Advances in Psychological Science ›› 2018, Vol. 26 ›› Issue (6): 975-983.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2018.00975
• Regular Articles • Previous Articles Next Articles
Received:
2017-09-26
Online:
2018-06-10
Published:
2018-04-28
Contact:
LI Heng
E-mail:leehem168@163.com
CLC Number:
LI Heng. Implicit space-time mappings on the front and back axis and their influencing factors[J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2018, 26(6): 975-983.
[1] | 江桂英, 李恒 . ( 2011). 概念隐喻研究在神经科学中的新进展——以心理现实性问题为例. 外语教学与研究, 43( 6), 934-941. |
[2] | 李爱梅, 彭元, 熊冠星 . ( 2015). 孕妇更长计远虑?——怀孕对女性跨期决策偏好的影响. 心理学报, 47( 11), 1360-1370. |
[3] | 李恒 . ( 2015). 压力感对汉语使用者时间隐喻理解的影响. 现代外语, 38( 6), 770-778. |
[4] | 李恒 . ( 2016). 时空隐喻的心理现实性:手势和手语的视角. 心理科学, 39( 5), 1080-1085. |
[5] |
李恒, 张积家 . ( 2016). 听觉任务下汉语母语者对不同类型时间句的加工. 心理学报, 48( 6), 617-624.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2016.00617 URL |
[6] | 刘馨元, 张志杰 . ( 2016). 不同时间关注点下的空间-时间联合编码效应. 心理科学, 39( 2), 279-284. |
[7] | 叶浩生 . ( 2010). 具身认知: 认知心理学的新取向. 心理科学进展, 18( 5), 705-710. |
[8] | Barsalou, L. W . ( 1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22( 4), 577-609. |
[9] |
Barsalou, L. W . ( 2016). On staying grounded and avoiding quixotic dead ends. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23( 4), 1122-1142.
doi: 10.3758/s13423-016-1028-3 URL pmid: 27112560 |
[10] |
Bender, A., & Beller, S. (2014). Mapping spatial frames of reference onto time: A review of theoretical accounts and empirical findings. Cognition, 132, 342-382.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.03.016 URL pmid: 24873738 |
[11] |
Boroditsky, L. (2000). Metaphoric structuring: Understanding time through spatial metaphors. Cognition, 75, 1-28.
doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00073-6 URL pmid: 10815775 |
[12] |
Boroditsky, L. (2001). Does language shape thought?: Mandarin and English speakers' conceptions of time. Cognitive Psychology, 43( 1), 1-22.
doi: 10.1006/cogp.2001.0748 URL pmid: 11487292 |
[13] |
Boroditsky, L., & Ramscar, M. (2002). The roles of body and mind in abstract thought. Psychological Science, 13( 2), 185-189.
doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00434 URL pmid: 11934006 |
[14] | Casasanto, D. (2016). Temporal language and temporal thinking may not go hand in hand. In B. Lewandowska- Tomaszczyk (Ed.), Conceptualizations of time( pp. 169- 186). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. |
[15] |
Casasanto, D., & Bottini, R. (2014). Mirror reading can reverse the flow of time. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143( 2), 473-479.
doi: 10.1037/a0033297 URL pmid: 23773158 |
[16] |
Casasanto, D., & Jasmin, K. (2012). The hands of time: Temporal gestures in English speakers. Cognitive Linguistics, 23( 4), 643-674.Chen, J. Y., 427-436.
doi: 10.1515/cog-2012-0020 URL |
[17] |
Chen, J. Y., & O’Seaghdha, P. G . ( 2013). Do Mandarin and English speakers think about time differently? Review of existing evidence and some new data. Journal of Chinese Linguistics, 41, 338-358.
doi: 10.1215/10679847-2144914 URL |
[18] | Cienki, A. J . ( 1998). Metaphoric gestures and some of their relations to verbal metaphoric expressions. In J.-P. Koenig (Ed.), Discourse and cognition: Bridging the gap( pp. 189-204). Stanford, California: Center for the Study of Language and Information. |
[19] | Clark H. H. ( 1973). Space, time, semantics and the child. In T. E. Moore (Ed.), Cognitive development and acquisition of language (pp. 27-63). New York, NY: Academic Press. |
[20] |
de la Fuente J., Santiago J., Román, A, Dumitrache C., & Casasanto D . ( 2014). When you think about it, your past is in front of you: How culture shapes spatial conceptions of time. Psychological Science, 25, 1682-1690.
doi: 10.1177/0956797614534695 URL |
[21] | Duffy, S. E., & Feist, M. I . ( 2014). Individual differences in the interpretation of ambiguous statements about time. Cognitive Linguistics, 25( 1), 29-54. |
[22] |
Eikmeier V., Alex-Ruf S., Maienborn C., & Ulrich R . ( 2015). How strongly linked are mental time and space along the left-right axis? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 41, 1878-1883.
doi: 10.1037/xlm0000129 URL pmid: 25961360 |
[23] | Evans, V. (2004). The structure of time: Language, meaning, and temporal cognition. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. |
[24] |
Fuhrman O., McCormick K., Chen E., Jiang H. D., Shu D. F., Mao S. M., & Boroditsky L . ( 2011). How linguistic and cultural forces shape conceptions of time: English and Mandarin time in 3D. Cognitive Science, 35, 1305-1328.
doi: 10.1111/j.1551-6709.2011.01193.x URL pmid: 21884222 |
[25] | Gibbs R. W.,Jr.( 2017). Metaphor Wars: Conceptual metaphors in human life Cambridge: Cambridge University Press Conceptual metaphors in human life. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. |
[26] | Gu Y., Zheng Y. Q., & Swerts M . ( 2016). Which is in front of Chinese people: Past or Future? A study on Chinese people’s space-time mapping. In A. Papafragou, D. Grodner, D. Mirman, & J. C. Trueswell (Eds.), Proceedings of the 38th annual conference of the cognitive science society(pp. 2603-2608). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society. |
[27] | Haser, V. (2005). Metaphor, metonymy, and experientialist philosophy: Challenging cognitive semantics. Berlin: Walter de Gruyter. |
[28] |
Hendricks, R. K., & Boroditsky, L. (2017). New space-time metaphors foster new nonlinguistic representations. Topics in Cognitive Science, 9, 800-818.
doi: 10.1111/tops.12279 URL pmid: 28635107 |
[29] |
January, D., & Kako, E. (2007). Re-evaluating evidence for linguistic relativity: Reply to Boroditsky (2001). Cognition, 104, 417-426.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2006.07.008 URL pmid: 16914131 |
[30] |
Ji L. J., Guo T. Y., Zhang Z. Y., & Messervey D . ( 2009). Looking into the past: Cultural differences in perception and representation of past information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96, 761-769.
doi: 10.1037/a0014498 URL pmid: 19309200 |
[31] | Lakoff G. , & Johnson, M.( 1980) . Metaphors we live by. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. |
[32] | Laudau, M. J . ( 2016). Conceptual metaphor in social psychology: The poetics of everyday life. New York, NY: Routledge. |
[33] |
Li, H. (2017). Barbara Lewandowska-tomaszczyk: Conceptualizations of time. Cognitive Linguistics, 28( 2), 361-370.
doi: 10.1515/cog-2016-0122 URL |
[34] |
Li, H., & Cao, Y. (2017). Personal attitudes toward time: The relationship between temporal focus, space-time mappings and real life experiences. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 58( 3), 193-198.
doi: 10.1111/sjop.12358 URL pmid: 28266035 |
[35] |
Li, H., & Cao, Y. (2018). The hope of the future: The experience of pregnancy influences women’s implicit space-time mappings. The Journal of Social Psychology, 158, 152-156.
doi: 10.1080/00224545.2017.1297289 URL pmid: 28375781 |
[36] |
McGlone, M. S . ( 2007). What is the explanatory value of a conceptual metaphor?. Language and Communication, 27( 2), 109-126.
doi: 10.1016/j.langcom.2006.02.016 URL |
[37] | McNeill, D. ( 1992). Hand and mind: What gestures reveal about thought. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. |
[38] |
Meier B. P., Schnall S., Schwarz N., & Bargh J. A . ( 2012). Embodiment in social psychology. Topics in Cognitive Science, 4( 4), 705-716.
doi: 10.1111/tops.2012.4.issue-4 URL |
[39] |
Miles L. K., Nind L. K., & Macrae C. N . ( 2010). Moving through time. Psychological Science, 21( 2), 222-223.
doi: 10.1177/0956797609359333 URL |
[40] | Moore K. E. ( 2014). The spatial language of time. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Press. |
[41] |
Murphy, G. L . ( 1996). On metaphoric representation. Cognition, 60( 2), 173-204.
doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(96)00711-1 URL |
[42] |
Núñez, R., & Cooperrider, K. (2013). The tangle of space and time in human cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17( 5), 220-229.
doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.03.008 URL pmid: 23608363 |
[43] |
Núñez, R. E., & Sweetser, E. (2006). With the future behind them: Convergent evidence from Aymara language and gesture in the crosslinguistic comparison of spatial construals of time. Cognitive Science, 30( 3), 401-450.
doi: 10.1207/s15516709cog0000_62 URL pmid: 21702821 |
[44] |
Rinaldi L., Locati F., Parolin L., Bernardi N. F., & Girelli L . ( 2016). Walking on a mental time line: Temporal processing affects step movements along the sagittal space. Cortex, 78, 170-173.
doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2016.02.013 URL pmid: 27049003 |
[45] |
Sell, A. J., & Kaschak, M. P . ( 2011). Processing time shifts affects the execution of motor responses. Brain and Language, 117( 1), 39-44.
doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2010.07.003 URL pmid: 20696469 |
[46] |
Shipp A. J., Edwards J. R., & Lambert L. S . ( 2009). Conceptualization and measurement of temporal focus: The subjective experience of the past, present, and future. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 110( 1), 1-22.
doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2009.05.001 URL |
[47] |
Sinha C., Sinha V. D. S., & Zinken J . ( 2016). When time is not space: The social and linguistic construction of time intervals and temporal event relations in an Amazonian culture. In B. Lewandowska-Tomaszczyk (Ed.), Conceptualizations of time( pp. 151-186). Amsterdam/ Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins.
doi: 10.1075/hcp.52.08sin URL |
[48] |
Sinha C., Sinha V. D. S., Zinken J., & Sampaio W . ( 2011). When time is not space: The social and linguistic construction of time intervals and temporal event relations in an Amazonian culture. Language and Cognition, 3( 1), 137-169.
doi: 10.1515/langcog.2011.006 URL |
[49] |
Stocker K., Hartmann M., Martarelli C. S., & Mast F. W . ( 2016). Eye movements reveal mental looking through time. Cognitive Science, 40( 7), 1648-1670.
doi: 10.1111/cogs.12301 URL |
[50] |
Sullivan, K., & Bui, L. T . ( 2016). With the future coming up behind them: Evidence that time approaches from behind in Vietnamese. Cognitive Linguistics, 27( 2), 205-233.
doi: 10.1515/cog-2015-0066 URL |
[51] |
Torralbo A., Santiago J., & Lupiáñez J . ( 2006). Flexible conceptual projection of time onto spatial frames of reference. Cognitive Science, 30, 745-757.
doi: 10.1207/s15516709cog0000_67 URL pmid: 21702834 |
[52] |
Tse, C. S., & Altarriba, J. (2008). Evidence against linguistic relativity in Chinese and English: A case study of spatial and temporal metaphors. Journal of Cognition & Culture, 8( 3), 335-357.
doi: 10.1163/156853708X358218 URL |
[53] |
Walker, E., & Cooperrider, K. (2016). The Continuity of metaphor: Evidence from temporal gestures. Cognitive Science, 40, 481-495.
doi: 10.1111/cogs.12254 URL pmid: 26059310 |
[54] | Walker, E. J., & Núñez, R. (2017). Speaking, gesturing, reasoning: Methods and issues in the study. In B. Lewandowska-Tomaszczyk (Ed.), Conceptualization of time(pp. 67-84). Amsterdam/ Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins. |
[55] |
Waliński, J. T . ( 2016). Reflection of temporal horizon in linguistic performance. In B. Lewandowska-Tomaszczyk (Ed.), Conceptualization of time(pp. 273-293). Amsterdam/ Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins.
doi: 10.1075/hcp.52.12wal URL |
[56] |
Walker E. J., Bergen B. K., & Núñez R . ( 2017). The spatial alignment of time: Differences in alignment of deictic and sequence time along the sagittal and lateral axes. Acta Psychologica, 175, 13-20.
doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2017.02.001 URL pmid: 28259726 |
[57] | Whorf B. L. (2000). The relation of habitual thought and behavior to language. In J. B. Carroll (Ed.), Language, thought, and reality: Selected writings of Benjamin Lee Whorf (pp. 134-159). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. |
[1] | YANG Xue, ZHU Xu. Impact and application of affective touch on mental health [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2022, 30(12): 2789-2798. |
[2] | JIN Yuwei, SUN Xiao, SONG Yaowu. Embodied memory and its intrinsic mechanism [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2022, 30(11): 2497-2506. |
[3] | BAI Yating, HE Wenguang. The embodied cognition effect of the second language: Automatic activation or native language mediation? [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2021, 29(11): 1970-1978. |
[4] | YU Wenhua, LU Zhongyi. A new perspective on the cognitive function of gestures: The “spatializing” gesture hypothesis [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2020, 28(3): 426-433. |
[5] | XIONG Suhong, SUN Hongjie, LU Ji, YAO Qi, ZHANG Quancheng. Eating behavior from the perspective of embodied cognition: Based on the mental simulation approach [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2020, 28(3): 486-496. |
[6] | LI Ying, ZHANG Can, WANG Yue. The effect of moral emotions on the metaphorical mapping of morality and its neural mechanism [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2019, 27(7): 1224-1231. |
[7] | CHEN Lin, TIAN Xiaoming, DUAN Jinyun. The cognitive mechanism of advice taking [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2019, 27(1): 149-159. |
[8] | LU Hong, ZHANG Xinyuan, WEI Wenqi, LIU Yi. The influence of spatial physical order on psychology and behavior [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2018, 26(3): 560-570. |
[9] | LIU Chuanjun, LIAO Jiangqun. An analytical approach to understanding and solving the replication crisis of the embodiment effect [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2018, 26(12): 2260-2271. |
[10] | GAO Zhihua; LU Zhongyi; CUI Xinying. What is the mechanism of negation processes? Introduction and commentary on psychological theories on negation [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2017, 25(3): 413-423. |
[11] | CHEN Jie, LIU Lei, WANG Rong, SHEN Haizhou. The effect of musical training on executive functions [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2017, 25(11): 1854-1864. |
[12] | SUN Bingli; TIAN Yu; SUN Haiyang; WANG Wenzhong. Self: A Perspective from psychological counseling and therapy [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2016, 24(1): 83-90. |
[13] | YE Hongyan; ZHANG Fenghua. Embodiment in Moral Judgment [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2015, 23(8): 1480-1488. |
[14] | LEE Hui-Min I.; WANG Ruo-Meng; WANG Quan-Hong; QIU Jiang. Handwriting Psychology in the Age of Computers [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2015, 23(10): 1843-1851. |
[15] | Li Jinxing; Wang Zhenhong. Spatial-temporal Association of Response Codes Effect: Manifestation, Influencing Factors and Its Theories [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2015, 23(1): 30-40. |
Viewed | ||||||
Full text |
|
|||||
Abstract |
|
|||||