心理学报 ›› 2023, Vol. 55 ›› Issue (4): 513-528.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2023.00513
• 研究报告 • 下一篇
收稿日期:
2022-07-04
发布日期:
2022-12-30
出版日期:
2023-04-25
通讯作者:
郭春彦
E-mail:guocy@cnu.edu.cn
基金资助:
Received:
2022-07-04
Online:
2022-12-30
Published:
2023-04-25
Contact:
GUO Chunyan
E-mail:guocy@cnu.edu.cn
摘要:
本研究通过考察合体字和复合词加工过程中联结记忆和项目记忆的变化, 探究了在将不同项目绑定为整体的过程中联结编码和项目编码间的关系, 并结合脑电技术比较了合体字和复合词的加工过程在神经机制上的差异。行为结果显示: 与不能组成字或词的编码条件相比, 合体字和复合词编码条件下的联结记忆成绩有显著提升, 且项目记忆成绩没有下降; 与复合词编码相比, 合体字编码条件下的联结记忆和项目记忆成绩都显著更好。脑电结果显示: 复合词编码时晚期负成分(LNC)主要位于额区且发生较早, 而合体字编码时的LNC主要位于枕区且发生更晚; 合体字编码比复合词编码在α/β频段的神经振荡去同步化更强。这些结果表明, 将不同项目绑定为整体的操作可以强化大脑对联结信息的加工, 同时不会损害对项目的加工, 支持了“只有收益”的观点。本研究也从实证的角度为汉语学习材料的编排提供了参考。
中图分类号:
赵春宇, 郭春彦. (2023). 合体字与复合词中联结编码和项目编码的关系. 心理学报, 55(4), 513-528.
ZHAO Chunyu, GUO Chunyan. (2023). The relationship between associative encoding and item encoding in the multiple-component character unitization and compound word unitization. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 55(4), 513-528.
统计项 | 合体字组 | 复合词组 | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
成字 | 不成字 | 成词 | 不成词 | |
RT (s) | 1.19 ± 0.15 | 1.20 ± 0.24 | 0. 80 ± 0.12 | 0.98 ± 0.19 |
Pr值 (击中率-虚报率) | 0.75 ± 0.15 | 0.26 ± 0.18 | 0.67 ± 0.15 | 0.40 ± 0.20 |
难度评分 | 2.85 ± 0.56 | 1.40 ± 0.41 |
表1 学习阶段、测验阶段以及评分任务中, 被试在不同条件下的反应时、联结再认表现和难度评分的结果(M ± SD)
统计项 | 合体字组 | 复合词组 | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
成字 | 不成字 | 成词 | 不成词 | |
RT (s) | 1.19 ± 0.15 | 1.20 ± 0.24 | 0. 80 ± 0.12 | 0.98 ± 0.19 |
Pr值 (击中率-虚报率) | 0.75 ± 0.15 | 0.26 ± 0.18 | 0.67 ± 0.15 | 0.40 ± 0.20 |
难度评分 | 2.85 ± 0.56 | 1.40 ± 0.41 |
[1] |
Ahmad, F. N., & Hockley, W. E. (2014). The role of familiarity in associative recognition of unitized compound word pairs. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(12), 2301-2324.
doi: 10.1080/17470218.2014.923007 URL |
[2] |
Anderson, R. C., Ku, Y. M., Li, W., Chen, X., Wu, X., & Shu, H. (2013). Learning to see the patterns in Chinese characters. Scientific Studies of Reading, 17(1), 41-56.
doi: 10.1080/10888438.2012.689789 URL |
[3] |
Bader, R., Opitz, B., Reith, W., & Mecklinger, A. (2014). Is a novel conceptual unit more than the sum of its parts?: FMRI evidence from an associative recognition memory study. Neuropsychologia, 61, 123-134.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.06.006 pmid: 24946317 |
[4] |
Baddeley, A. (1992). Working memory. Science, 255(5044), 556-559.
doi: 10.1126/science.1736359 pmid: 1736359 |
[5] |
Buchler, N. G., Light, L. L., & Reder, L. M. (2008). Memory for items and associations: Distinct representations and processes in associative recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 59(2), 183-199.
doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2008.04.001 pmid: 18677424 |
[6] | Ceraso, J. (1985). Unit formation in perception and memory. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 19(4), 179-210. |
[7] |
Chung, K. K., Tong, X., Liu, P. D., McBride-Chang, C., & Meng, X. (2010). The processing of morphological structure information in Chinese coordinative compounds: An event- related potential study. Brain Research, 1352, 157-166.
doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.06.069 URL |
[8] |
de Brigard, F., Langella, S., Stanley, M. L., Castel, A. D., & Giovanello, K. S. (2020). Age-related differences in recognition in associative memory. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 27(2), 289-301.
doi: 10.1080/13825585.2019.1607820 |
[9] |
Delorme, A., & Makeig, S. (2004). EEGLAB: An open-source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 134(1), 9-21.
doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009 pmid: 15102499 |
[10] |
Diana, R. A., Yonelinas, A. P., & Ranganath, C. (2010). Medial temporal lobe activity during source retrieval reflects information type, not memory strength. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(8), 1808-1818.
doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21335 pmid: 19702458 |
[11] |
Gao, F., Wang, R., Armada-da-Silva, P., Wang, M. Y., Lu, H., Leong, C., & Yuan, Z. (2022). How the brain encodes morphological constraints during Chinese word reading: An EEG-fNIRS study. Cortex, 154, 184-196.
doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2022.05.016 pmid: 35780754 |
[12] |
Giovanello, K. S., Keane, M. M., & Verfaellie, M. (2006). The contribution of familiarity to associative memory in amnesia. Neuropsychologia, 44(10), 1859-1865.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.03.004 pmid: 16643967 |
[13] |
Giovanello, K. S., Schnyer, D., & Verfaellie, M. (2009). Distinct hippocampal regions make unique contributions to relational memory. Hippocampus, 19(2), 111-117.
doi: 10.1002/hipo.20491 pmid: 18727049 |
[14] | Giovanello, K. S., Verfaellie, M., & Keane, M. M. (2003). Disproportionate deficit in associative recognition relative to item recognition in global amnesia. Cognitive Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 3(3), 186-194. |
[15] |
Griffiths, B. J., Mayhew, S. D., Mullinger, K. J., Jorge, J., Charest, I., Wimber, M., & Hanslmayr, S. (2019). Alpha/beta power decreases track the fidelity of stimulus-specific information. Elife, 8, e49562.
doi: 10.7554/eLife.49562 URL |
[16] |
Gui, P., Ku, Y., Li, L., Li, X., Bodner, M., Lenz, F. A., ... Zhou, Y. D. (2017). Neural correlates of visuo-tactile crossmodal paired-associate learning and memory in humans. Neuroscience, 362, 181-195.
doi: S0306-4522(17)30601-2 pmid: 28843996 |
[17] |
Han, J., Cao, B., Cao, Y., Gao, H., & Li, F. (2016). The role of right frontal brain regions in integration of spatial relation. Neuropsychologia, 86, 29-37.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.04.008 pmid: 27072324 |
[18] |
Hanslmayr, S., Staudigl, T., & Fellner, M. C. (2012). Oscillatory power decreases and long-term memory: The information via desynchronization hypothesis. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 74.
doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00074 pmid: 22514527 |
[19] |
Haskins, A. L., Yonelinas, A. P., Quamme, J. R., & Ranganath, C. (2008). Perirhinal cortex supports encoding and familiarity- based recognition of novel associations. Neuron, 59(4), 554-560.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.07.035 pmid: 18760692 |
[20] |
Hockley, W. E., & Cristi, C. (1996). Tests of encoding tradeoffs between item and associative information. Memory and Cognition, 24(2), 202-216.
pmid: 8881323 |
[21] | Huang, G. (2019). EEG/ERP data analysis toolboxes. In L. Hu & Z. Zhang (Eds.), EEG signal processing and feature extraction (pp. 407-434). Singapore, Springer. |
[22] |
Ip, K. I., Marks, R. A., Hsu, L. S. J., Desai, N., Kuan, J. L., & Tardif, T. (2019). Morphological processing in Chinese engages left temporal regions. Brain and Language, 199, 104696.
doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2019.104696 URL |
[23] | Kamp, S. M., Bader, R., & Mecklinger, A. (2016). The effect of unitizing word pairs on recollection versus familiarity- based retrieval-Further evidence from ERPs. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 12(4), 169-178. |
[24] | Kamp, S. M., Bader, R., & Mecklinger, A. (2017). ERP subsequent memory effects differ between inter-item and unitization encoding tasks. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 11, 30. |
[25] |
Kamp, S. M., & Zimmer, H. D. (2015). Contributions of attention and elaboration to associative encoding in young and older adults. Neuropsychologia, 75, 252-264.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.06.026 URL |
[26] |
Khader, P., Heil, M., & Rösler, F. (2005). Material-specific long-term memory representations of faces and spatial positions: Evidence from slow event-related brain potentials. Neuropsychologia, 43(14), 2109-2124.
pmid: 16243055 |
[27] |
Kim, A. S., Vallesi, A., Picton, T. W., & Tulving, E. (2009). Cognitive association formation in episodic memory: Evidence from event-related potentials. Neuropsychologia, 47(14), 3162-3173.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.015 pmid: 19651150 |
[28] |
Kounios, J., Smith, R. W., Yang, W., Bachman, P., & D'Esposito, M. (2001). Cognitive association formation in human memory revealed by spatiotemporal brain imaging. Neuron, 29(1), 297-306.
pmid: 11182100 |
[29] |
Li, B., Zhang, M., Luo, J., Qiu, J., & Liu, Y. (2014). The difference in spatiotemporal dynamics between modus ponens and modus tollens in the Wason selection task: An event-related potential study. Neuroscience, 270, 177-182.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2014.04.007 pmid: 24726487 |
[30] |
Liu, Z., & Guo, C. (2019). Unitization improves item recognition through less overall neural processing. Neuroreport, 30(13), 882-886.
doi: 10.1097/WNR.0000000000001296 pmid: 31373967 |
[31] |
Liu, Z., & Guo, C. (2022). Effects of unitization on associative and item recognition: The “benefits-only” account. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 54(12), 1443-1454.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2022.01443 URL |
[刘泽军, 郭春彦. (2022). 整合对联结再认和项目再认的促进作用: “只有收益”观点. 心理学报, 54(12), 1443-1454.] | |
[32] |
Liu, Z. J., Wang, Y. J., & Guo, C. Y. (2019). Investigating the item recognition in associative memory: A unitization perspective. Advances in Psychological Science, 27(3), 490-498.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2019.00490 URL |
[刘泽军, 王余娟, 郭春彦. (2019). 从整合的角度看联结记忆中的项目再认. 心理科学进展, 27(3), 490-498.] | |
[33] | Liu, Z., Wu, J., Wang, Y., & Guo, C. (2020). Unitization does not impede overall item recognition performance: Behavioral and event-related potential study. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 167, 107-130. |
[34] |
Lopez-Calderon, J., & Luck, S. J. (2014). ERPLAB: An open-source toolbox for the analysis of event-related potentials. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 213.
doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00213 pmid: 24782741 |
[35] |
Lu, B., Liu, Z., Wang, Y., & Guo, C. (2020). The different effects of concept definition and interactive imagery encoding on associative recognition for word and picture stimuli. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 158, 178-189.
doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2020.09.012 pmid: 33080290 |
[36] |
Maris, E., & Oostenveld, R. (2007). Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG-and MEG-data. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 164(1), 177-190.
doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2007.03.024 pmid: 17517438 |
[37] |
Martin, E. (1968). Stimulus meaningfulness and paired- associate transfer: An encoding variability hypothesis. Psychological Review, 75(5), 421-441.
pmid: 4879426 |
[38] |
Memel, M., & Ryan, L. (2017). Visual integration enhances associative memory equally for young and older adults without reducing hippocampal encoding activation. Neuropsychologia, 100, 195-206.
doi: S0028-3932(17)30159-8 pmid: 28456521 |
[39] |
Memel, M., & Ryan, T. L. (2018). Visual integration of objects and scenes increases recollection-based responding despite differential MTL recruitment in young and older adults. Hippocampus, 28(12), 886-899.
doi: 10.1002/hipo.23011 pmid: 29999561 |
[40] |
Murray, B. D., & Kensinger, E. A. (2012). The effects of emotion and encoding strategy on associative memory. Memory & Cognition, 40(7), 1056-1069.
doi: 10.3758/s13421-012-0215-3 URL |
[41] |
Parks, C. M., & Yonelinas, A. P. (2015). The importance of unitization for familiarity-based learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 41(3), 881-903.
doi: 10.1037/xlm0000068 URL |
[42] |
Pilgrim, L. K., Murray, J. G., & Donaldson, D. I. (2012). Characterizing episodic memory retrieval: Electrophysiological evidence for diminished familiarity following unitization. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24(8), 1671-1681.
doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00186 pmid: 22220725 |
[43] |
Quamme, J. R., Yonelinas, A. P., & Norman, K. A. (2007). Effect of unitization on associative recognition in amnesia. Hippocampus, 17(3), 192-200.
pmid: 17203466 |
[44] |
Rhodes, S. M., & Donaldson, D. I. (2008). Electrophysiological evidence for the effect of interactive imagery on episodic memory: Encouraging familiarity for non-unitized stimuli during associative recognition. Neuroimage, 39(2), 873-884.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.08.041 pmid: 17950624 |
[45] |
Robey, A., & Riggins, T. (2017). Increasing relational memory in childhood with unitization strategies. Memory & Cognition, 46(1), 100-111.
doi: 10.3758/s13421-017-0748-6 |
[46] | Shao, H., & Weng, X. (2011, November). Unitization benefits associative recognition whereas impairs item recognition. In 2011 Third International Conference on Multimedia Information Networking and Security (pp. 314-317). IEEE. |
[47] |
Sommer, K., Vita, S., & de Pascalis, V. (2018). The late posterior negativity in episodic memory: A correlate of stimulus retrieval? Biological Psychology, 133, 44-53.
doi: S0301-0511(18)30067-X pmid: 29391180 |
[48] |
Su, M., Wang, J., Maurer, U., Zhang, Y., Li, J., McBride, C., ... Shu, H. (2015). Gene-environment interaction on neural mechanisms of orthographic processing in Chinese children. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 33, 172-186.
pmid: 26294811 |
[49] | Tibon, R., Gronau, N., & Levy, D. A. (2017). Associative unitization via semantic relatedness benefits episodic recognition of component elements. PsyArXiv. https://osf.io/preprints/psyarxiv/cjxse/ |
[50] | Tulving, E. (1972). Episodic and semantic memory. In E. Tulving & W. Donaldson (Eds.), Organization of memory (pp. 381-403). New York, Academic Press. |
[51] |
Wiegand, I., Bader, R., & Mecklinger, A. (2010). Multiple ways to the prior occurrence of an event: An electrophysiological dissociation of experimental and conceptually driven familiarity in recognition memory. Brain Research, 1360, 106-118.
doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.08.089 pmid: 20816760 |
[52] |
Zheng, Z. W., Li, J., & Xiao, F. Q. (2015). Familiarity contributes to associative memory: The role of unitization. Advances in Psychological Science, 23(2), 202-212.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2015.00202 URL |
[郑志伟, 李娟, 肖凤秋. (2015). 熟悉性能够支持联结记忆: 一体化编码的作用. 心理科学进展, 23(2), 202-212.] | |
[53] |
Zheng, Z., Li, J., Xiao, F., Broster, L. S., Jiang, Y., & Xi, M. (2015). The effects of unitization on the contribution of familiarity and recollection processes to associative recognition memory: Evidence from event-related potentials. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 95(3), 355-362.
doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2015.01.003 pmid: 25583573 |
[54] |
Zheng, Z., Li, J., Xiao, F., Ren, W., & He, R. (2016). Unitization improves source memory in older adults: An event-related potential study. Neuropsychologia, 89, 232-244.
doi: S0028-3932(16)30224-X pmid: 27343684 |
[1] | 蔡文琦, 张向阳, 王小娟, 杨剑峰. 汉语复合词语素意义与整词语义整合加工的时间进程[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(8): 1207-1219. |
[2] | 龙翼婷, 姜英杰, 崔璨, 岳阳. 奖赏预测误差对项目和联结记忆影响的分离:元记忆的作用[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(6): 877-891. |
[3] | 张引, 梁腾飞, 叶超雄, 刘强. 长时联结表征对工作记忆的抑制效应[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(5): 562-571. |
[4] | 吴建设, 常嘉宝, 邱寅晨, Joseph Dien. 汉语复合词视觉识别的时间进程:基于同形语素的行为与ERP证据[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(2): 113-127. |
[5] | 律原;梁九清;郭春彦. 项目间语义可整合性对联结再认的影响 ——来自ERPs研究证据[J]. 心理学报, 2015, 47(4): 427-438. |
[6] | 梁九清,郭春彦. 跨领域项目间联结记忆中项目提取和关系提取的分离:一项事件相关电位研究[J]. 心理学报, 2012, 44(5): 625-633. |
[7] | 聂爱情,郭春彦,沈模卫. 图形项目记忆与位置来源提取的ERP研究[J]. 心理学报, 2007, 39(01): 50-57. |
[8] | 杨志新. 嗓音源判断过程中的范畴效应和通道效应[J]. 心理学报, 2001, 33(4): 41-45. |
[9] | 徐彩华, 宋凤宁, 张必隐, 李镗. 汉语儿童复合词构词意识的发展与阅读理解[J]. 心理学报, 2000, 32(增刊): 82-86. |
[10] | 杨志新. 系列范畴词表时序记忆和项目记忆研究[J]. 心理学报, 1998, 30(1): 35-42. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||