Advances in Psychological Science ›› 2022, Vol. 30 ›› Issue (5): 1106-1118.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2022.01106
• Regular Articles • Previous Articles Next Articles
Received:
2021-07-30
Online:
2022-05-15
Published:
2022-03-24
Contact:
BI Cuihua
E-mail:cuihuabi1215@163.com
BI Cuihua, QI Huaiyuan. The role of time perception in intertemporal decision-making: New exploration of time decision-making model[J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2022, 30(5): 1106-1118.
模型名称 与提出者 | 时间感知与客观时间的构建 | 整体模型 | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
公式 | 公式解释 | 公式 | 公式解释 | |
对数/指数时间 折扣模型 Takahashi 等人(2005) Takahashi 等人(2008) | | 时间感知与客观时距呈对数关系; T为对延迟时间的时间长度感知; a为自由参数, b是与自我控制有关参数; D为延迟时距, c自由参数; s为冲动性参数。D为延迟时距。 | | SV为折扣后收益的主观价值; A为延迟收益的客观价值; k为延迟折扣; e为自然对数底。 |
感知时间 基础模型 Kim和 Zauberman (2009) | | 时间感知与客观时距呈指数关系; T为对延迟时间的时间长度感知; α为时间收缩程度, 即感知值与客观值之比; β时间敏感度, 反映个体差异; D为延迟时距。 | | SV为折扣后收益的主观价值; A为延迟收益的客观价值; k为延迟折扣; e为自然对数底。 |
多模态漂移 扩散模型 Amasino 等人(2019) | | 时间感知是左侧和右侧选项的时间差与注意程度的函数。 | & \text{S}{{\text{V}}_{t}}= \\ & \ \ \ \ \text{S}{{\text{V}}_{t-1}}+\frac{{{\text{ }\!\!\delta\!\!\text{ }}_{A}}{{\text{A}}_{left}}}{1+{{\text{ }\!\!\delta\!\!\text{ }}_{T}}{{\text{T}}_{left}}}-\frac{{{\text{ }\!\!\delta\!\!\text{ }}_{A}}{{\text{A}}_{right}}}{1+{{\text{ }\!\!\delta\!\!\text{ }}_{T}}{{\text{T}}_{tight}}} \\ \end{align}$ & \text{S}{{\text{V}}_{t}}=\text{S}{{\text{V}}_{t-1}}+\text{ }\!\!\delta\!\!\text{ }{{\text{ }\!\!\grave{\ }\!\!\text{ }}_{A}}\left( {{\text{A}}_{left}}-{{\text{A}}_{tight}} \right)- \\ & \ \ \ \ \ \ \ \ \text{ }\!\!\delta\!\!\text{ }{{\text{ }\!\!\grave{\ }\!\!\text{ }}_{T}}\left( {{\text{T}}_{left}}-{{\text{T}}_{tight}} \right) \\ \end{align}$ | SV为折扣后收益的主观价值; A为延迟收益的客观价值; k为延迟折扣; |
模型名称 与提出者 | 时间感知与客观时间的构建 | 整体模型 | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
公式 | 公式解释 | 公式 | 公式解释 | |
对数/指数时间 折扣模型 Takahashi 等人(2005) Takahashi 等人(2008) | | 时间感知与客观时距呈对数关系; T为对延迟时间的时间长度感知; a为自由参数, b是与自我控制有关参数; D为延迟时距, c自由参数; s为冲动性参数。D为延迟时距。 | | SV为折扣后收益的主观价值; A为延迟收益的客观价值; k为延迟折扣; e为自然对数底。 |
感知时间 基础模型 Kim和 Zauberman (2009) | | 时间感知与客观时距呈指数关系; T为对延迟时间的时间长度感知; α为时间收缩程度, 即感知值与客观值之比; β时间敏感度, 反映个体差异; D为延迟时距。 | | SV为折扣后收益的主观价值; A为延迟收益的客观价值; k为延迟折扣; e为自然对数底。 |
多模态漂移 扩散模型 Amasino 等人(2019) | | 时间感知是左侧和右侧选项的时间差与注意程度的函数。 | & \text{S}{{\text{V}}_{t}}= \\ & \ \ \ \ \text{S}{{\text{V}}_{t-1}}+\frac{{{\text{ }\!\!\delta\!\!\text{ }}_{A}}{{\text{A}}_{left}}}{1+{{\text{ }\!\!\delta\!\!\text{ }}_{T}}{{\text{T}}_{left}}}-\frac{{{\text{ }\!\!\delta\!\!\text{ }}_{A}}{{\text{A}}_{right}}}{1+{{\text{ }\!\!\delta\!\!\text{ }}_{T}}{{\text{T}}_{tight}}} \\ \end{align}$ & \text{S}{{\text{V}}_{t}}=\text{S}{{\text{V}}_{t-1}}+\text{ }\!\!\delta\!\!\text{ }{{\text{ }\!\!\grave{\ }\!\!\text{ }}_{A}}\left( {{\text{A}}_{left}}-{{\text{A}}_{tight}} \right)- \\ & \ \ \ \ \ \ \ \ \text{ }\!\!\delta\!\!\text{ }{{\text{ }\!\!\grave{\ }\!\!\text{ }}_{T}}\left( {{\text{T}}_{left}}-{{\text{T}}_{tight}} \right) \\ \end{align}$ | SV为折扣后收益的主观价值; A为延迟收益的客观价值; k为延迟折扣; |
[1] | 黄希庭.(1993). 时距信息加工的认知研究. 西南师范大学学报(自然科学版), (2), 207-215. |
[2] | 黄希庭, 张志杰, 凤四海, 郭秀艳, 吕厚超, 陈莹.(2005). 时间心理学的新探索. 心理科学, (6), 6-9+15. |
[3] | 江程铭, 刘洪志, 蔡晓红, 李纾.(2016). 跨期选择单维占优模型的过程检验. 心理学报, 48(1), 59-72. |
[4] | 焦长勇, 赵雷, 何铨, 胡凤培, 江程铭.(2015). 跨期选择中的跨时间周期效应. 应用心理学, 21(2), 124-130. |
[5] | 李爱梅, 孙海龙, 熊冠星, 王笑天, 李斌.(2016). “时间贫穷”对跨期决策和前瞻行为的影响及其认知机制. 心理科学进展, 24(6), 874-884. |
[6] | 李爱梅, 王海侠, 孙海龙, 熊冠星, 杨韶丽.(2018). “长计远虑”的助推效应:怀孕与环境跨期决策. 心理学报, 50(8), 858-867. |
[7] | 梁竹苑, 刘欢.(2011). 跨期选择的性质探索. 心理科学进展, 19(7), 959-966. |
[8] | 刘洪志, 江程铭, 饶俪琳, 李纾.(2015). “时间折扣”还是“单维占优”?--跨期决策的心理机制. 心理学报, 47(4), 522-532. |
[9] | 刘扬, 孙彦.(2016). 时间分解效应及其对跨期决策的影响. 心理学报, 48(4), 362-370. |
[10] | 陆阳, 庄新田.(2015). 跨期决策中动态不一致性程度研究. 东北大学学报(自然科学版), (5), 753-756. |
[11] | 孙红月, 江程铭.(2016). 跨期决策是基于选项还是基于维度?. 心理科学进展, 24(3), 431-437. |
[12] | 索涛, 张锋, 赵国祥, 李红.(2014). 时间感知差异对跨期选择倾向的影响作用. 心理学报, 46(2), 165-173. |
[13] | 王盼盼, 何嘉梅.(2020). 情景预见对跨期决策的影响机制. 心理学报, 52(1), 38-54. |
[14] | 王鹏, 王晓田, 高娟, 黎夏岚, 徐静.(2019). 适应性时间管理: 死亡意识对时间知觉和跨期决策的影响. 心理学报, 51(12), 1341-1350. |
[15] | 庄锦英, 应娟娟, 杨文渊.(2017). 时间方向等因素影响跨期决策的实验研究. 心理与行为研究, (2), 218-222. |
[16] |
Agostino C. S., Caetano M. S., Balci F., Claessens P. M., & Zana Y.(2017). Individual differences in long-range time representation. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 79(3), 833-840.
doi: 10.3758/s13414-017-1286-9 URL |
[17] |
Amasino D. R., Sullivan N. J., Kranton R. E., & Huettel S. A.(2019). Amount and time exert independent influences on intertemporal choice. Nature Human Behaviour, 3(4), 383-392.
doi: 10.1038/s41562-019-0537-2 pmid: 30971787 |
[18] |
Andersen S., Harrison G. W., Lau M. I., & Rutström E. E.(2014). Discounting behavior: A reconsideration. European Economic Review, 71, 15-33.
doi: 10.1016/j.euroecorev.2014.06.009 URL |
[19] |
Apaydın N., Üstün S., Kale E. H., Çelikağ İ., Özgüven H. D., Baskak B., & Çiçek M.(2018). Neural mechanisms underlying time perception and reward anticipation. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 12, 115.
doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00115 pmid: 29662447 |
[20] |
Augenblick N., Niederle M., & Sprenger C.(2015). Working over time: Dynamic inconsistency in real effort tasks. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 130(3), 1067-1115.
doi: 10.1093/qje/qjv020 URL |
[21] |
Baldassi C., Cerreia-Vioglio S., Maccheroni F., Marinacci M., & Pirazzini M.(2020). A behavioral characterization of the drift diffusion model and its multialternative extension for choice under time pressure. Management Science, 66(11), 5075-5093.
doi: 10.1287/mnsc.2019.3475 URL |
[22] |
Block R. A., & Gruber R. P.(2014). Time perception, attention, and memory: A selective review. Acta Psychologica, 149, 129-133.
doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2013.11.003 URL |
[23] |
Bradford W. D., Dolan P., & Galizzi M. M.(2019). Looking ahead: Subjective time perception and individual discounting. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 58(1), 43-69.
doi: 10.1007/s11166-019-09298-1 |
[24] |
Carvalho L. S., Meier S., & Wang S. W.(2016). Poverty and economic decision-making: Evidence from changes in financial resources at payday. American Economic Review, 106(2), 260-84.
doi: 10.1257/aer.20140481 pmid: 28003681 |
[25] |
Celeghin A., Diano M., Bagnis A., Viola M., & Tamietto M.(2017). Basic emotions in human neuroscience: Neuroimaging and beyond. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1432.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01432 pmid: 28883803 |
[26] |
Chen Z., Guo Y., & Feng T.(2018). Neuroanatomical correlates of time perspective: A voxel-based morphometry study. Behavioural Brain Research, 339, 255-260.
doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2017.11.004 URL |
[27] |
Damsma A., van der Mijn R., & van Rijn H.(2018). Neural markers of memory consolidation do not predict temporal estimates of encoded items. Neuropsychologia, 117, 36-45.
doi: S0028-3932(18)30195-7 pmid: 29729958 |
[28] |
Eryilmaz H., van de Ville D., Schwartz S., & Vuilleumier P.(2011). Impact of transient emotions on functional connectivity during subsequent resting state: A wavelet correlation approach. Neuroimage, 54(3), 2481-2491.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.10.021 pmid: 20955802 |
[29] | Faro D., McGill A. L., & Hastie R.(2013). The influence of perceived causation on judgments of time: An integrative review and implications for decision-making. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 217. |
[30] |
Fereday R., Buehner M. J., & Rushton S. K.(2019). The role of time perception in temporal binding: Impaired temporal resolution in causal sequences. Cognition, 193, 104005.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2019.06.017 URL |
[31] |
Fisher G.(2017). An attentional drift diffusion model over binary-attribute choice. Cognition, 168, 34-45.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2017.06.007 URL |
[32] |
Fobbs W. C., & Mizumori S. J.(2017). A framework for understanding and advancing intertemporal choice research using rodent models. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 139, 89-97.
doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2017.01.004 URL |
[33] |
Guo Y., Chen Z., & Feng T.(2017). The effect of future time perspective on delay discounting is mediated by the gray matter volume of vmPFC. Neuropsychologia, 102, 229-236.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.06.021 URL |
[34] | Han R., & Takahashi T.(2012). Psychophysics of time perception and valuation in temporal discounting of gain and loss. Physica a Statal Mechanics & its Applications, 391(24), 6568-6576. |
[35] |
Ikink I., Engelmann J. B., van den Bos W., Roelofs K., & Figner B.(2019). Time ambiguity during intertemporal decision-making is aversive, impacting choice and neural value coding. NeuroImage, 185, 236-244.
doi: S1053-8119(18)31967-0 pmid: 30296559 |
[36] |
Ivry R. B., & Schlerf J. E.(2008). Dedicated and intrinsic models of time perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(7), 273-280.
doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.04.002 URL |
[37] |
Kaur S., Kremer M., & Mullainathan S.(2015). Self-control at work. Journal of Political Economy, 123(6), 1227-1277.
doi: 10.1086/683822 URL |
[38] | Kees J.(2010). Temporal framing in health advertising: The role of risk and future orientation. Journal of Current Issues & Research in Advertising, 32(1), 33-46. |
[39] |
Khosravi P., Parker A. J., Shuback A. T., & Adleman N. E.(2020). Attention control ability, mood state, and emotional regulation ability partially affect executive control of attention on task-irrelevant emotional stimuli. Acta Psychologica, 210, 103169.
doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2020.103169 URL |
[40] | Kim B. K., & Zauberman G.(2009). Perception of anticipatory time in temporal discounting. Journal of Neuroence Psychology & Economics, 83(2), 67-100. |
[41] |
Kim B. K., & Zauberman G.(2019). Psychological time and intertemporal preference. Current Opinion in Psychology, 26, 90-93.
doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2018.06.005 URL |
[42] |
Kim D., & Jang S. S.(2017). Stress and food choices: Examining gender differences and the time horizon framing effect. International Journal of Hospitality Management, 67, 134-142.
doi: 10.1016/j.ijhm.2017.08.012 URL |
[43] |
Kim J., Kim D., & Jung M. W.(2018). Distinct dynamics of striatal and prefrontal neural activity during temporal discrimination. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 12, 34.
doi: 10.3389/fnint.2018.00034 URL |
[44] |
Kim J., & Nan X.(2019). Temporal framing effects differ for narrative versus non-narrative messages: The case of promoting HPV vaccination. Communication Research, 46(3), 401-417.
doi: 10.1177/0093650215626980 URL |
[45] |
Kim K., & Kim H. S.(2018). Time matters: Framing antismoking messages using current smokers’ preexisting perceptions of temporal distance to smoking-related health risks. Health Communication, 33(3), 338-348.
doi: 10.1080/10410236.2016.1266579 URL |
[46] |
Koritzky G., He Q., Xue G., Wong S., Xiao L., & Bechara A.(2013). Processing of time within the prefrontal cortex: Recent time engages posterior areas whereas distant time engages anterior areas. Neuroimage, 72, 280-286.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.01.056 pmid: 23380168 |
[47] |
Li H., Guo Y., & Yu Q.(2019). Self-control makes the difference: The psychological mechanism of dual processing model on internet addicts’ unusual behavior in intertemporal choice. Computers in Human Behavior, 101, 95-103.
doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2019.07.010 URL |
[48] |
Manolis C., & Roberts J. A.(2012). Subjective well-being among adolescent consumers: The effects of materialism, compulsive buying, and time affluence. Applied Research in Quality of Life, 7(2), 117-135.
doi: 10.1007/s11482-011-9155-5 URL |
[49] |
Marinho V., Oliveira T., Rocha K., Ribeiro J., Magalhães F., Bento T., … Teixeira S.(2018). The dopaminergic system dynamic in the time perception: A review of the evidence. International Journal of Neuroscience, 128(3), 262-282.
doi: 10.1080/00207454.2017.1385614 pmid: 28950734 |
[50] |
Marini M., & Paglieri F.(2019). Decoy effects in intertemporal and probabilistic choices the role of time pressure, immediacy, and certainty. Behavioural Processes, 162, 130-141.
doi: S0376-6357(18)30464-9 pmid: 30849515 |
[51] |
Masuda A., Sano C., Zhang Q., Goto H., McHugh T. J., Fujisawa S., & Itohara S.(2020). The hippocampus encodes delay and value information during delay-discounting decision making. Elife, 9, e52466.
doi: 10.7554/eLife.52466 URL |
[52] |
McClure S. M., Ericson K. M., Laibson D. I., Loewenstein G., & Cohen J. D.(2007). Time discounting for primary rewards. Journal of Neuroscience, 27(21), 5796-5804.
doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4246-06.2007 pmid: 17522323 |
[53] |
McClure S. M., Laibson D. I., Loewenstein G., & Cohen J. D.(2004). Separate neural systems value immediate and delayed monetary rewards. Science, 306(5695), 503-507.
pmid: 15486304 |
[54] |
Moos K., Vossel S., Weidner R., Sparing R., & Fink G. R.(2012). Modulation of top-down control of visual attention by cathodal tDCS over right IPS. Journal of Neuroscience, 32(46), 16360-16368.
doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6233-11.2012 URL |
[55] |
Muehlbacher S., & Kirchler E.(2019). Individual differences in mental accounting. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 2866.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02866 pmid: 31920887 |
[56] |
Nani A., Manuello J., Liloia D., Duca S., Costa T., & Cauda F.(2019). The neural correlates of time: A meta- analysis of neuroimaging studies. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 31(12), 1796-1826.
doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01459 URL |
[57] |
Nan X., & Qin Y.(2019). How thinking about the future affects our decisions in the present: Effects of time orientation and episodic future thinking on responses to health warning messages. Human Communication Research, 45(2), 148-168.
doi: 10.1093/hcr/hqy017 URL |
[58] |
Paasche C., Weibel S., Wittmann M., & Lalanne L.(2019). Time perception and impulsivity: A proposed relationship in addictive disorders. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 106, 182-201.
doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.12.006 URL |
[59] |
Paternoster R., & Pogarsky G.(2009). Rational choice, agency and thoughtfully reflective decision making: The short and long-term consequences of making good choices. Journal of Quantitative Criminology, 25(2), 103-127.
doi: 10.1007/s10940-009-9065-y URL |
[60] |
Pine A., Seymour B., Roiser J. P., Bossaerts P., Friston K. J., Curran H. V., & Dolan R. J.(2009). Encoding of marginal utility across time in the human brain. Journal of Neuroscience, 29(30), 9575-9581.
doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1126-09.2009 URL |
[61] |
Rajagopal P., & Rha J. Y.(2009). The mental accounting of time. Journal of Economic Psychology, 30(5), 772-781.
doi: 10.1016/j.joep.2009.06.008 URL |
[62] |
Read D.(2001). Is time-discounting hyperbolic or subadditive?. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 23(1), 5-32.
doi: 10.1023/A:1011198414683 URL |
[63] |
Reeck C., Figner B., Weber E. U., Steffener J., Krosch A. R., Wager T. D., & Johnson E. J.(2021). Framing the future first: Medial temporal lobe activation discriminates delay and acceleration framing in intertemporal choice. Journal of Neuroscience, Psychology, and Economics, 14(2), 71-80.
doi: 10.1037/npe0000122 URL |
[64] |
Sadoff S., Samek A., & Sprenger C.(2020). Dynamic inconsistency in food choice: Experimental evidence from two food deserts. The Review of Economic Studies, 87(4), 1954-1988.
doi: 10.1093/restud/rdz030 URL |
[65] |
Samanez-Larkin G. R., Mata R., Radu P. T., Ballard I. C., Carstensen L. L., & McClure S. M.(2011). Age differences in striatal delay sensitivity during intertemporal choice in healthy adults. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 5, 126.
doi: 10.3389/fnins.2011.00126 pmid: 22110424 |
[66] |
Sayman S., & Öncüler A.(2009). An investigation of time inconsistency. Management Science, 55(3), 470-482.
doi: 10.1287/mnsc.1080.0942 URL |
[67] |
Simons J., Vansteenkiste M., Lens W., & Lacante M.(2004). Placing motivation and future time perspective theory in a temporal perspective. Educational Psychology Review, 16(2), 121-139.
doi: 10.1023/B:EDPR.0000026609.94841.2f URL |
[68] |
Siu N. Y., Lam H. H., Le J. J., & Przepiorka A. M.(2014). Time perception and time perspective differences between adolescents and adults. Acta Psychologica, 151, 222-229.
doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.06.013 URL |
[69] |
Sprenger C.(2015). Judging experimental evidence on dynamic inconsistency. American Economic Review, 105(5), 80-85.
doi: 10.1257/aer.p20151107 URL |
[70] |
Sripada C. S., Gonzalez R., Luan Phan K., & Liberzon I.(2011). The neural correlates of intertemporal decision- making: Contributions of subjective value, stimulus type, and trait impulsivity. Human Brain Mapping, 32(10), 1637-1648.
doi: 10.1002/hbm.21136 URL |
[71] |
Stephan E., Liberman N., & Trope Y.(2011). The effects of time perspective and level of construal on social distance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47(2), 397- 402.
pmid: 21836728 |
[72] | Szollos A.(2009). Toward a psychology of chronic time pressure: Conceptual and methodological review. Time & Society, 18(2-3), 332-350. |
[73] |
Takahashi T.(2005). Loss of self-control in intertemporal choice may be attributable to logarithmic time-perception. Medical Hypotheses, 65(4), 691-693.
pmid: 15990243 |
[74] | Takahashi T., Oono H., & Radford M. H.(2008). Psychophysics of time perception and intertemporal choice models. Physica A: Statistical Mechanics and its Applications, 387(8-9), 2066-2074. |
[75] |
Tanaka S. C., Doya K., Okada G., Ueda K., Okamoto Y., & Yamawaki S.(2004). Prediction of immediate and future rewards differentially recruits cortico-basal ganglia loops. Nature Neuroscience, 7(8), 887-893.
pmid: 15235607 |
[76] |
Tavares G., Perona P., & Rangel A.(2017). The attentional drift diffusion model of simple perceptual decision-making. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 11, 468.
doi: 10.3389/fnins.2017.00468 URL |
[77] |
Teki S., Gu B. M., & Meck W. H.(2017). The persistence of memory: How the brain encodes time in memory. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 17, 178-185.
doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2017.09.003 URL |
[78] | Üstün S., Kale E. H., & Çiçek M.(2017). Neural networks for time perception and working memory. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 11, 83. |
[79] |
Wang Q., Luo S., Monterosso J., Zhang J., Fang X., Dong Q., & Xue G.(2014). Distributed value representation in the medial prefrontal cortex during intertemporal choices. Journal of Neuroscience, 34(22), 7522-7530.
doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0351-14.2014 URL |
[80] |
Whillans A. V., Dunn E. W., Smeets P., Bekkers R., & Norton M. I.(2017). Buying time promotes happiness. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114(32), 8523-8527.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1706541114 URL |
[81] |
Wittmann M., Leland D. S., & Paulus M. P.(2007). Time and decision making: Differential contribution of the posterior insular cortex and the striatum during a delay discounting task. Experimental Brain Research, 179(4), 643-653.
doi: 10.1007/s00221-006-0822-y pmid: 17216152 |
[82] |
Wittmann M., & Paulus M. P.(2008). Decision making, impulsivity and time perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(1), 7-12.
pmid: 18042423 |
[83] |
Wittmann M., Simmons A. N., Flagan T., Lane S. D., Wackermann J., & Paulus M. P.(2011). Neural substrates of time perception and impulsivity. Brain Research, 1406, 43-58.
doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2011.06.048 pmid: 21763642 |
[84] |
Xu M., Zhang S. Y., Dan Y., & Poo M. M.(2014). Representation of interval timing by temporally scalable firing patterns in rat prefrontal cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(1), 480-485.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1321314111 URL |
[85] |
Yamada Y., & Kawabe T.(2011). Emotion colors time perception unconsciously. Consciousness and Cognition, 20(4), 1835-1841.
doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2011.06.016 pmid: 21764331 |
[86] |
Zauberman G., Kim B. K., Malkoc S. A., & Bettman J. R.(2009). Discounting time and time discounting: Subjective time perception and intertemporal preferences. Journal of Marketing Research, 46(4), 543-556.
doi: 10.1509/jmkr.46.4.543 URL |
[87] | Zhang S., Liu P., & Feng T.(2019). To do it now or later: The cognitive mechanisms and neural substrates underlying procrastination. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 10(4), e1492. |
[88] | Zhao C. X., Jiang C. M., Zhou L., Li S., Rao L. L., & Zheng R.(2015). The hidden opportunity cost of time effect on intertemporal choice. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 311. |
[89] | Zhou L., Yang Y., & Li S.(2021). Music-induced emotions influence intertemporal decision making. Cognition & Emotion. Advance online publication. |
[90] |
Zhou M.(2013). “I am Really Good at It” or “I am Just Feeling Lucky”: The effects of emotions on information problem-solving. Educational Technology Research and Development, 61(3), 505-520.
doi: 10.1007/s11423-013-9300-y URL |
[91] |
Zimbardo P. G., & Boyd J. N.(1999). Putting time in psychological perspective: a valid, reliable individual- differences metric. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77(6), 1271-1288.
doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.77.6.1271 URL |
[1] | ZHANG Shuyue, HUANG Junqing, ZHAO Feng, XU Kepeng. Social exclusion influenced intertemporal decision-making and its mechanism [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2022, 30(3): 486-498. |
[2] | WENG Chunchun, WANG Ning. Animal research paradigm and related neural mechanism of interval timing [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2020, 28(9): 1478-1492. |
[3] | JIA Lina; WANG Lili; ZANG Xuelian; FENG Wenfeng; ZHANG Zhijie. Emotional Time Perception: An Embodiment Perspective [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2015, 23(8): 1331-1339. |
[4] | YAO Zhuxi; ZHANG Liang; ZHANG Kan. Development and Influencing Factors of the Scalar Timing Model [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2015, 23(5): 784-792. |
[5] | A Bu-Lai-Ti-Jiang; LIU Yang; ZHU Xiao-Rui; ZHENG Rui; LIANG Zhu-Yuan; RAO Li-Lin; WU Bin; LI Shu. The Effect of Self on Intertemporal Choice from the Perspective of Cultural Individualism-collectivism [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2015, 23(11): 1981-1990. |
[6] | LIU Yang;SUN Yan. New Avenues for Framing Effect Research in Decision-making: From Risky to Intertemporal and from Verbal to Graph Framing [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2014, 22(8): 1205-1217. |
[7] | LIN Miao;QIAN Xiu-Ying. The Role of Attention In Time Perception and Its Theories [J]. , 2012, 20(6): 875-882. |
[8] | ZhangZguhuwl;Yin Huazhan;Huang Xiting. Time Perception and Contingent Negative Variation [J]. , 2007, 15(2): 308-312. |
Viewed | ||||||
Full text |
|
|||||
Abstract |
|
|||||