Advances in Psychological Science ›› 2023, Vol. 31 ›› Issue (5): 866-886.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2023.00866
• Regular Articles • Previous Articles
YANG Lei1, CHEN Weiyang2(), BAI Baoyu1, ZHONG Nian1
Received:
2022-07-06
Online:
2023-05-15
Published:
2023-02-13
Contact:
CHEN Weiyang
E-mail:chenwy@swufe.edu.cn
CLC Number:
YANG Lei, CHEN Weiyang, BAI Baoyu, ZHONG Nian. Experimental evidence for internal mechanisms of cumulative cultural evolution[J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2023, 31(5): 866-886.
[1] |
蔡华俭, 黄梓航, 林莉, 张明杨, 王潇欧, 朱慧珺,... 敬一鸣. (2020). 半个多世纪来中国人的心理与行为变化——心理学视野下的研究. 心理科学进展, 28(10), 1599-1618.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2020.01599 |
[2] |
蔡华俭, 张明杨, 包寒吴霜, 朱慧珺, 杨紫嫣, 程曦,... 王梓西. (2023). 心理学视野下的社会变迁研究:研究设计与分析方法. 心理科学进展, 31(2), 159-172.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2023.00159 |
[3] |
陈维扬, 谢天. (2020). 文化演化的认知视角——从个体社会学习出发探究文化动态性. 心理科学进展, 28(12), 2137-2149.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2020.02137 |
[4] | 韩玉胜. (2021). 儒家伦理传统与儒家道德记忆. 伦理学研究, (5), 41-46. |
[5] |
黄梓航, 敬一鸣, 喻丰, 古若雷, 周欣悦, 张建新, 蔡华俭. (2018). 个人主义上升, 集体主义式微?——全球文化变迁与民众心理变化. 心理科学进展, 26(11), 2068-2080.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2018.02068 |
[6] |
黄梓航, 王俊秀, 苏展, 敬一鸣, 蔡华俭. (2021). 中国社会转型过程中的心理变化:社会学视角的研究及其对心理学家的启示. 心理科学进展, 29(12), 2246-2259.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2021.02246 |
[7] |
姜永志, 白晓丽. (2015). 文化变迁中的价值观发展: 概念、结构与方法. 心理科学进展, 23(5), 888-896.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2015.00888 |
[8] | 刘国芳, 辛自强, 林崇德. (2021). 人际信任中的坏苹果效应及其传递. 心理与行为研究, 15(5), 691-696. |
[9] | 辛自强, 刘国芳. (2012). 文化进化的实验与非实验研究方法. 北京师范大学学报:社会科学版, (3), 5-13. |
[10] | 张积家, 王娟, 肖二平, 和秀梅. (2013). 文化和情境影响亲属词的概念结构. 心理学报, 45(8), 825-839. |
[11] | 周晓虹. (1988). 试论当代中国青年文化的反哺意义. 青年研究, (11), 22-26. |
[12] | 周晓虹. (2000). 文化反哺:变迁社会中的亲子传承. 社会学研究, (2), 51-66. |
[13] |
Acerbi, A., Jacquet, P. O., & Tennie, C. (2012). Behavioral constraints and the evolution of faithful social learning. Current Zoology, 58(2), 307-318.
doi: 10.1093/czoolo/58.2.307 URL |
[14] |
Acerbi, A., Kendal, J., & Tehrani, J. J. (2017). Cultural complexity and demography: The case of folktales. Evolution and Human Behavior, 38(4), 474-480.
doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2017.03.005 URL |
[15] | Allen, J. A., Garland, E. C., Dunlop, R. A., & Noad, M. J. (2018). Cultural revolutions reduce complexity in the songs of humpback whales. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 285(1891). http://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2018.2088 |
[16] |
Baldwin, D. A., & Moses, L. J. (1996). The ontogeny of social information gathering. Child Development, 67(5), 1915-1939.
doi: 10.2307/1131601 URL |
[17] | Bartlett, F. C. (1932). Remembering: A study in experimental and social psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge university press. |
[18] |
Baum, W. M., Richerson, P. J., Efferson, C. M., & Paciotti, B. M. (2004). Cultural evolution in laboratory microsocieties including traditions of rule giving and rule following. Evolution and Human Behavior, 25(5), 305-326.
doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2004.05.003 URL |
[19] |
Bebbington, K., MacLeod, C., Ellison, T. M., & Fay, N. (2017). The sky is falling: Evidence of a negativity bias in the social transmission of information. Evolution and Human Behavior, 38(1), 92-101.
doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.07.004 URL |
[20] |
Blaine, T., & Boyer, P. (2018). Origins of sinister rumors: A preference for threat-related material in the supply and demand of information. Evolution and Human Behavior, 39(1), 67-75.
doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2017.10.001 URL |
[21] |
Blakey, K. H., Rafetseder, E., Atkinson, M., Renner, E., Cowan-Forsythe, F., Sati, S. J., & Caldwell, C. A. (2021). Development of strategic social information seeking: Implications for cumulative culture. Plos One, 16(8), e0256605. http://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0256605
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0256605 URL |
[22] | Bluet, A., Osiurak, F., Claidière, N., & Reynaud, E. (2022). Impact of technical reasoning and theory of mind on cumulative technological culture: Insights from a model of micro-societies. Humanities and Social Sciences Communications, 9(1). http://doi.org/10.1057/s41599-022-01251-z |
[23] | Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1985). Culture and the evolutionary process. Chicago: University of Chicago press. |
[24] | Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1996). Why culture is common, but cultural evolution is rare. Proceedings of the British Academy, 88, 77-93. |
[25] |
Brand, C. O., Acerbi, A., & Mesoudi, A. (2019). Cultural evolution of emotional expression in 50 years of song lyrics. Evolutionary Human Sciences, 1, e11. http://doi.org/10.1017/ehs.2019.11
doi: 10.1017/ehs.2019.11 URL |
[26] |
Brand, C. O., Heap, S., Morgan, T. J. H., & Mesoudi, A. (2020). The emergence and adaptive use of prestige in an online social learning task. Scientific Reports, 10(1), 12095. http://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-68982-4
doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-68982-4 URL pmid: 32694697 |
[27] |
Brand, C. O., Mesoudi, A., & Smaldino, P. E. (2021). Analogy as a catalyst for cumulative cultural evolution. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 25(6), 450-461.
doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2021.03.002 URL |
[28] |
Bromham, L., Hua, X., Fitzpatrick, T. G., & Greenhill, S. J. (2015). Rate of language evolution is affected by population size. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 112(7), 2097-2102.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1419704112 pmid: 25646448 |
[29] | Buskell, A. (2022). Cumulative culture and complex cultural traditions. Mind & Language, 37(3), 284-303. |
[30] |
Caldwell, C. A. (2020). Using experimental research designs to explore the scope of cumulative culture in humans and other animals. Topics in Cognitive Science, 12(2), 673-689.
doi: 10.1111/tops.12391 pmid: 30375756 |
[31] | Caldwell, C. A., Atkinson, M., Blakey, K. H., Dunstone, J., Kean, D., Mackintosh, G.,... Wilks, C. E. H. (2020). Experimental assessment of capacities for cumulative culture: Review and evaluation of methods. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 11(1), e1516. http://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1516 |
[32] |
Caldwell, C. A., Atkinson, M., & Renner, E. (2016). Experimental approaches to studying cumulative cultural evolution. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 25(3), 191-195.
pmid: 27397972 |
[33] |
Caldwell, C. A., & Eve, R. M. (2014). Persistence of contrasting traditions in cultural evolution: Unpredictable payoffs generate slower rates of cultural change. Plos One, 9(6), e99708. http://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0099708
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0099708 URL |
[34] |
Caldwell, C. A., & Millen, A. E. (2008a). Studying cumulative cultural evolution in the laboratory. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 363(1509), 3529-3539.
doi: 10.1098/rstb.2008.0133 URL |
[35] |
Caldwell, C. A., & Millen, A. E. (2008b). Experimental models for testing hypotheses about cumulative cultural evolution. Evolution and Human Behavior, 29(3), 165-171.
doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2007.12.001 URL |
[36] |
Caldwell, C. A., & Millen, A. E. (2009). Social learning mechanisms and cumulative cultural evolution: Is imitation necessary? Psychological Science, 20(12), 1478-1483.
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02469.x URL |
[37] |
Caldwell, C. A., & Millen, A. E. (2010). Conservatism in laboratory microsocieties: Unpredictable payoffs accentuate group-specific traditions. Evolution and Human Behavior, 31(2), 123-130.
doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2009.08.002 URL |
[38] |
Caldwell, C. A., Schillinger, K., Evans, C. L., & Hopper, L. M. (2012). End state copying by humans (Homo sapiens): Implications for a comparative perspective on cumulative culture. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 126(2), 161-169.
doi: 10.1037/a0026828 pmid: 22468937 |
[39] |
Cantor, M., Chimento, M., Smeele, S. Q., He, P., Papageorgiou, D., Aplin, L. M., & Farine, D. R. (2021). Social network architecture and the tempo of cumulative cultural evolution. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 288(1946), 20203107. http://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2020.3107
doi: 10.1098/rspb.2020.3107 URL |
[40] |
Castro, L., & Toro, M. A. (2014). Cumulative cultural evolution: The role of teaching. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 347, 74-83.
doi: 10.1016/j.jtbi.2014.01.006 pmid: 24434740 |
[41] |
Cavalli-Sforza, L., & Feldman, M. W. (1973). Models for cultural inheritance I. Group mean and within group variation. Theoretical Population Biology, 4(1), 42-55.
pmid: 4726009 |
[42] | Cavalli-Sforza, L. L., & Feldman, M. W. (1981). Cultural transmission and evolution: A quantitative approach. Princeton: Princeton University Press. |
[43] |
Claidière, N., Amedon, G. K., André, J., Kirby, S., Smith, K., Sperber, D., & Fagot, J. (2018). Convergent transformation and selection in cultural evolution. Evolution and Human Behavior, 39(2), 191-202.
doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2017.12.007 URL |
[44] | Creanza, N., Kolodny, O., & Feldman, M. W. (2017a). Cultural evolutionary theory: How culture evolves and why it matters. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 114(30), 7782-7789. |
[45] |
Creanza, N., Kolodny, O., & Feldman, M. W. (2017b). Greater than the sum of its parts? Modelling population contact and interaction of cultural repertoires. Journal of the Royal Society Interface, 14(130), 20170171. http://doi.org/10.1098/rsif.2017.0171
doi: 10.1098/rsif.2017.0171 URL |
[46] |
Csibra, G., & Gergely, G. (2011). Natural pedagogy as evolutionary adaptation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 366(1567), 1149-1157.
doi: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0319 URL |
[47] |
Davis, S., Rawlings, B., Clegg, J. M., Ikejimba, D., Watson-Jones, R. E., Whiten, A., & Legare, C. H. (2022). Cognitive flexibility supports the development of cumulative cultural learning in children. Scientific Reports, 12(1), 14073. http://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-18231-7
doi: 10.1038/s41598-022-18231-7 URL pmid: 35982124 |
[48] | Dawkins, R. (1976). The selfish gene. New York: Oxford University Press. |
[49] |
Dean, L. G., Kendal, R. L., Schapiro, S. J., Thierry, B., & Laland, K. N. (2012). Identification of the social and cognitive processes underlying human cumulative culture. Science, 335(6072), 1114-1118.
doi: 10.1126/science.1213969 pmid: 22383851 |
[50] |
Dean, L. G., Vale, G. L., Laland, K. N., Flynn, E., & Kendal, R. L. (2014). Human cumulative culture: A comparative perspective. Biological Reviews, 89(2), 284-301.
doi: 10.1111/brv.2014.89.issue-2 URL |
[51] |
de Oliveira, E., Reynaud, E., & Osiurak, F. (2019). Roles of technical reasoning, theory of mind, creativity, and fluid cognition in cumulative technological culture. Human Nature, 30(3), 326-340.
doi: 10.1007/s12110-019-09349-1 |
[52] |
Derex, M., Beugin, M., Godelle, B., & Raymond, M. (2013). Experimental evidence for the influence of group size on cultural complexity. Nature, 503(7476), 389-391.
doi: 10.1038/nature12774 |
[53] |
Derex, M., Bonnefon, J., Boyd, R., & Mesoudi, A. (2019). Causal understanding is not necessary for the improvement of culturally evolving technology. Nature Human Behaviour, 3(5), 446-452.
doi: 10.1038/s41562-019-0567-9 pmid: 30936426 |
[54] |
Derex, M., & Boyd, R. (2015). The foundations of the human cultural niche. Nature Communications, 6(1), 8398. http://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms9398
doi: 10.1038/ncomms9398 URL |
[55] |
Derex, M., & Boyd, R. (2016). Partial connectivity increases cultural accumulation within groups. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 113(11), 2982-2987.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1518798113 pmid: 26929364 |
[56] |
Derex, M., Godelle, B., & Raymond, M. (2013). Social learners require process information to outperform individual learners. Evolution, 67(3), 688-697.
doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2012.01804.x pmid: 23461320 |
[57] |
Derex, M., & Mesoudi, A. (2020). Cumulative cultural evolution within evolving population structures. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 24(8), 654-667.
doi: S1364-6613(20)30107-8 pmid: 32466991 |
[58] |
Derex, M., Perreault, C., & Boyd, R. (2018). Divide and conquer: Intermediate levels of population fragmentation maximize cultural accumulation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 373(1743), 20170062. http://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2017.0062
doi: 10.1098/rstb.2017.0062 URL |
[59] |
Deutsch, M., & Gerard, H. B. (1955). A study of normative and informational social influences upon individual judgment. The journal of abnormal and social psychology, 51(3), 629-636.
doi: 10.1037/h0046408 URL |
[60] |
Dixson, B. J. W., Rantala, M. J., Melo, E. F., & Brooks, R. C. (2017). Beards and the big city: Displays of masculinity may be amplified under crowded conditions. Evolution and Human Behavior, 38(2), 259-264.
doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.10.009 URL |
[61] |
Dunstone, J., & Caldwell, C. A. (2018). Cumulative culture and explicit metacognition: A review of theories, evidence and key predictions. Palgrave Communications, 4(1), 1-11.
doi: 10.1057/s41599-017-0055-7 |
[62] |
Eriksson, K., & Coultas, J. C. (2014). Corpses, maggots, poodles and rats: Emotional selection operating in three phases of cultural transmission of urban legends. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 14(1-2), 1-26.
doi: 10.1163/15685373-12342107 URL |
[63] | Evans, C. (2016). Empirical investigations of social learning, cooperation, and their role in the evolution of complex culture (Unpublished doctorial dissertation). University of St Andrews. |
[64] |
Fay, N., de Kleine, N., Walker, B., & Caldwell, C. A. (2019). Increasing population size can inhibit cumulative cultural evolution. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 116(14), 6726-6731.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1811413116 pmid: 30872484 |
[65] |
Fay, N., Ellison, T. M., Tylén, K., Fusaroli, R., Walker, B., & Garrod, S. (2018). Applying the cultural ratchet to a social artefact: The cumulative cultural evolution of a language game. Evolution and Human Behavior, 39(3), 300-309.
doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2018.02.002 URL |
[66] |
Fogarty, L. (2018). Cultural complexity and evolution in fluctuating environments. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 373(1743), 20170063. http://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2017.0063
doi: 10.1098/rstb.2017.0063 URL |
[67] |
Fogarty, L., & Creanza, N. (2017). The niche construction of cultural complexity: Interactions between innovations, population size and the environment. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 372(1735), 20160428. http://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2016.0428
doi: 10.1098/rstb.2016.0428 URL |
[68] |
Fogarty, L., Strimling, P., & Laland, K. N. (2011). The evolution of teaching. Evolution: International Journal of Organic Evolution, 65(10), 2760-2770.
doi: 10.1111/evo.2011.65.issue-10 URL |
[69] | Galef Jr, B. G. (1988). Imitation in animals:History, definition, and interpretation of data from the psychological laboratory. In T. R.Zentall & B. G.Galef (Eds.), Social learning: Psychological and biological perspectives (pp. 3-28). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. |
[70] |
Garland, E. C., Garrigue, C., & Noad, M. J. (2022). When does cultural evolution become cumulative culture? A case study of humpback whale song. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 377(1843), 20200313. http://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2020.0313
doi: 10.1098/rstb.2020.0313 URL |
[71] |
Goldstein, T. R., & Winner, E. (2012). Enhancing empathy and theory of mind. Journal of Cognition and Development, 13(1), 19-37.
doi: 10.1080/15248372.2011.573514 URL |
[72] |
Gopnik, A., O'Grady, S., Lucas, C. G., Griffiths, T. L., Wente, A., Bridgers, S.,... Dahl, R. E. (2017). Changes in cognitive flexibility and hypothesis search across human life history from childhood to adolescence to adulthood. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 114(30), 7892-7899.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1700811114 pmid: 28739917 |
[73] |
Greenfield, P. M. (2013). The changing psychology of culture from 1800 through 2000. Psychological Science, 24(9), 1722-1731.
doi: 10.1177/0956797613479387 pmid: 23925305 |
[74] |
Harrison, R. A., & Whiten, A. (2018). Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) display limited behavioural flexibility when faced with a changing foraging task requiring tool use. Peerj, 6, e4366. http://doi.org/10.7717/peerj.4366
doi: 10.7717/peerj.4366 URL |
[75] | Henrich, J. (2015). The secret of our success: How culture is driving human evolution, domesticating our species, and making us smarter. Princeton: Princeton University Press. |
[76] |
Hewlett, B. S., Fouts, H. N., Boyette, A. H., & Hewlett, B. L. (2011). Social learning among Congo Basin hunter-gatherers. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 366(1567), 1168-1178.
doi: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0373 URL |
[77] |
Heyes, C. (2016). Who Knows? Metacognitive social learning strategies. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(3), 204-213.
doi: S1364-6613(15)00312-5 pmid: 26778808 |
[78] |
Heyes, C. (2019). Précis of cognitive gadgets: The cultural evolution of thinking. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 42, e169. http://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X18002145
doi: 10.1017/S0140525X18002145 URL |
[79] |
Heyes, C. M. (1993). Imitation, culture and cognition. Animal Behaviour, 46(5), 999-1010.
doi: 10.1006/anbe.1993.1281 URL |
[80] |
Hutchison, J., Cunningham, S. J., Slessor, G., Urquhart, J., Smith, K., & Martin, D. (2018). Context and perceptual salience influence the formation of novel stereotypes via cumulative cultural evolution. Cognitive Science, 42(S1), 186-212.
doi: 10.1111/cogs.12560 URL |
[81] |
Jacobs, R. C., & Campbell, D. T. (1961). The perpetuation of an arbitrary tradition through several generations of a laboratory microculture. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 62(3), 649-658.
doi: 10.1037/h0044182 URL |
[82] | Jiménez, Á. V., & Mesoudi, A. (2019). Prestige and dominance: A review of the dual evolutionary model of social hierarchy. Preprint at: http://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/sh7mg |
[83] |
Jiménez, Á. V., & Mesoudi, A. (2021). The Cultural transmission of prestige and dominance social rank cues: An experimental simulation. Evolutionary Psychological Science, 7(2), 189-199.
doi: 10.1007/s40806-020-00261-x |
[84] |
Kane, A. A., Argote, L., & Levine, J. M. (2005). Knowledge transfer between groups via personnel rotation: Effects of social identity and knowledge quality. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 96(1), 56-71.
doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2004.09.002 URL |
[85] |
Kendal, R. L., Boogert, N. J., Rendell, L., Laland, K. N., Webster, M., & Jones, P. L. (2018). Social learning strategies: Bridge-building between fields. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22(7), 651-665.
doi: S1364-6613(18)30094-9 pmid: 29759889 |
[86] | Kendal, R. L., Coolen, I., van Bergen, Y., & Laland, K. N. (2005). Trade-offs in the adaptive use of social and asocial learning. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 35, 333-379. |
[87] |
Kirby, S., & Tamariz, M. (2022). Cumulative cultural evolution, population structure and the origin of combinatoriality in human language. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 377(1843), 20200319. http://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2020.0319
doi: 10.1098/rstb.2020.0319 URL |
[88] |
Kirby, S., Tamariz, M., Cornish, H., & Smith, K. (2015). Compression and communication in the cultural evolution of linguistic structure. Cognition, 141, 87-102.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2015.03.016 pmid: 25966840 |
[89] | Kline, M. A. (2015). How to learn about teaching: An evolutionary framework for the study of teaching behavior in humans and other animals. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 38. http://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X14000090 |
[90] |
Legare, C. H. (2019). The development of cumulative cultural learning. Annual Review of Developmental Psychology, 1(1), 119-147.
doi: 10.1146/devpsych.2019.1.issue-1 URL |
[91] |
Legare, C. H., & Nielsen, M. (2015). Imitation and innovation: The dual engines of cultural learning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19(11), 688-699.
doi: S1364-6613(15)00182-5 pmid: 26440121 |
[92] |
Lehner, S. R., Burkart, J. M., & van Schaik, C. P. (2011). Can captive orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus abelii) be coaxed into cumulative build-up of techniques? Journal of Comparative Psychology, 125(4), 446-455.
doi: 10.1037/a0024413 pmid: 21767010 |
[93] |
Lewis, H. M., & Laland, K. N. (2012). Transmission fidelity is the key to the build-up of cumulative culture. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 367(1599), 2171-2180.
doi: 10.1098/rstb.2012.0119 URL |
[94] | Lindenfors, P., Envall, I., Isaksson, S., & Enquist, M. (2015). An empirical study of cultural evolution: The development of European cooking from medieval to modern times. Cliodynamics, 6(2), 115-129. |
[95] |
Lucas, A. J., Kings, M., Whittle, D., Davey, E., Happé, F., Caldwell, C. A., & Thornton, A. (2020). The value of teaching increases with tool complexity in cumulative cultural evolution. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 287(1939), 20201885. http://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2020.1885
doi: 10.1098/rspb.2020.1885 URL |
[96] | Mackintosh, G. (2020). The role of mental state understanding in distinctively human cumulative cultural evolution (Unpublished doctorial dissertation). University of Stirling. |
[97] |
Martin, D., Hutchison, J., Slessor, G., Urquhart, J., Cunningham, S. J., & Smith, K. (2014). The spontaneous formation of stereotypes via cumulative cultural evolution. Psychological Science, 25(9), 1777-1786.
doi: 10.1177/0956797614541129 pmid: 25052829 |
[98] |
McElreath, R., Lubell, M., Richerson, P. J., Waring, T. M., Baum, W., Edsten, E.,... Paciotti, B. (2005). Applying evolutionary models to the laboratory study of social learning. Evolution and Human Behavior, 26(6), 483-508.
doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2005.04.003 URL |
[99] | Mead, M. (1970). Culture and commitment: A study of the generation gap. Garden City, NY: Natural History Press. |
[100] |
Mesoudi, A. (2008). An experimental simulation of the “copy-successful-individuals” cultural learning strategy: Adaptive landscapes, producer-scrounger dynamics, and informational access costs. Evolution and Human Behavior, 29(5), 350-363.
doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2008.04.005 URL |
[101] |
Mesoudi, A. (2016). Cultural evolution: A review of theory, findings and controversies. Evolutionary Biology, 43(4), 481-497.
doi: 10.1007/s11692-015-9320-0 URL |
[102] |
Mesoudi, A. (2017). Pursuing Darwin’s curious parallel: Prospects for a science of cultural evolution. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 114(30), 7853-7860.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1620741114 pmid: 28739929 |
[103] |
Mesoudi, A., & O'Brien, M. J. (2008). The cultural transmission of Great Basin projectile-point technology I: An experimental simulation. American Antiquity, 73(1), 3-28.
doi: 10.1017/S0002731600041263 URL |
[104] |
Mesoudi, A., & Thornton, A. (2018). What is cumulative cultural evolution? Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 285(1880), 20180712. http://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2018.0712
doi: 10.1098/rspb.2018.0712 URL |
[105] |
Mesoudi, A., & Whiten, A. (2008). The multiple roles of cultural transmission experiments in understanding human cultural evolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 363(1509), 3489-3501.
doi: 10.1098/rstb.2008.0129 URL |
[106] |
Migliano, A. B., Battiston, F., Viguier, S., Page, A. E., Dyble, M., Schlaepfer, R.,... Vinicius, L. (2020). Hunter-gatherer multilevel sociality accelerates cumulative cultural evolution. Science Advances, 6(9), eaax5913. http://doi.org/10.1126/sciadv.aax5913
doi: 10.1126/sciadv.aax5913 URL |
[107] |
Miu, E., Gulley, N., Laland, K. N., & Rendell, L. (2020). Flexible learning, rather than inveterate innovation or copying, drives cumulative knowledge gain. Science Advances, 6(23), eaaz0286. http://doi.org/doi:10.1126/sciadv.aaz0286
doi: http://doi.org/doi:10.1126/sciadv.aaz0286 URL |
[108] |
Moffett, M. W. (2013). Human identity and the evolution of societies. Human Nature, 24(3), 219-267.
doi: 10.1007/s12110-013-9170-3 URL |
[109] | Morgan, T. J. H., Uomini, N. T., Rendell, L. E., Chouinard-Thuly, L., Street, S. E., Lewis, H. M.,... Laland, K. N. (2015). Experimental evidence for the co-evolution of hominin tool-making teaching and language. Nature Communications, 6(1). http://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms7029 |
[110] |
Morin, O., & Acerbi, A. (2017). Birth of the cool: A two-centuries decline in emotional expression in Anglophone fiction. Cognition and Emotion, 31(8), 1663-1675.
doi: 10.1080/02699931.2016.1260528 pmid: 27910735 |
[111] | Muthukrishna, M., & Henrich, J. (2016). Innovation in the collective brain. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 371(1690). http://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2015.0192 |
[112] | Muthukrishna, M., Shulman, B. W., Vasilescu, V., & Henrich, J. (2014). Sociality influences cultural complexity. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 281(1774). http://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2013.2511 |
[113] |
Navarro, D. J., Perfors, A., Kary, A., Brown, S. D., & Donkin, C. (2018). When extremists win: Cultural transmission via iterated learning when populations are heterogeneous. Cognitive Science, 42(7), 2108-2149.
doi: 10.1111/cogs.12667 pmid: 30062733 |
[114] |
Neiman, F. D. (1995). Stylistic variation in evolutionary perspective: Inferences from decorative diversity and interassemblage distance in Illinois Woodland ceramic assemblages. American Antiquity, 60(1), 7-36.
doi: 10.2307/282074 URL |
[115] | Nelson, M., Hegmon, M., Kulow, S., Peeples, M., Kintigh, K., & Kinzig, A. (2011). Resisting diversity: A long-term archaeological study. Ecology and Society, 16(1). http://doi.org/10.5751/ES-03887-160125 |
[116] |
Norenzayan, A., Shariff, A. F., Gervais, W. M., Willard, A. K., McNamara, R. A., Slingerland, E., & Henrich, J. (2016). The cultural evolution of prosocial religions. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 39, e1. http://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X14001356
doi: 10.1017/S0140525X14001356 URL |
[117] |
Osiurak, F., Cretel, C., Duhau-Marmon, N., Fournier, I., Marignier, L., de Oliveira, E.,... Reynaud, E. (2020). The pedagogue, the engineer, and the friend: From Whom Do We Learn? Human Nature, 31(4), 462-482.
doi: 10.1007/s12110-020-09379-0 |
[118] |
Osiurak, F., de Oliveira, E., Navarro, J., Lesourd, M., Claidière, N., & Reynaud, E. (2016). Physical intelligence does matter to cumulative technological culture. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145(8), 941-984.
doi: 10.1037/xge0000189 URL |
[119] |
Osiurak, F., de Oliveira, E., Navarro, J., & Reynaud, E. (2020). The castaway island: Distinct roles of theory of mind and technical reasoning in cumulative technological culture. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 149(1), 58-66.
doi: 10.1037/xge0000614 URL |
[120] |
Osiurak, F., Lasserre, S., Arbanti, J., Brogniart, J., Bluet, A., Navarro, J., & Reynaud, E. (2021). Technical reasoning is important for cumulative technological culture. Nature Human Behaviour, 5(12), 1643-1651.
doi: 10.1038/s41562-021-01159-9 pmid: 34239080 |
[121] |
Osiurak, F., & Reynaud, E. (2020). The elephant in the room: What matters cognitively in cumulative technological culture. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 43, e156. http://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X19003236
doi: 10.1017/S0140525X19003236 URL |
[122] |
Perry, S., Carter, A., Smolla, M., Akçay, E., Nöbel, S., Foster, J. G., & Healy, S. D. (2021). Not by transmission alone: The role of invention in cultural evolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 376(1828), 20200049. http://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2020.0049
doi: 10.1098/rstb.2020.0049 URL |
[123] | Ravignani, A., Delgado, T., & Kirby, S. (2016). Musical evolution in the lab exhibits rhythmic universals. Nature Human Behaviour, 1(1). http://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-016-0007 |
[124] |
Reindl, E., Gwilliams, A. L., Dean, L. G., Kendal, R. L., & Tennie, C. (2020). Skills and motivations underlying children’s cumulative cultural learning: Case not closed. Palgrave Communications, 6(1), 106.
doi: 10.1057/s41599-020-0483-7 |
[125] | Richerson, P. J., & Boyd, R. (2005). Not by genes alone: How culture transformed human evolution. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. |
[126] |
Saldana, C., Fagot, J., Kirby, S., Smith, K., & Claidière, N. (2019). High-fidelity copying is not necessarily the key to cumulative cultural evolution: A study in monkeys and children. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 286(1904), 20190729. http://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2019.0729
doi: 10.1098/rspb.2019.0729 URL |
[127] |
Schimmelpfennig, R., Razek, L., Schnell, E., & Muthukrishna, M. (2022). Paradox of diversity in the collective brain. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 377(1843), 20200316. http://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2020.0316
doi: 10.1098/rstb.2020.0316 URL |
[128] |
Scott, I. M., Clark, A. P., Josephson, S. C., Boyette, A. H., Cuthill, I. C., Fried, R. L.,... Penton-Voak, I. S. (2014). Human preferences for sexually dimorphic faces may be evolutionarily novel. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 111(40), 14388-14393. http://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1409643111
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1409643111 URL pmid: 25246593 |
[129] |
Scott-Phillips, T. C. (2017). A (simple) experimental demonstration that cultural evolution is not replicative, but reconstructive—and an explanation of why this difference matters. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 17(1-2), 1-11.
doi: 10.1163/15685373-12342188 URL |
[130] |
Sinclair, N. C., Ursell, J., South, A., & Rendell, L. (2022). From Beethoven to Beyoncé: Do changing aesthetic cultures amount to “cumulative cultural evolution?”. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 663397. http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.663397
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.663397 URL |
[131] |
Smaldino, P. E. (2019). Social identity and cooperation in cultural evolution. Behavioural Processes, 161, 108-116.
doi: S0376-6357(17)30327-3 pmid: 29223462 |
[132] |
Tamariz, M., & Kirby, S. (2016). The cultural evolution of language. Current Opinion in Psychology, 8, 37-43.
doi: S2352-250X(15)00222-5 pmid: 29506800 |
[133] |
Tan, R., & Fay, N. (2011). Cultural transmission in the laboratory: Agent interaction improves the intergenerational transfer of information. Evolution and Human Behavior, 32(6), 399-406.
doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2011.01.001 URL |
[134] |
Tennie, C., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Ratcheting up the ratchet: On the evolution of cumulative culture. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364(1528), 2405-2415.
doi: 10.1098/rstb.2009.0052 URL |
[135] |
Thompson, B., van Opheusden, B., Sumers, T., & Griffiths, T. L. (2022). Complex cognitive algorithms preserved by selective social learning in experimental populations. Science, 376(6588), 95-98.
doi: 10.1126/science.abn0915 pmid: 35357938 |
[136] | Tomasello, M. (1999). The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard university press. |
[137] |
Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., & Moll, H. (2005). Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28(5), 675-691.
doi: 10.1017/S0140525X05000129 pmid: 16262930 |
[138] |
Tomasello, M., Kruger, A. C., & Ratner, H. H. (1993). Cultural learning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16(3), 495-511.
doi: 10.1017/S0140525X0003123X URL |
[139] | Tomasello, M., & Moll, H. (2010). The gap is social:Human shared intentionality and culture. In P.Kappeler & J. B.Silk (Eds.), Mind the gap (pp. 331-349). Berlin/Heidelberg: Springer Verlag. |
[140] | Tong, Y., Wang, F., & Danovitch, J. (2020). The role of epistemic and social characteristics in children’s selective trust: Three meta-analyses. Developmental Science, 23(2), e12895. http://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12895 |
[141] |
Vaesen, K., & Houkes, W. (2021). Is human culture cumulative? Current Anthropology, 62(2), 218-238.
doi: 10.1086/714032 URL |
[142] |
Varnum, M. E. W., & Grossmann, I. (2017). Cultural change: The how and the why. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 12(6), 956-972.
doi: 10.1177/1745691617699971 pmid: 28915361 |
[143] |
Varnum, M. E. W., & Grossmann, I. (2021). The psychology of cultural change: Introduction to the special issue. The American Psychologist, 76(6), 833-837.
doi: 10.1037/amp0000898 URL |
[144] |
Wales, N. (2012). Modeling neanderthal clothing using ethnographic analogues. Journal of Human Evolution, 63(6), 781-795.
doi: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2012.08.006 pmid: 23084621 |
[145] |
Wasielewski, H. (2014). Imitation is necessary for cumulative cultural evolution in an unfamiliar, opaque task. Human Nature, 25(1), 161-179.
doi: 10.1007/s12110-014-9192-5 URL |
[146] |
Whiten, A. (2017). A second inheritance system: The extension of biology through culture. Interface Focus, 7(5), 20160142. http://doi.org/10.1098/rsfs.2016.0142
doi: 10.1098/rsfs.2016.0142 URL |
[147] | Whiten, A., Biro, D., Bredeche, N., Garland, E. C., & Kirby, S. (2022). The emergence of collective knowledge and cumulative culture in animals, humans and machines. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 377(1843). http://doi.org/10.1098/rsfs.2016.0142 |
[148] |
Wilks, C. E. H., Rafetseder, E., Renner, E., Atkinson, M., & Caldwell, C. A. (2021). Cognitive prerequisites for cumulative culture are context-dependent: Children’s potential for ratcheting depends on cue longevity. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 204, 105031. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2020.105031
doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2020.105031 URL |
[149] |
Winterhalder, B., & Smith, E. A. (2000). Analyzing adaptive strategies: Human behavioral ecology at twenty-five. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 9(2), 51-72.
doi: 10.1002/(ISSN)1520-6505 URL |
[150] |
Zwirner, E., & Thornton, A. (2015). Cognitive requirements of cumulative culture: Teaching is useful but not essential. Scientific Reports, 5(1), 16781. http://doi.org/10.1038/srep16781
doi: 10.1038/srep16781 URL |
[1] | ZHOU Kong, ZHOU Xuan, YING Xueqing. From idea generation to idea implementation: The effects of team pay-for-performance on team innovation [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2023, 31(6): 944-957. |
[2] | XIAO Sufang, XU Zhengli. Conceptualization of voice-taking and its effect on work behaviors: From the perspective of regulatory focus theory [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2023, 31(5): 697-708. |
[3] | CHENG Meixia, KUANG Ziyi, LENG Xiaoxue, ZHANG Yang, WANG Fuxing. Can learning by non-interactive teaching promote learning? [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2023, 31(5): 769-782. |
[4] | WU Michael Shengtao, MAO Yunyun, WU Shuhan, FENG Jianren, ZHANG Qingpeng, XIE Tian, CHEN Hao, ZHU Tingshao. The big data analysis in cultural psychology [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2023, 31(3): 317-329. |
[5] | YANG Mengxi, LIN Yuying, CHEN Wansi, CHEN Xuan, BAO Hongying, LI Xinyu. Double-edged sword effect of supervisor bottom-line mentality on team innovation [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2023, 31(3): 361-370. |
[6] | CAI Huajian, ZHANG Mingyang, BAO Han-Wu-Shuang, ZHU Huijun, YANG Ziyan, CHENG Xi, HUANG Zihang, WANG Zixi. Examining societal change from the perspective of psychology: Research design and analytic techniques [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2023, 31(2): 159-172. |
[7] | LIU Wei, SHEN Xiaoling. A dynamic perspective on the relationship between team reflection-in-action and innovation: A moderated mediation model integrating cognition and emotion [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2022, 30(8): 1759-1769. |
[8] | WANG Jiayan, LAN Yuanmei, LI Chaoping. Challenge-hindrance stressors and innovation: A meta-analysis [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2022, 30(4): 761-780. |
[9] | SHI Huiying, TANG Jie, LIU Pingping. Instability of the watching eyes effect and perceived norms: A new perspective [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2022, 30(12): 2718-2734. |
[10] | HUANG Zi-Hang, WANG Jun-Xiu, SU Zhan, JING Yi-Ming, CAI Hua-Jian. Cultural and psychological changes during the social transformation in China: Sociological research and its enlightenments to psychologists [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2021, 29(12): 2246-2259. |
[11] | ZHAO Kai, XIANG Shuting. How to reconcile team innovation paradox? An explorative study from the perspectives of members’ cognitive style “composition” and “configuration” [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2021, 29(1): 1-18. |
[12] | CHEN Weiyang, XIE Tian. The cognitive perspective of cultural evolution: Exploring cultural dynamics from the view of social learning [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2020, 28(12): 2137-2149. |
[13] | CAI Huajian, HUANG Zihang, LIN Li, ZHANG Mingyang, WANG Xiaoou, ZHU Huijun, XIE Yiping, YANG Ying, YANG Ziyang, JING Yiming. The psychological change of the Chinese people over the past half century: A literature review [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2020, 28(10): 1599-1688. |
[14] | LI Qian, GONG Shiyang, LI Chaofan. The impact of team cultural diversity on team innovation [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2019, 27(9): 1521-1539. |
[15] | WANG Hongyu, CUI Zhisong, ZOU Chunlong, YU Jiali, ZHAO Di. Loyal or rebel? Employee bootleg innovation in Chinese context [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2019, 27(6): 975-989. |
Viewed | ||||||
Full text |
|
|||||
Abstract |
|
|||||