Advances in Psychological Science ›› 2022, Vol. 30 ›› Issue (1): 65-76.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2022.00065
• Regular Articles • Previous Articles Next Articles
ZHAO Lihua, YANG Yunmei, LI Jing()
Received:
2021-03-05
Online:
2022-01-15
Published:
2021-11-25
CLC Number:
ZHAO Lihua, YANG Yunmei, LI Jing. The relationship between children’s reading and theory of mind[J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2022, 30(1): 65-76.
[1] | 陈丽娟, 徐晓东. (2020). 文学阅读如何影响读者的心理理论. 心理科学进展, 28(3), 434-442. |
[2] |
Adrian, J. E., Clemente, R. A., Villanueva, L., & Rieffe, C. (2005). Parent-child picture-book reading, mothers' mental state language and children's theory of mind. Journal of Child Language, 32(3), 673-686.
pmid: 16220639 |
[3] |
Altun, D. (2018). The efficacy of multimedia stories in preschoolers’ explicit and implicit story comprehension. Early Childhood Education Journal, 46(6), 629-642.
doi: 10.1007/s10643-018-0916-8 URL |
[4] |
Anguera, J. A., Boccanfuso, J., Rintoul, J. L., Al-Hashimi, O., Faraji, F., Janowich, J., … Gazzaley, A. (2013). Video game training enhances cognitive control in older adults. Nature, 501(7465), 97-101.
doi: 10.1038/nature12486 URL |
[5] |
Atkinson, L., Slade, L., Powell, D., & Levy, J. P. (2017). Theory of mind in emerging reading comprehension: A longitudinal study of early indirect and direct effects. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 164, 225-238.
doi: S0022-0965(16)30190-4 pmid: 28552390 |
[6] |
Bal, P. M., & Veltkamp, M. (2013). How does fiction reading influence empathy? An experimental investigation on the role of emotional transportation. Plos One, 8(1), e55341.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0055341 URL |
[7] |
Barnes-Holmes, Y., McHugh, L., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2004). Perspective-taking and theory of mind: A relational frame account. The Behavior Analyst Today, 5(1), 15-25.
doi: 10.1037/h0100133 URL |
[8] |
Barzilai, S., Zohar, A. R., & Mor-Hagani, S. (2018). Promoting integration of multiple texts: A review of instructional approaches and practices. Educational Psychology Review, 30(3), 973-999.
doi: 10.1007/s10648-018-9436-8 URL |
[9] | Blackmore, S. (2005). Consciousness: A very short introduction. Oxford University Press. |
[10] |
Boerma, I. E., Mol, S. E., & Jolles, J. (2016). Reading pictures for story comprehension requires mental imagery skills. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1630.
pmid: 27822194 |
[11] |
Boerma, I. E., Mol, S. E., & Jolles, J. (2017). The role of home literacy environment, mentalizing, expressive verbal ability, and print exposure in third and fourth graders’ reading comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading, 21(3), 179-193.
doi: 10.1080/10888438.2016.1277727 URL |
[12] |
Brooks, R., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2015). Connecting the dots from infancy to childhood: A longitudinal study connecting gaze following, language, and explicit theory of mind. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 130, 67-78.
doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2014.09.010 URL |
[13] |
Bruner, J. S. (1991). The narrative construction of reality. Critical Inquiry, 18(1), 1-21.
doi: 10.1086/448619 URL |
[14] |
Bzdok, D., Schilbach, L., Vogeley, K., Schneider, K., Laird, A. R., Langner, R., & Eickhoff, S. B. (2012). Parsing the neural correlates of moral cognition: ALE meta-analysis on morality, theory of mind, and empathy. Brain Structure and Function, 217(4), 783-796.
doi: 10.1007/s00429-012-0380-y URL |
[15] |
Caputi, M., Lecce, S., Pagnin, A., & Banerjee, R. (2012). Longitudinal effects of theory of mind on later peer relations: The role of prosocial behavior. Developmental Psychology, 48(1), 257-270.
doi: 10.1037/a0025402 URL |
[16] |
Christensen, W., & Michael, J. (2016). From two systems to a multi-systems architecture for mindreading. New Ideas in Psychology, 40, 48-64.
doi: 10.1016/j.newideapsych.2015.01.003 URL |
[17] |
Courtin, C., & Melot, A. M. (2005). Metacognitive development of deaf children: Lessons from the appearance-reality and false belief tasks. Developmental Science, 8(1), 16-25.
pmid: 15647063 |
[18] |
Dehaene, S., Cohen, L., Morais, J., & Kolinsky, R. (2015). Illiterate to literate: Behavioural and cerebral changes induced by reading acquisition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 16(4), 234-244.
doi: 10.1038/nrn3924 URL |
[19] |
Dempsey, L., & Skarakis-Doyle, E. (2019). Story comprehension in pre-readers: Understanding goal structure and generating inferences when a story has competing goals. Early Child Development and Care, 189(10), 1724-1736.
doi: 10.1080/03004430.2017.1408610 |
[20] |
Derksen, D. G., Hunsche, M. C., Giroux, M. E., Connolly, D. A., & Bernstein, D. M. (2018). A systematic review of theory of mind’s precursors and functions. Zeitschrift Für Psychologie, 226(2), 87-97.
doi: 10.1027/2151-2604/a000325 URL |
[21] |
Devine, R. T., & Hughes, C. (2016). Measuring theory of mind across middle childhood: Reliability and validity of the silent films and strange stories tasks. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 149, 23-40.
doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2015.07.011 pmid: 26255713 |
[22] |
Devine, R. T., & Hughes, C. (2018). Family correlates of false belief understanding in early childhood: A meta‐analysis. Child Development, 89(3), 971-987.
doi: 10.1111/cdev.12682 pmid: 27883189 |
[23] |
Devine, R. T., & Hughes, C. (2019). Let's talk: Parents' mental talk (not mind-mindedness or mindreading capacity) predicts children's false belief understanding. Child Development, 90(4), 1236-1253.
doi: 10.1111/cdev.12990 pmid: 29115674 |
[24] |
Devine, R. T., White, N., Ensor, R., & Hughes, C. (2016). Theory of mind in middle childhood: Longitudinal associations with executive function and social competence. Developmental Psychology, 52(5), 758-771.
doi: 10.1037/dev0000105 pmid: 26914214 |
[25] |
Diergarten, A. K., & Nieding, G. (2015). Children's and adults’ ability to build online emotional inferences during comprehension of audiovisual and auditory texts. Journal of Cognition and Development, 16(2), 381-406.
doi: 10.1080/15248372.2013.848871 URL |
[26] |
Dore, R. A., Amendum, S. J., Golinkoff, R. M., & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (2018). Theory of mind: A hidden factor in reading comprehension?. Educational Psychology Review, 30(3), 1067-1089.
doi: 10.1007/s10648-018-9443-9 URL |
[27] |
Dyer, J. R., Shatz, M., & Wellman, H. M. (2000). Young children's storybooks as a source of mental state information. Cognitive Development, 15(1), 17-37.
doi: 10.1016/S0885-2014(00)00017-4 URL |
[28] |
Faires, J., Nichols, W. D., & Rickelman, R. J. (2010). Effects of parental involvement in developing competent readers in first grade. Reading Psychology, 21(3), 195-215.
doi: 10.1080/02702710050144340 URL |
[29] | Feldman, C. F., Bruner, J., Renderer, B., & Spitzer, S. (1990). Narrative comprehension. In B. K. Britton & A. D. Pellegrini (Eds.), Narrative thought and narrative language (pp.1-78). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. |
[30] |
Ferstl, E. C., Neumann, J., Bogler, C., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2008). The extended language network: A meta‐analysis of neuroimaging studies on text comprehension. Human Brain Mapping, 29(5), 581-593.
doi: 10.1002/(ISSN)1097-0193 URL |
[31] |
Flavell, J. H., Green, F. L., & Flavell, E. R. (2000). Development of children's awareness of their own thoughts. Journal of Cognition and Development, 1(1), 97-112.
doi: 10.1207/S15327647JCD0101N_10 URL |
[32] |
Florit, E., de Carli, P., Giunti, G., & Mason, L. (2020). Advanced theory of mind uniquely contributes to children’s multiple-text comprehension. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 189, 104708.
doi: S0022-0965(19)30062-1 pmid: 31634737 |
[33] |
Gamannossi, B. A., & Pinto, G. (2014). Theory of mind and language of mind in narratives: Developmental trends from kindergarten to primary school. First Language, 34(3), 262-272.
doi: 10.1177/0142723714535875 URL |
[34] |
Giménez-Dasí, M., Pons, F., & Bender, P. K. (2016). Imaginary companions, theory of mind and emotion understanding in young children. European Early Childhood Education Research Journal, 24(2), 186-197.
doi: 10.1080/1350293X.2014.919778 URL |
[35] |
Gonzales, C. R., Fabricius, W. V., & Kupfer, A. S. (2018). Introspection plays an early role in children's explicit theory of mind development. Child Development, 89(5), 1545-1552.
doi: 10.1111/cdev.12876 URL |
[36] |
Gopnik, A., & Astington, J. W. (1988). Children's understanding of representational change and its relation to the understanding of false belief and the appearance- reality distinction. Child Development, 59(1), 26-37.
pmid: 3342716 |
[37] |
Graesser, A. C., Millis, K. K., & Zwaan, R. A. (1997). Discourse comprehension. Annual Review of Psychology, 48(1), 163-189.
doi: 10.1146/psych.1997.48.issue-1 URL |
[38] |
Guajardo, N. R., & Cartwright, K. B. (2016). The contribution of theory of mind, counterfactual reasoning, and executive function to pre-readers’ language comprehension and later reading awareness and comprehension in elementary school. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 144, 27-45.
doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2015.11.004 pmid: 26689129 |
[39] |
Hale, C. M., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2003). The influence of language on theory of mind: A training study. Developmental Science, 6(3), 346-359.
doi: 10.1111/desc.2003.6.issue-3 URL |
[40] |
Hannon, B., & Frias, S. (2012). A new measure for assessing the contributions of higher level processes to language comprehension performance in preschoolers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 104(4), 897-921.
doi: 10.1037/a0029156 URL |
[41] |
Hassabis, D., & Maguire, E. A. (2009). The construction system of the brain. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364(1521), 1263-1271.
doi: 10.1098/rstb.2008.0296 URL |
[42] |
Hassabis, D., Spreng, R. N., Rusu, A. A., Robbins, C. A., Mar, R. A., & Schacter, D. L. (2014). Imagine all the people: How the brain creates and uses personality models to predict behavior. Cerebral Cortex, 24(8), 1979-1987.
doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht042 URL |
[43] |
Hofmann, S. G., Doan, S. N., Sprung, M., Wilson, A., Ebesutani, C., Andrews, L. A., … Harris, P. L. (2016). Training children's theory-of-mind: A meta-analysis of controlled studies. Cognition, 150, 200-212.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2016.01.006 pmid: 26901235 |
[44] |
Holmer, E., Heimann, M., & Rudner, M. (2016). Theory of mind and reading comprehension in deaf and hard-of- hearing signing children. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 854.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00854 pmid: 27375532 |
[45] |
Hoover, W. A., & Gough, P. B. (1990). The simple view of reading. Reading and Writing, 2(2), 127-160.
doi: 10.1007/BF00401799 URL |
[46] |
Horowitz-Kraus, T., Eaton, K., Farah, R., Hajinazarian, A., Vannest, J., & Holland, S. K. (2015). Predicting better performance on a college preparedness test from narrative comprehension at the age of 6 years: An fMRI study. Brain Research, 1629, 54-62.
doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2015.10.008 pmid: 26459989 |
[47] |
Huang, S. R., & Cui, C. (2020). Preventing child sexual abuse using picture books: The effect of book character and message framing. Journal of Child Sexual Abuse, 29(4), 448-467.
doi: 10.1080/10538712.2020.1719449 URL |
[48] |
Hughes, C., & Devine, R. T. (2015). Individual differences in theory of mind from preschool to adolescence: Achievements and directions. Child Development Perspectives, 9(3), 149-153.
doi: 10.1111/cdep.2015.9.issue-3 URL |
[49] |
Hutton, J. S., Phelan, K., Horowitz-Kraus, T., Dudley, J., Altaye, M., Dewitt, T., & Holland, S. K. (2017). Shared reading quality and brain activation during story listening in preschool-age children. The Journal of Pediatrics, 191, 204-211.
doi: S0022-3476(17)31097-1 pmid: 29173308 |
[50] |
Ivey, G., & Johnston, P. H. (2013). Engagement with young adult literature: Outcomes and processes. Reading Research Quarterly, 48(3), 255-275.
doi: 10.1002/rrq.46 URL |
[51] | Jacobs, J. E., & Paris, S. G. (1987). Children's metacognition about reading: Issues in definition, measurement, and instruction. Educational Psychologist, 22(3-4), 255-278. |
[52] |
Keen, S. (2006). A theory of narrative empathy. Narrative, 14(3), 207-236.
doi: 10.1353/nar.2006.0015 URL |
[53] |
Kim, Y.-S. G. (2015). Language and cognitive predictors of text comprehension: Evidence from multivariate analysis. Child Development, 86(1), 128-144.
doi: 10.1111/cdev.2015.86.issue-1 URL |
[54] |
Kim, Y.-S. G. (2016). Direct and mediated effects of language and cognitive skills on comprehension of oral narrative texts (listening comprehension) for children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 141, 101-120.
doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2015.08.003 URL |
[55] |
Kim, Y.-S. G. (2017). Why the simple view of reading is not simplistic: Unpacking component skills of reading using a direct and indirect effect model of reading (DIER). Scientific Studies of Reading, 21(4), 310-333.
doi: 10.1080/10888438.2017.1291643 URL |
[56] |
Kim, Y.-S. G. (2020). Theory of mind mediates the relations of language and domain-general cognitions to discourse comprehension. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 194, 104813.
doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2020.104813 URL |
[57] |
Kim, Y.-S. G., & Phillips, B. (2014). Cognitive correlates of listening comprehension. Reading Research Quarterly, 49(3), 269-281.
doi: 10.1002/rrq.2014.49.issue-3 URL |
[58] |
Kirby, J. R., & Savage, R. S. (2008). Can the simple view deal with the complexities of reading? Literacy, 42(2), 75-82.
doi: 10.1111/read.2008.42.issue-2 URL |
[59] |
Kloo, D., Sodian, B., Kristen-Antonow, S., Kim, S., & Paulus, M. (2021). Knowing minds: Linking early perspective taking and later metacognitive insight. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 39(1), 39-53.
doi: 10.1111/bjdp.v39.1 URL |
[60] |
Knudsen, E. I. (2004). Sensitive periods in the development of the brain and behavior. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(8), 1412-1425.
pmid: 15509387 |
[61] |
LaRusso, M., Kim, H. Y., Selman, R., Uccelli, P., Dawson, T., Jones, S., … Snow, C. (2016). Contributions of academic language, perspective taking, and complex reasoning to deep reading comprehension. Journal of Research on Educational Effectiveness, 9(2), 201-222.
doi: 10.1080/19345747.2015.1116035 URL |
[62] | Lecce, S., Bianco, F., Demicheli, P., & Cavallini, E. (2014). Training preschoolers on first‐order false belief understanding: Transfer on advanced ToM skills and metamemory. Child Development, 85(6), 2404-2418. |
[63] |
Lecce, S., Caputi, M., & Pagnin, A. (2014). Long-term effect of theory of mind on school achievement: The role of sensitivity to criticism. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 11(3), 305-318.
doi: 10.1080/17405629.2013.821944 URL |
[64] |
Lecce, S., Zocchi, S., Pagnin, A., Palladino, P., & Taumoepeau, M. (2010). Reading minds: The relation between children’s mental state knowledge and their metaknowledge about reading. Child Development, 81(6), 1876-1893.
doi: 10.1111/cdev.2010.81.issue-6 URL |
[65] |
Lillard, A. S., & Kavanaugh, R. D. (2014). The contribution of symbolic skills to the development of an explicit theory of mind. Child Development, 85(4), 1535-1551.
doi: 10.1111/cdev.2014.85.issue-4 URL |
[66] | Linell, P.(2009. Rethinking Language, Mind and World Dialogically: Interactional and Contextual Theories of Human Sense-Making. Greenwich, CT: Information Age Publishing. |
[67] |
Lysaker, J. T., & Alicea, Z. A. (2017). Theorizing fiction reading engagement during wordless book reading. Linguistics and Education, 37, 42-51.
doi: 10.1016/j.linged.2016.11.001 URL |
[68] |
Lysaker, J. T., & Miller, A. (2013). Engaging social imagination: The developmental work of wordless book reading. Journal of Early Childhood Literacy, 13(2), 147-174.
doi: 10.1177/1468798411430425 URL |
[69] |
Lysaker, J. T., & Nie, A. Y. (2017). Social and relational aspects of comprehending in one fourth grader’s unaided and illustration-aided picturebook retellings: Retelling as co-authoring. Journal of Literacy Research, 49(1), 38-67.
doi: 10.1177/1086296X16684583 URL |
[70] | Lysaker, J. T., Tonge, C., Gauson, D., & Miller, A. (2011). Reading and social imagination: What relationally oriented reading instruction can do for children. Reading Psychology, 32(6), 520-566. |
[71] |
Mar, R. A. (2011). The neural bases of social cognition and story comprehension. Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 103-134.
doi: 10.1146/psych.2011.62.issue-1 URL |
[72] | Mar, R. A. (2018). Evaluating whether stories can promote social cognition: Introducing the Social Processes and Content Entrained by Narrative (SPaCEN) framework. Discourse Processes, 55(5-6), 454-479. |
[73] |
Mar, R. A., & Oatley, K. (2008). The function of fiction is the abstraction and simulation of social experience. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3(3), 173-192.
doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6924.2008.00073.x URL |
[74] |
Mar, R. A., Oatley, K., Hirsh, J., dela Paz, J., & Peterson, J. B. (2006). Bookworms versus nerds: Exposure to fiction versus non-fiction, divergent associations with social ability, and the simulation of fictional social worlds. Journal of Research in Personality, 40(5), 694-712.
doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2005.08.002 URL |
[75] |
Mason, L. (2018). Multiplicity in the digital era: Processing and learning from multiple sources and modalities of instructional presentations. Learning and Instruction, 57, 76-81.
doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2018.03.004 URL |
[76] |
Mason, R. A., & Just, M. A. (2009). The role of the theory-of-mind cortical network in the comprehension of narratives. Language and Linguistics Compass, 3(1), 157-174.
doi: 10.1111/j.1749-818X.2008.00122.x URL |
[77] |
McCreary, J. J., & Marchant, G. J. (2016). Reading and empathy. Reading Psychology, 38(2), 182-202.
doi: 10.1080/02702711.2016.1245690 URL |
[78] |
Melot, A. M., & Angeard, N. (2003). Theory of mind: Is training contagious?. Developmental Science, 6(2), 178-184.
doi: 10.1111/desc.2003.6.issue-2 URL |
[79] |
Merzenich, M. M., van Vleet, T. M., & Nahum, M. (2014). Brain plasticity-based therapeutics. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 385.
doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00385 pmid: 25018719 |
[80] |
Miller, S. A. (2009). Children’s understanding of second- order mental states. Psychological Bulletin, 135(5), 749-773.
doi: 10.1037/a0016854 pmid: 19702381 |
[81] |
Moll, H., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2011). How does it look? Level 2 perspective‐taking at 36 months of age. Child Development, 82(2), 661-673.
doi: 10.1111/cdev.2011.82.issue-2 URL |
[82] |
Montoya-Rodríguez, M. M., & Molina-Cobos, F. J. (2019). Training perspective taking skills in individuals with intellectual disabilities: A functional approach. Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science, 14, 1-10.
doi: 10.1016/j.jcbs.2019.08.003 |
[83] |
Oakhill, J. V., & Cain, K. (2012). The precursors of reading ability in young readers: Evidence from a four-year longitudinal study. Scientific Studies of Reading, 16(2), 91-121.
doi: 10.1080/10888438.2010.529219 URL |
[84] | Ostrov, J. M., & Kamper, K. E. (2015). Future directions for research on the development of relational and physical peer victimization. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 44(3), 509-519. |
[85] |
Paris, S. G., & Jacobs, J. E. (1984). The benefits of informed instruction for children's reading awareness and comprehension skills. Child Development, 55(6), 2083-2093.
doi: 10.2307/1129781 URL |
[86] |
Parsons, L. T. (2013). An examination of fourth graders’ aesthetic engagement with literary characters. Reading Psychology, 34(1), 1-25.
doi: 10.1080/02702711.2011.566762 URL |
[87] |
Pelletier, J., & Beatty, R. (2015). Children's understanding of Aesop's fables: Relations to reading comprehension and theory of mind. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1448.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01448 pmid: 26500569 |
[88] | Pelletier, J., & Wilde Astington, J. (2004). Action, consciousness and theory of mind: Children's ability to coordinate story characters' actions and thoughts. Early Education & Development, 15(1), 5-22. |
[89] |
Perfetti, C., & Stafura, J. (2014). Word knowledge in a theory of reading comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading, 18(1), 22-37.
doi: 10.1080/10888438.2013.827687 URL |
[90] |
Ricketts, J., Jones, C. R., Happe, F., & Charman, T. (2013). Reading comprehension in autism spectrum disorders: The role of oral language and social functioning. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43(4), 807-816.
doi: 10.1007/s10803-012-1619-4 pmid: 22843036 |
[91] | Riggio, M. M., & Cassidy, K. W. (2009). Preschoolers' processing of false beliefs within the context of picture book reading. Early Education & Development, 20(6), 992-1015. |
[92] |
Roby, E., & Scott, R. M. (2018). The relationship between parental mental-state language and 2.5-year-olds' performance on a nontraditional false-belief task. Cognition, 180, 10-23.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2018.06.017 URL |
[93] |
Ruffman, T., Slade, L., & Crowe, E. (2002). The relation between children's and mothers’ mental state language and theory-of-mind understanding. Child Development, 73(3), 734-751.
pmid: 12038548 |
[94] |
Schnotz, W. (2002). Commentary: Towards an integrated view of learning from text and visual displays. Educational Psychology Review, 14(1), 101-120.
doi: 10.1023/A:1013136727916 URL |
[95] |
Scott, R. M., & Baillargeon, R. (2017). Early false-belief understanding. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21(4), 237-249.
doi: S1364-6613(17)30018-9 pmid: 28259555 |
[96] |
Slaughter, V. (1998). Children's understanding of pictorial and mental representations. Child Development, 69(2), 321-332.
pmid: 9586208 |
[97] |
Slaughter, V., & Gopnik, A. (1996). Conceptual coherence in the child's theory of mind: Training children to understand belief. Child Development, 67(6), 2967-2988.
pmid: 9071768 |
[98] |
Sodian, B., & Kristen-Antonow, S. (2015). Declarative joint attention as a foundation of theory of mind. Developmental Psychology, 51(9), 1190-1200.
doi: 10.1037/dev0000039 pmid: 26192041 |
[99] |
Stadtler, M., & Bromme, R. (2013). Multiple document comprehension: An approach to public understanding of science. Cognition and Instruction, 31(2), 122-129.
doi: 10.1080/07370008.2013.771106 URL |
[100] |
Strasser, K., & Río, F. d. (2014). The role of comprehension monitoring, theory of mind, and vocabulary depth in predicting story comprehension and recall of kindergarten children. Reading Research Quarterly, 49(2), 169-187.
doi: 10.1002/rrq.2014.49.issue-2 URL |
[101] |
Tamir, D. I., Bricker, A. B., Dodell-Feder, D., & Mitchell, J. P. (2016). Reading fiction and reading minds: The role of simulation in the default network. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11(2), 215-224.
doi: 10.1093/scan/nsv114 URL |
[102] |
Tamir, D. I., & Mitchell, J. P. (2011). The default network distinguishes construals of proximal versus distal events. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(10), 2945-2955.
doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00009 URL |
[103] |
Tompkins, V., Guo, Y., & Justice, L. M. (2012). Inference generation, story comprehension, and language skills in the preschool years. Reading and Writing, 26(3), 403-429.
doi: 10.1007/s11145-012-9374-7 URL |
[104] | Vygotsky, L. S. (1980). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Harvard university press. |
[105] |
Wellman, H. M., Cross, D., & Watson, J. (2001). Meta‐analysis of theory-of-mind development: The truth about false belief. Child Development, 72(3), 655-684.
pmid: 11405571 |
[106] |
Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception. Cognition, 13(1), 103-128.
pmid: 6681741 |
[107] |
Wood, D., Bruner, J. S., & Ross, G. (1976). The role of tutoring in problem solving. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 17(2), 89-100.
pmid: 932126 |
[108] |
Yuill, N. (2009). The relation between ambiguity understanding and metalinguistic discussion of joking riddles in good and poor comprehenders: Potential for intervention and possible processes of change. First Language, 29(1), 65-79.
doi: 10.1177/0142723708097561 URL |
[109] |
Zwaan, R. A., & Radvansky, G. A. (1998). Situation models in language comprehension and memory. Psychological Bulletin, 123(2), 162-185.
pmid: 9522683 |
[1] | WU Xiuying, LI Feifei, LIU Baogen. The contribution of dictionary interactivity on young children’s electronic picture book reading [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2023, 31(5): 759-768. |
[2] | LIU Haidan, LI Minyi. Associations between home literacy environment and children’s receptive vocabulary: A meta-analysis [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2022, 30(3): 556-579. |
[3] | LI Hehui, HUANG Huiya, DONG Lin, LUO Yuejia, TAO Wuhai. Developmental dyslexia and cerebellar abnormalities: Multiple roles of the cerebellum and causal relationships between the two [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2022, 30(2): 343-353. |
[4] | YIN Rong. Comparative studies of mind reading: Similarities and differences in theory of mind between non-human primates and humans and corresponding explanations [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2022, 30(11): 2540-2557. |
[5] | LI Lin, ZHAO Sainan, ZHANG Lijuan, WANG Jingxin. Understanding mechanisms of prediction error cost in Chinese reading for older adults [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2022, 30(1): 1-14. |
[6] | ZHANG Manman, ZANG Chuanli, BAI Xuejun. The spatial extent and depth of parafoveal pre-processing during Chinese reading [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2020, 28(6): 871-882. |
[7] | ZHAO Ying, WU Xinchun, XIE Ruibo, FENG Jie, SUN Peng, CHEN Hongjun. Effects of visual language on reading among people who are deaf and hard of hearing and the underlying mechanisms [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2020, 28(6): 969-977. |
[8] | CHEN Lijuan, XU Xiaodong. How does literature reading affect readers’ Theory of Mind? [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2020, 28(3): 434-442. |
[9] | ZHU Zhaoxia, LIU Li, CUI Lei, PENG Danling. The influence of writing on reading: Evidence from the contrast between traditional writing and typing [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2019, 27(5): 796-803. |
[10] | ZANG Chuanli, LU Zijia, ZHANG Zhichao. The role of semantic and syntactic information in parafoveal prcoessing during reading [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2019, 27(1): 11-19. |
[11] | ZHAO Jing. Skills of visual attention span in developmental dyslexia [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2019, 27(1): 20-26. |
[12] | YANG Jianfeng, DANG Min, ZHANG Rui, WANG Xiaojuan. The neural circuit of semantic processing and its dynamic cooperation with the neural circuit of phonological processing in reading Chinese characters [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2018, 26(3): 381-390. |
[13] | MENG Zhu, YAN Guoli. Mechanism of the irrelevant speech effect in reading: Is the interference determined by content, or process? [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2018, 26(2): 262-269. |
[14] | CHEN Tong, WU Zhen. Children’s distributive justice: The role of Theory of Mind [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2017, 25(8): 1299-1309. |
[15] | HUA Huimin, GU Junjuan, LIN Nan, LI Xingshan. Letter/character position encoding in visual word recognition [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2017, 25(7): 1132-1138. |
Viewed | ||||||
Full text |
|
|||||
Abstract |
|
|||||