 263: doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00263.
|
2 |
Cheng K., Shettleworth S. J., Huttenlocher J., & Rieser J . ( 2007). Bayesian integration of spatial information. Psychological Bulletin, 133( 4), 625-637.
doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.133.4.625
URL
|
3 |
Chrastil E. R., Sherrill K. R., Hasselmo M. E., & Stern C. E . ( 2016). Which way and how far? Tracking of translation and rotation information for human path integration. Human Brain Mapping, 37( 10),3636-3655.
doi: 10.1002/hbm.23265
URL
|
4 |
Collett, T.S., & Graham P. ( 2004). Animal navigation: Path integration, visual landmarks and cognitive maps. Current Biology, 14( 12), R475-R477.
doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2004.06.013
URL
|
5 |
Foo P., Duchon A., Warren W. H., & Tarr M. J . ( 2007). Humans do not switch between path knowledge and landmarks when learning a new environment. Psychological Research, 71( 3), 240-251.
doi: 10.1007/s00426-006-0080-4
URL
|
6 |
Foo P., Warren W. H., Duchon A., & Tarr M. J . ( 2005). Do humans integrate routes into a cognitive map? Map-versus landmark-based navigation of novel shortcuts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31( 2), 195-215.
doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.31.2.195
URL
|
7 |
Guo, J. C.S., & Wan X.A, . ( 2015). The effect of learning in virtual path integration. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 47( 6), 711-720.
|
|
[ 过继成思, 宛小昂 . ( 2015). 虚拟路径整合的学习效应. 心理学报, 47( 6), 711-720.]
|
8 |
He, Q., & McNamara T.P, . ( 2018). Spatial updating strategy affects the reference frame in path integration. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25( 3), 1073-1079.
|
9 |
Kearns M. J., Warren W. H., Duchon A. P., & Tarr M. J . ( 2002). Path integration from optic flow and body senses in a homing task. Perception, 31, 349-374.
doi: 10.1068/p3311
URL
|
10 |
Li, D., & Yang, Z . ( 2015). Spatial navigation: The relationship between landmark learning and path integration. Advances in Psychological Science, 23( 10), 1755-1762.
|
|
[ 李丹, 杨昭宁 . ( 2015). 空间导航: 路标学习和路径整合的关系. 心理科学进展, 23(10#), 1755-1762.]
|
11 |
Loomis J. M., Klatzky R. L., Golledge R. G., Cicinelli J. G., Pellegrino J. W., & Fry P. A . ( 1993). Nonvisual navigation by blind and sighted: assessment of path integration ability. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122( 1), 73-91.
|
12 |
Philbeck J. W., Klatzky R. L., Behrmann M., Loomis J. M., & Goodridge J . ( 2001). Active control of locomotion facilitates nonvisual navigation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27( 1), 141-153.
|
13 |
Philbeck, J.W., & O’Leary S. ( 2005). Remembered landmarks enhance the precision of path integration. Psicologica, 26( 1), 7-24.
|
14 |
Poucet B., Sargolini F., Song E. Y., Hangya B., Fox S., & Muller R. U . ( 2014). Independence of landmark and self-motion-guided navigation: A different role for grid cells. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 369( 1635), 20130370.
|
15 |
Rossano, M.J., & Reardon W.P, . ( 1999). Goal specificity and the acquisition of survey knowledge. Environment and Behavior, 31(3), 395-412.
doi: 10.1177/00139169921972164
URL
|
16 |
Sjolund L. A., Kelly J. W., & McNamara T. P . ( 2018). Optimal combination of environmental cues and path integration during navigation. Memory & Cognition, 46( 1), 89-99.
|
17 |
Sweller, J. ( 1994). Cognitive load theory, learning difficulty, and instructional design. Learning and Instruction, 4( 4), 295-312.
|
18 |
Sweller, J., & Chandler P. ( 1994). Why some material is difficult to learn. Cognition and Instruction, 12( 3), 185-233.
|
19 |
Theeuwes, J. ( 2010). Top-down and bottom-up control of visual selection. Acta Psychologica, 135( 2), 77-99.
|
20 |
Wan, X. ( 2016). The phenomenon and mechanisms of path integration in humans. Hangzhou: Zhejiang University Press.
|
|
[ 宛小昂 . ( 2016). 人类路径整合的现象与机制. 杭州: 浙江大学出版社.]
|
21 |
Wan X., Wang R. F., & Crowell J. A . ( 2012). The effect of landmarks in human path integration. Acta Psychologica, 140( 1), 7-12.
|
22 |
Wan X., Wang R. F., & Crowell J. A . ( 2012). The effect of landmarks in human path integration. Acta Psychologica, 140( 1), 7-12.
|
23 |
Wan X., Wang R. F., & Crowell J. A . ( 2013). Effects of basic path properties on human path integration. Spatial Cognition & Computation, 13(1), 79-101.
|
24 |
Wang, R.F . ( 2016). Building a cognitive map by assembling multiple path integration. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23, 692-702.
|
25 |
Wang R. F., Crowell J. A., Simons D. J., Irwin D. E., Kramer A. F., Ambinder M. S., … Hsieh B. B . ( 2006). Spatial updating relies on an egocentric representation of space: Effects of the number of objects. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13( 2), 281-286.
|
26 |
Wiener J. M., Berthoz A., & Wolbers T . ( 2011). Dissociable cognitive mechanisms underlying human path integration. Experimental Brain Research, 208( 1),61-71.
doi: 10.1007/s00221-010-2460-7
URL
|
27 |
Zhang, L., & Mou W. ( 2017). Piloting systems reset path integration systems during position estimation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 43( 3), 472-491.
|
28 |
Zhao, M., & Warren W.H, . ( 2015 b). How you get there from here: Interaction of visual landmarks and path integration in human navigation. Psychological Science, 26( 6),915-924.
|
29 |
Zhou, J.S., & Zhang K . ( 2005). Kinestheic, Irrelevant Motion and Path Integration. Psychological Science, 28( 6), 1306-1308.
|
|
[ 周佳树, 张侃 . ( 2005). 运动觉、布局无关运动与路径整合. 心理科学, 28( 6), 1306-1308.]
|