心理学报 ›› 2021, Vol. 53 ›› Issue (10): 1161-1172.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.01161
• 研究报告 • 上一篇
收稿日期:
2020-08-19
发布日期:
2021-08-23
出版日期:
2021-10-25
通讯作者:
郭永玉
E-mail:yyguo@njnu.edu.cn
基金资助:
BAI Jie1, YANG Shenlong2, XU Buxiao3, GUO Yongyu1()
Received:
2020-08-19
Online:
2021-08-23
Published:
2021-10-25
Contact:
GUO Yongyu
E-mail:yyguo@njnu.edu.cn
摘要:
一些欧美研究发现高阶层者与低阶层者相比再分配偏向更低, 但是这一结论尚缺少跨文化一致性的证据, 而且对于这一现象的心理机制和干预策略的探讨也相对不足。基于此, 本研究通过3个子研究分别来关注我国民众再分配偏向的阶层差异及其心理机制, 并探讨可能的干预策略。结果发现:(1)与西方社会的情形相似, 在中国社会高阶层者的再分配偏向也显著低于低阶层者; (2)社会阶层对再分配偏向的影响, 部分是通过贫富差距归因倾向起作用的:与低阶层者相比, 高阶层者更倾向于将贫富差距归因于能力、努力、志向或抱负等个体内部因素, 进而再分配偏向更低; (3)通过启动高阶层者谦卑的心态, 能够降低其贫富差距内归因倾向, 进而提升其再分配偏向。此结果验证了社会阶层不平等维持模型的重要观点, 同时也为推动社会再分配、促进发展成果共享等提供了一定的社会治理启示。
中图分类号:
白洁, 杨沈龙, 徐步霄, 郭永玉. (2021). 达者何以兼济天下:高阶层再分配偏向的心理机制及谦卑的作用. 心理学报, 53(10), 1161-1172.
BAI Jie, YANG Shenlong, XU Buxiao, GUO Yongyu. (2021). How can successful people share their goodness with the world: The psychological mechanism underlying the upper social classes’ redistributive preferences and the role of humility. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 53(10), 1161-1172.
变量 | %/M | SD | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | 9 | 10 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1性别 | 51.50 | — | ||||||||||
2年龄 | 51.02 | 16.17 | 0.02 | |||||||||
3民族 | 92.60 | — | 0.00 | -0.03** | ||||||||
4宗教 | 11.40 | — | -0.08*** | -0.04*** | -0.22*** | |||||||
5政治 | 90.20 | — | 0.02 | 0.02 | 0.00 | 0.00 | ||||||
6婚姻 | 20.60 | — | -0.03** | 0.03** | 0.01 | 0.02 | -0.02 | |||||
7客观 | 0.00 | 0.86 | -0.15*** | -0.40*** | -0.10*** | 0.07*** | -0.03** | 0.00 | ||||
8主观 | 4.35 | 1.61 | 0.03* | 0.00 | -0.04*** | 0.00 | 0.02 | 0.06*** | 0.25*** | |||
9 家庭 | 2.67 | 0.72 | -0.01 | -0.07*** | -0.05*** | 0.00 | 0.00 | 0.07*** | 0.31** | 0.45*** | ||
10个人 | 1.73 | 0.59 | -0.02* | 0.00 | -0.05*** | 0.01 | 0.00 | 0.05*** | 0.25*** | 0.40*** | 0.55*** | |
11偏向 | 3.78 | 0.93 | 0.02 | 0.10*** | 0.03* | -0.03* | 0.02 | 0.00 | -0.12*** | -0.06*** | -0.08*** | -0.06*** |
表1 各变量的描述性统计和相关分析
变量 | %/M | SD | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | 9 | 10 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1性别 | 51.50 | — | ||||||||||
2年龄 | 51.02 | 16.17 | 0.02 | |||||||||
3民族 | 92.60 | — | 0.00 | -0.03** | ||||||||
4宗教 | 11.40 | — | -0.08*** | -0.04*** | -0.22*** | |||||||
5政治 | 90.20 | — | 0.02 | 0.02 | 0.00 | 0.00 | ||||||
6婚姻 | 20.60 | — | -0.03** | 0.03** | 0.01 | 0.02 | -0.02 | |||||
7客观 | 0.00 | 0.86 | -0.15*** | -0.40*** | -0.10*** | 0.07*** | -0.03** | 0.00 | ||||
8主观 | 4.35 | 1.61 | 0.03* | 0.00 | -0.04*** | 0.00 | 0.02 | 0.06*** | 0.25*** | |||
9 家庭 | 2.67 | 0.72 | -0.01 | -0.07*** | -0.05*** | 0.00 | 0.00 | 0.07*** | 0.31** | 0.45*** | ||
10个人 | 1.73 | 0.59 | -0.02* | 0.00 | -0.05*** | 0.01 | 0.00 | 0.05*** | 0.25*** | 0.40*** | 0.55*** | |
11偏向 | 3.78 | 0.93 | 0.02 | 0.10*** | 0.03* | -0.03* | 0.02 | 0.00 | -0.12*** | -0.06*** | -0.08*** | -0.06*** |
变量 | %/M | SD | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | 9 | 10 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1性别 | 41.70 | — | ||||||||||
2 年龄 | 32.73 | 8.73 | -0.04 | |||||||||
3 民族 | 94.20 | — | 0.00 | -0.05 | ||||||||
4 户口 | 60.20 | — | -0.05 | -0.11** | -0.05 | |||||||
5 政治 | 19.00 | — | 0.13** | -0.08* | -0.04 | 0.20*** | ||||||
6 宗教 | 90.30 | — | -0.07 | -0.07 | 0.32*** | 0.02 | 0.12** | |||||
7 婚姻 | 61.80 | — | 0.13*** | 0.49*** | -0.08* | -0.10* | -0.04 | -0.14*** | ||||
8 客观 | 0.01 | 0.76 | -0.14*** | -0.18*** | 0.10* | -0.30*** | -0.27*** | 0.12** | -0.26*** | |||
9 主观 | 4.15 | 1.88 | 0.04 | -0.07 | -0.02 | -0.13** | -0.04 | -0.03 | -0.02 | 0.30*** | ||
10归因 | 0.27 | 1.05 | 0.02 | 0.00 | -0.01 | 0.10* | 0.11** | -0.05 | 0.00 | -0.07 | 0.22*** | |
11偏向 | 5.35 | 1.00 | 0.03 | 0.23*** | 0.05 | 0.02 | 0.03 | 0.14*** | 0.09* | 0.01 | -0.21*** | -0.15*** |
表2 各变量的描述性统计和相关分析
变量 | %/M | SD | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | 9 | 10 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1性别 | 41.70 | — | ||||||||||
2 年龄 | 32.73 | 8.73 | -0.04 | |||||||||
3 民族 | 94.20 | — | 0.00 | -0.05 | ||||||||
4 户口 | 60.20 | — | -0.05 | -0.11** | -0.05 | |||||||
5 政治 | 19.00 | — | 0.13** | -0.08* | -0.04 | 0.20*** | ||||||
6 宗教 | 90.30 | — | -0.07 | -0.07 | 0.32*** | 0.02 | 0.12** | |||||
7 婚姻 | 61.80 | — | 0.13*** | 0.49*** | -0.08* | -0.10* | -0.04 | -0.14*** | ||||
8 客观 | 0.01 | 0.76 | -0.14*** | -0.18*** | 0.10* | -0.30*** | -0.27*** | 0.12** | -0.26*** | |||
9 主观 | 4.15 | 1.88 | 0.04 | -0.07 | -0.02 | -0.13** | -0.04 | -0.03 | -0.02 | 0.30*** | ||
10归因 | 0.27 | 1.05 | 0.02 | 0.00 | -0.01 | 0.10* | 0.11** | -0.05 | 0.00 | -0.07 | 0.22*** | |
11偏向 | 5.35 | 1.00 | 0.03 | 0.23*** | 0.05 | 0.02 | 0.03 | 0.14*** | 0.09* | 0.01 | -0.21*** | -0.15*** |
因变量 | R2 | β | SE | t | 95% CI |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
内归因倾向作 因变量 | 0.09 | ||||
年龄 | 0.03 | 0.05 | 0.64 | [-0.06, 0.12] | |
户口类型 | 0.09 | 0.04 | 2.32* | [0.01, 0.17] | |
政治面貌 | 0.16 | 0.04 | 3.76*** | [0.08, 0.25] | |
宗教信仰 | -0.06 | 0.04 | -1.44 | [-0.13, 0.02] | |
婚姻状况 | -0.07 | 0.05 | -1.46 | [-0.17, 0.02] | |
主观阶层 | 0.24 | 0.04 | 6.11*** | [0.16, 0.32] | |
再分配偏向作 因变量 | 0.14 | ||||
年龄 | 0.25 | 0.04 | 5.77*** | [0.17, 0.34] | |
户口类型 | 0.03 | 0.04 | 0.78 | [-0.05, 0.11] | |
政治面貌 | 0.01 | 0.04 | 0.19 | [-0.08, 0.09] | |
宗教信仰 | 0.15 | 0.04 | 3.82*** | [0.07, 0.22] | |
婚姻状况 | -0.02 | 0.05 | -0.38 | [-0.11, 0.08] | |
主观阶层 | -0.17 | 0.04 | -4.20*** | [-0.24, -0.09] | |
内归因倾向 | -0.11 | 0.04 | -2.85* | [-0.19, -0.04] |
表3 内归因倾向在主观阶层预测效应中的中介作用分析
因变量 | R2 | β | SE | t | 95% CI |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
内归因倾向作 因变量 | 0.09 | ||||
年龄 | 0.03 | 0.05 | 0.64 | [-0.06, 0.12] | |
户口类型 | 0.09 | 0.04 | 2.32* | [0.01, 0.17] | |
政治面貌 | 0.16 | 0.04 | 3.76*** | [0.08, 0.25] | |
宗教信仰 | -0.06 | 0.04 | -1.44 | [-0.13, 0.02] | |
婚姻状况 | -0.07 | 0.05 | -1.46 | [-0.17, 0.02] | |
主观阶层 | 0.24 | 0.04 | 6.11*** | [0.16, 0.32] | |
再分配偏向作 因变量 | 0.14 | ||||
年龄 | 0.25 | 0.04 | 5.77*** | [0.17, 0.34] | |
户口类型 | 0.03 | 0.04 | 0.78 | [-0.05, 0.11] | |
政治面貌 | 0.01 | 0.04 | 0.19 | [-0.08, 0.09] | |
宗教信仰 | 0.15 | 0.04 | 3.82*** | [0.07, 0.22] | |
婚姻状况 | -0.02 | 0.05 | -0.38 | [-0.11, 0.08] | |
主观阶层 | -0.17 | 0.04 | -4.20*** | [-0.24, -0.09] | |
内归因倾向 | -0.11 | 0.04 | -2.85* | [-0.19, -0.04] |
因变量 | R2 | β | SE | t | 95% CI |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
内归因倾向作因变量 | 0.03 | ||||
年龄 | 0.03 | 0.05 | 0.64 | [-0.06, 0.12] | |
户口类型 | 0.04 | 0.04 | 0.96 | [-0.04, 0.13] | |
政治面貌 | 0.18 | 0.05 | 3.82*** | [0.09, 0.28] | |
宗教信仰 | -0.04 | 0.04 | -0.99 | [-0.13,.04] | |
婚姻状况 | -0.08 | 0.05 | -1.54 | [-.18, 0.02] | |
客观阶层 | 0.03 | 0.06 | 0.42 | [-0.10, 0.15] | |
再分配偏向作因变量 | 0.10 | ||||
年龄 | 0.27 | 0.05 | 5.86*** | [0.18, 0.36] | |
户口类型 | 0.06 | 0.04 | 1.39 | [-0.02, 0.14] | |
政治面貌 | 0.04 | 0.05 | 0.90 | [-0.05, 0.14] | |
宗教信仰 | 0.14 | 0.04 | 3.34*** | [0.06, 0.23] | |
婚姻状况 | -0.02 | 0.05 | -0.35 | [-0.12, 0.08] | |
客观阶层 | 0.08 | 0.06 | 1.31 | [-0.04, 0.21] | |
内归因倾向 | -0.15 | 0.04 | -3.65*** | [-0.23, -0.07] |
表4 内归因倾向在客观阶层预测效应中的中介作用分析
因变量 | R2 | β | SE | t | 95% CI |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
内归因倾向作因变量 | 0.03 | ||||
年龄 | 0.03 | 0.05 | 0.64 | [-0.06, 0.12] | |
户口类型 | 0.04 | 0.04 | 0.96 | [-0.04, 0.13] | |
政治面貌 | 0.18 | 0.05 | 3.82*** | [0.09, 0.28] | |
宗教信仰 | -0.04 | 0.04 | -0.99 | [-0.13,.04] | |
婚姻状况 | -0.08 | 0.05 | -1.54 | [-.18, 0.02] | |
客观阶层 | 0.03 | 0.06 | 0.42 | [-0.10, 0.15] | |
再分配偏向作因变量 | 0.10 | ||||
年龄 | 0.27 | 0.05 | 5.86*** | [0.18, 0.36] | |
户口类型 | 0.06 | 0.04 | 1.39 | [-0.02, 0.14] | |
政治面貌 | 0.04 | 0.05 | 0.90 | [-0.05, 0.14] | |
宗教信仰 | 0.14 | 0.04 | 3.34*** | [0.06, 0.23] | |
婚姻状况 | -0.02 | 0.05 | -0.35 | [-0.12, 0.08] | |
客观阶层 | 0.08 | 0.06 | 1.31 | [-0.04, 0.21] | |
内归因倾向 | -0.15 | 0.04 | -3.65*** | [-0.23, -0.07] |
[1] |
Alesina A., & Angeletos G. M.(2005). Fairness and redistribution. American Economic Review, 95(4), 960-980.
doi: 10.1257/0002828054825655 URL |
[2] |
Andersen R., & Curtis J.(2015). Social class, economic inequality, and the convergence of policy preferences: Evidence from 24 modern democracies. Canadian Review of Sociology, 52(3), 266-288.
doi: 10.1111/cars.2015.52.issue-3 URL |
[3] |
Belmi P., & Laurin K.(2016). Who wants to get to the top? Class and lay theories about power. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 111(4), 505-529.
doi: 10.1037/pspi0000060 URL |
[4] | Berg J. H., Stephan W. G., & Dodson M.(2006). Attributional modesty in women. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 5(s5), 711-727. |
[5] |
Blickle G., Diekmann C., Schneider P. B., Kalthöfer Y., & Summers J. K.(2012). When modesty wins: Impression management through modesty, political skill, and career success—A two-study investigation. European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 21(6), 899-922.
doi: 10.1080/1359432X.2011.603900 URL |
[6] |
Brown-Iannuzzi J. L., Lundberg K. B., Kay A. C., & Payne B. K.(2015). Subjective status shapes political preferences. Psychological Science, 26(1), 15-26.
doi: 10.1177/0956797614553947 pmid: 25416138 |
[7] |
Brown-Iannuzzi J. L., Lundberg K. B., & Mckee S.(2017). The politics of socioeconomic status: How socioeconomic status may influence political attitudes and engagement. Current Opinion in Psychology, 18, 11-14.
doi: S2352-250X(17)30072-6 pmid: 29221505 |
[8] |
Bullock H. E.(2017). Social class and policy preferences: Implications for economic inequality and interclass relations. Current Opinion in Psychology, 18, 141-146.
doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.08.021 URL |
[9] | Cai F.(2020). Creation and protection: Why do we need more redistribution. World Economics and Politics, 1, 5-21. |
[ 蔡昉.(2020). 创造与保护: 为什么需要更多的再分配. 世界经济与政治, 1, 5-21.] | |
[10] |
Cohen S., Alper C. M., Doyle W. J., Adler N., Treanor J. J., & Turner R. B.(2008). Objective and subjective socioeconomic status and susceptibility to the common cold. Health Psychology, 27(2), 268-274.
doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.27.2.268 URL |
[11] |
Davis D. E., McElroy S., Choe E., Westbrook C. J., DeBlaere C., van Tongeren D. R.,... Placeres V.(2017). Development of the experiences of humility scale. Journal of Psychology and Theology, 45(1), 3-16.
doi: 10.1177/009164711704500101 URL |
[12] |
Davis D. E., Worthington E. L., & Hook J. N.(2010). Humility: Review of measurement strategies and conceptualization as personality judgment. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 5(4), 243-252.
doi: 10.1080/17439761003791672 URL |
[13] |
Durante R., Putterman L., & van der Weele J.(2014). Preferences for redistribution and perception of fairness: An experimental study. Journal of the European Economic Association, 12(4), 1059-1086.
doi: 10.1111/jeea.12082 URL |
[14] | Exline J. J., Campbell W. K., Baumeister R. F., Joiner T., & Krueger J.(2004). Humility and modesty. In C. Peterson, & M. P. Seligman (Eds.), Character strengths and virtues: A handbook of classification (pp. 461-475). New York: Oxford University Press. |
[15] |
Exline J. J., & Hill P. C.(2012). Humility: A consistent and robust predictor of generosity. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 7(3), 208-218.
doi: 10.1080/17439760.2012.671348 URL |
[16] | Hayes A. F.(2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York: The Guilford Press. |
[17] |
Hu X. Y., Guo Y. Y., Li J., & Yang S. L.(2016). Perceived societal fairness and goal attainment: The different effects of social class and their mechanism. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 48(3),271-289.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2016.00271 URL |
[ 胡小勇, 郭永玉, 李静, 杨沈龙.(2016). 社会公平感对不同阶层目标达成的影响及其过程. 心理学报, 48(3),271-289.] | |
[18] |
Hussak L. J., & Cimpian A.(2015). An early-emerging explanatory heuristic promotes support for the status quo. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 109(5), 739-752.
doi: 10.1037/pspa0000033 URL |
[19] |
Kraus M. W., & Callaghan B.(2014). Noblesse oblige? Social status and economic inequality maintenance among politicians. PLoS One, 9(1), e85293.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0085293 URL |
[20] |
Kraus M. W., Côté S., & Keltner D.(2010). Social class, contextualism, and empathic accuracy. Psychological Science, 21(11), 1716-1723.
doi: 10.1177/0956797610387613 URL |
[21] |
Kraus M. W., Piff P. K., & Keltner D.(2009). Social class, sense of control, and social explanation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97(6), 992-1004.
doi: 10.1037/a0016357 URL |
[22] |
Krawczyk M.(2010). A glimpse through the veil of ignorance: Equality of opportunity and support for redistribution. Journal of Public Economics, 94(1-2), 131-141.
doi: 10.1016/j.jpubeco.2009.10.003 URL |
[23] |
Kruse E., Chancellor J., & Lyubomirsky S.(2017). State humility: Measurement, conceptual validation, and intrapersonal processes. Self and Identity, 16(4), 399-438.
doi: 10.1080/15298868.2016.1267662 URL |
[24] |
Laurison D.(2016). Social class and political engagement in the United States. Sociology Compass, 10(8), 684-697.
doi: 10.1111/soc4.12390 URL |
[25] |
Leckelt M., Richter D., Schroder C., Kufner A. C., Grabka M. M., & Back M. D.(2019). The rich are different: Unravelling the perceived and self‐reported personality profiles of high‐net‐worth individuals. British Journal of Psychology, 110(4), 769-789.
doi: 10.1111/bjop.12360 pmid: 30466138 |
[26] | Li J.(2014). Study on the tendency of attribution on the gap between the rich and the poor in different social classes. Guangzhou: World Publishing Corporation. |
[ 李静.(2014). 不同社会阶层对贫富差距的心理归因研究. 广州: 世界图书出版公司.] | |
[27] | Li Q. B.(2012). China’s income redistribution: Determinants of preferences, policy formation and effects calculation (Unpublished doctorial dissertation) . Nankai University, Tianjin, China. |
[ 李清彬.(2012). 中国居民收入再分配的倾向决定, 政策形成和效应测算 (博士学位论文). 南开大学, 天津.] | |
[28] |
Li W. Q., Yang Y., Wu J. H., & Kou Y.(2020). Testing the status-legitimacy hypothesis in China: Objective and subjective socioeconomic status divergently predict system justification. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 46(7), 1044-1058.
doi: 10.1177/0146167219893997 URL |
[29] | Liu X.(2007). Class structure and the middle class location in urban China. Sociological Studies, 6, 1-14. |
[ 刘欣.(2007). 中国城市的阶层结构与中产阶层的定位. 社会学研究, 6, 1-14.] | |
[30] | Lu X. Y. (Ed.).(2002). Contemporary Chinese social class research report. Beijing,China: Social Sciences Academic Press. |
[ 陆学艺.(Ed.).(2002). 当代中国社会阶层研究报告. 北京: 社会科学文献出版社.] | |
[31] |
Martin S. R., Côté S., & Woodruff T.(2016). Echoes of our upbringing: How growing up wealthy or poor relates to narcissism, leader behavior, and leader effectiveness. Academy of Management Journal, 59(6), 2157-2177.
doi: 10.5465/amj.2015.0680 URL |
[32] |
Meltzer A. H., & Richard S. F.(1981). A rational theory of the size of government. Journal of Political Economy, 89(5), 914-927.
doi: 10.1086/261013 URL |
[33] |
Miyamoto Y.(2017). Culture and social class. Current Opinion in Psychology, 18, 67-72.
doi: S2352-250X(17)30045-3 pmid: 28826007 |
[34] |
Page B. I., Bartels L. M., & Seawright J.(2013). Democracy and the policy preferences of wealthy Americans. Perspectives on Politics, 11(1), 51-73.
doi: 10.1017/S153759271200360X URL |
[35] | Pan C. Y., & He L. X.(2011). Pursuing self interests or distributive justice? An empirical study of the preference for redistribution of Chinese residents. Journal of Economic Review, 5, 20-29. |
[ 潘春阳, 何立新.(2011). 独善其身还是兼济天下?——中国居民再分配偏好的实证研究. 经济评论, 5, 20-29.] | |
[36] |
Piff P. K.(2014). Wealth and the inflated self: Class, entitlement, and narcissism. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 40(1), 34-43.
doi: 10.1177/0146167213501699 URL |
[37] |
Piff P. K., Kraus M. W., Cote S., Cheng B. H., & Keltner D.(2010). Having less, giving more: The influence of social class on prosocial behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99(5), 771-784.
doi: 10.1037/a0020092 URL |
[38] | Piff P. K., Kraus M. W., & Keltner D.(2018). Unpacking the inequality paradox: The psychological roots of inequality and social class. In J. M. Olson (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (pp. 53-124). San Diego: Elsevier Academic Press. |
[39] | Qiu L., Zheng X., & Wang Y. F.(2008). Revision of the positive affect and negative affect scale. Chinese Journal of Applied Psychology, 14(3), 249-254. |
[ 邱林, 郑雪, 王雁飞.(2008). 积极情感消极情感量表(PANAS)的修订. 应用心理学, 14(3), 249-254.] | |
[40] |
Reeves A., & de Vries R.(2016). Does media coverage influence public attitudes towards welfare recipients? The impact of the 2011 English riots. British Journal of Sociology, 67(2), 281-306.
doi: 10.1111/bjos.2016.67.issue-2 URL |
[41] |
Rodriguez‐Bailon R., Bratanova B., Willis G. B., Lopez‐Rodriguez L., Sturrock A., & Loughnan S.(2017). Social class and ideologies of inequality: How they uphold unequal societies. Journal of Social Issues, 73(1), 99-116.
doi: 10.1111/josi.2017.73.issue-1 URL |
[42] |
Rodriguez‐Bailon R., Sanchez‐Rodriguez A., Garcia‐Sanchez E., Petkanopoulou K., & Willis G. B.(2020). Inequality is in the air: Contextual psychosocial effects of power and social class. Current Opinion in Psychology, 33, 120-125.
doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2019.07.004 URL |
[43] |
Sakurai K., Kawakami N., Yamaoka K., Ishikawa H., & Hashimoto H.(2010). The impact of subjective and objective social status on psychological distress among men and women in Japan. Social Science and Medicine, 70(11), 1832-1839.
doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2010.01.019 pmid: 20303205 |
[44] |
Sandel M. J.(2018). Populism, liberalism, and democracy. Philosophy and Social Criticism, 44(4), 353-359.
doi: 10.1177/0191453718757888 URL |
[45] |
Sands M. L., & de Kadt D.(2020). Local exposure to inequality raises support of people of low wealth for taxing the wealthy. Nature, 586(7828), 257-261.
doi: 10.1038/s41586-020-2763-1 URL |
[46] |
Tan J. J. X., & Kraus M. W.(2015). Lay theories about social class buffer lower-class individuals against poor self-rated health and negative affect. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 41(3), 446-461.
doi: 10.1177/0146167215569705 URL |
[47] |
Whitson J. A., & Galinsky A. D.(2008). Lacking control increases illusory pattern perception. Science, 322(5898), 115-117.
doi: 10.1126/science.1159845 URL |
[48] |
Whyte M. K., & Han C.(2008). Popular attitudes toward distributive injustice: Beijing and Warsaw compared. Journal of Chinese Political Science, 13(1), 29-51.
doi: 10.1007/s11366-008-9016-8 URL |
[49] | Worthington E. L., & Allison S. T.(2018). Heroic humility: What the science of humility can say to people raised on self-focus. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. |
[50] |
Wright J. C., Nadelhoffer T., Ross L. T., & Sinnott-Armstrong W.(2018). Be it ever so humble: Proposing a dual-dimension account and measurement of humility. Self and Identity, 17(1), 92-125.
doi: 10.1080/15298868.2017.1327454 URL |
[51] |
Xie W., Ho B., Meier S., & Zhou X.(2017). Rank reversal aversion inhibits redistribution across societies. Nature Human Behaviour, 1(8), 0142.
doi: 10.1038/s41562-017-0142 URL |
[52] | Xu J. B., & Liu H.(2013). Social justice recognition, fluidity expectation and the preference of residence redistribution: An empirical study based on CGSS data. Journal of Yunnan University of Finance and Economics, 2, 48-56. |
[ 徐建斌, 刘华.(2013). 社会公平认知、流动性预期与居民再分配偏好——基于CGSS数据的实证研究. 云南财经大学学报 2, 48-56.] | |
[53] | Xu J. B., Liu H., & Yin K. G.(2013). A research review about influence factors of residents’ redistributive preferences. Journal of Social Sciences Abroad, 2, 50-55. |
[ 徐建斌, 刘华, 尹开国.(2013). 居民再分配偏好的影响因素研究述评. 国外社会科学, 2, 50-55.] | |
[54] |
Yang S. L., Guo Y. Y., Hu X. Y., Shu S. L., & Li J.(2016). Do lower class individuals possess higher levels of system justification? An examination from the social cognitive perspectives. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 48(11), 1467-1478.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2016.01467 URL |
[ 杨沈龙, 郭永玉, 胡小勇, 舒首立, 李静.(2016). 低阶层者的系统合理化水平更高吗?——基于社会认知视角的考察. 心理学报, 48(11), 1467-1478.] | |
[55] |
Yu G. L., Zhao F. Q., Wang H., & Li S.(2020). Subjective social class and distrust among Chinese college students: The mediating roles of relative deprivation and belief in a just world. Current Psychology, 39, 2221-2230.
doi: 10.1007/s12144-018-9908-5 URL |
[1] | 孙庆洲, 黄靖茹, 虞晓芬, 高倾德. 授人以鱼还是授人以渔?高、低社会阶层的捐助行为差异[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(10): 1677-1695. |
[2] | 陈思静, 杨莎莎, 汪昊, 万丰华. 主观社会阶层正向预测利他性惩罚[J]. 心理学报, 2022, 54(12): 1548-1561. |
[3] | 韦庆旺, 李木子, 陈晓晨. 社会阶层与社会知觉:热情和能力哪个更重要?[J]. 心理学报, 2018, 50(2): 243-252. |
[4] | 刘圣明, 陈力凡, 王思迈. 满招损, 谦受益:团队沟通视角下谦卑型领导行为对团队创造力的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2018, 50(10): 1159-1168. |
[5] | 杨林川, 马红宇, 姜海, 梁娟, 齐玲. 社会公正对权威合法性的影响: 社会阶层的调节作用[J]. 心理学报, 2017, 49(7): 980-994. |
[6] | 胡小勇;郭永玉;李静;杨沈龙. 社会公平感对不同阶层目标达成的影响及其过程[J]. 心理学报, 2016, 48(3): 271-289. |
[7] | 杨沈龙;郭永玉;胡小勇; 舒首立;李静. 低阶层者的系统合理化水平更高吗? ——基于社会认知视角的考察[J]. 心理学报, 2016, 48(11): 1467-1478. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||