Advances in Psychological Science ›› 2022, Vol. 30 ›› Issue (1): 51-64.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2022.00051
• Regular Articles • Previous Articles Next Articles
YAN Lizhu1, CHEN Yanxiu1, LIU Xun2, FU Shimin1, NAN Weizhi1()
Received:
2021-04-10
Online:
2022-01-15
Published:
2021-11-25
CLC Number:
YAN Lizhu, CHEN Yanxiu, LIU Xun, FU Shimin, NAN Weizhi. The flexibility of spatial-numerical associations and its internal mechanism[J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2022, 30(1): 51-64.
[1] | 邓之君, 吴慧中, 陈英和. (2017). 数字空间联结的工作记忆机制. 心理科学进展, 25(9), 1492-1502. |
[2] | 韩萌, 毛新瑞, 蔡梦彤, 贾茜, 郭春彦. (2017). 大小判断任务中正负号及其异同对SNARC效应的影响. 心理学报, 49(8), 995-1008. |
[3] | 胡林成, 熊哲宏. (2011). 刺激模拟量的空间表征: 面积和亮度的类SNARC效应. 心理科学, 34(1), 58-62. |
[4] | 金桂春, 王有智, 王丽. (2017). Simon效应对空间-数字反应编码联合效应的抑制作用. 心理与行为研究, 15(4), 489-494. |
[5] | 康武, 杨敏, 王丽平. (2013). SNARC效应:现状、理论及建议. 心理科学, 36(5), 1242-1248. |
[6] | 李雅君, 刘阳, 闻素霞. (2018). 书写习惯对数字空间表征SNARC效应的影响. 心理与行为研究, 16(5), 618-623. |
[7] | 刘雍江, 左全顺, 郭增辉, 李慧如, 韦磐石. (2018). 贵州少数民族儿童青少年SNARC效应的发展. 心理科学, 41(5), 116-122. |
[8] | 乔福强, 张恩涛, 陈功香. (2016). 情境对序数的空间表征之影响. 心理科学, 39(3), 566-572. |
[9] | 司继伟, 周超, 张传花, 仲蕾蕾. (2013). 不同加工深度非符号数量信息的SNARC效应:眼动证据. 心理学报, 45(1), 11-22. |
[10] | 孙玉, 司继伟, 黄碧娟. (2016). 分数的数量表征. 心理科学进展, 24(8), 1207-1216. |
[11] | 唐丹丹, 陈安涛, 李红, 朱海. (2020). 前额叶神经振荡活动反映了稳固的刺激冲突和反应冲突. 心理科学, 43(1), 23-32. |
[12] | 唐丹丹, 彭微微, 杨青松, 陈安涛, 陈雪飞. (2018). 刺激冲突和反应冲突的事件相关电位特征:中顶部P3. 心理与行为研究, 16(1), 31-36. |
[13] | 王力, 张栎文, 张明亮, 陈安涛. (2012). 视觉运动Simon效应和认知Simon效应的影响因素及机制. 心理科学进展, 20(5), 662-671. |
[14] | 魏萍, 周晓林. (2005). 从知觉负载理论来理解选择性注意. 心理科学进展, 13(4), 413-420. |
[15] | 徐晓东, 刘昌. (2006). 数字的空间特性. 心理科学进展, 14(6), 851-858. |
[16] |
Abrahamse, E., van Dijck, J. P., & Fias, W. (2016). How does working memory enable number-induced spatial biases? Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 977.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00977 pmid: 27445937 |
[17] |
Bächtold, D., Baumüller, M., & Brugger, P. (1998). Stimulus- response compatibility in representational space. Neuropsychologia, 36(8), 731-735.
pmid: 9751438 |
[18] |
Brysbaert, M. (1995). Arabic number reading: On the nature of the numerical scale and the origin of phonological recoding. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124(4), 434-452.
doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.124.4.434 URL |
[19] |
Bulf, H., de Hevia, M. D., & Macchi Cassia, V. (2016). Small on the left, large on the right: Numbers orient visual attention onto space in preverbal infants. Developmental Science, 19(3), 394-401.
doi: 10.1111/desc.12315 URL |
[20] |
Chen, Q., & Verguts, T. (2010). Beyond the mental number line: A neural network model of number-space interactions. Cognitive Psychology, 60(3), 218-240.
doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2010.01.001 pmid: 20138261 |
[21] | Cohen, , Kadosh, R., & Walsh, V. (2009). Numerical representation in the parietal lobes: Abstract or not abstract? Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 32, 313-328. |
[22] |
Cutini, S., Scarpa, F., Scatturin, P., Dell'Acqua, R., & Zorzi, M. (2014). Number-space interactions in the human parietal cortex: Enlightening the SNARC effect with functional near-infrared spectroscopy. Cerebral Cortex, 24(2), 444-451.
doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs321 URL |
[23] |
Daar, M., & Pratt, J. (2008). Digits affect actions: the SNARC effect and response selection. Cortex, 44(4), 400-405.
doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2007.12.003 URL |
[24] | Dehaene, S. (1989). The psychophysics of numerical comparison: A reexamination of apparently incompatible data. Attention Perception & Psychophysics, 45(6), 557-566. |
[25] |
Dehaene, S. (1992). Varieties of numerical abilities. Cognition, 44(1-2), 1-42.
pmid: 1511583 |
[26] |
Dehaene, S., Bossini, S., & Giraux, P. (1993). The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122(3), 371-396.
doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.122.3.371 URL |
[27] |
Dehaene, S., Dupoux, E., & Mehler, J. (1990). Is numerical comparison digital? Analogical and symbolic effects in two-digit number comparison. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16(3), 626-641.
doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.16.3.626 URL |
[28] |
Dehaene, S., Molko, N., Cohen, L., & Wilson, A. J. (2004). Arithmetic and the brain. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 14(2), 218-224.
doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2004.03.008 URL |
[29] |
De Jong, R., Liang, C.-C., & Lauber, E. (1994). Conditional and unconditional automaticity: A dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus-response correspondence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20(4), 731-750.
doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.20.4.731 URL |
[30] |
Deng, Z. J., Chen, Y. H., Zhang, M., Li, Y. J., & Zhu, X. S. (2018). The association of number and space under different tasks: Insight from a process perspective. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 957.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00957 URL |
[31] |
Deng, Z. J., Chen, Y. H., Zhu, X. S., & Li, Y. J. (2017). The effect of working memory load on the SNARC effect: Maybe tasks have a word to say. Memory & Cognition, 45(3), 428-441.
doi: 10.3758/s13421-016-0676-x URL |
[32] |
Didino, D., Breil, C., & Knops, A. (2019). The influence of semantic processing and response latency on the SNARC effect. Acta Psychologica, 196, 75-86.
doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2019.04.008 URL |
[33] |
Drucker, C. B., & Brannon, E. M. (2014). Rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) map number onto space. Cognition, 132(1), 57-67.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.03.011 URL |
[34] |
Duncan, J. (1980). The locus of interference in the perception of simultaneous stimuli. Psychological Review, 87(3), 272-300.
pmid: 7384344 |
[35] | Felisatti, A., Laubrock, J., Shaki, S., & Fischer, M. H. (2020). A biological foundation for spatial-numerical associations:The brain's asymmetric frequency tuning. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1477(1), 44-53. |
[36] |
Fias, W., Brysbaert, M., Geypens, F., & d'Ydewalle, G. (1996). The Importance of Magnitude Information in Numerical Processing: Evidence from the SNARC Effect. Mathematical Cognition, 2(1), 95-110.
doi: 10.1080/135467996387552 URL |
[37] |
Fias, W., & van Dijck, J. P. (2016). The temporary nature of number-space interactions. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 70(1), 33-40.
doi: 10.1037/cep0000071 URL |
[38] |
Fischer, M. H., Mills, R. A., & Shaki, S. (2010). How to cook a SNARC: Number placement in text rapidly changes spatial-numerical associations. Brain and Cognition, 72(3), 333-336.
doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2009.10.010 pmid: 19917517 |
[39] |
Fischer, M. H., & Shaki, S. (2014). Spatial associations in numerical cognition--from single digits to arithmetic. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(8), 1461-1483.
doi: 10.1080/17470218.2014.927515 URL |
[40] |
Fischer, M. H., & Shaki, S. (2016). Measuring spatial-numerical associations: Evidence for a purely conceptual link. Psychological Research, 80(1), 109-112.
doi: 10.1007/s00426-015-0646-0 pmid: 25617061 |
[41] |
Fischer, M. H., Warlop, N., Hill, R. L., & Fias, W. (2004). Oculomotor bias induced by number perception. Experimental Psychology, 51(2), 91-97.
pmid: 15114901 |
[42] |
Fitousi, D., & Algom, D. (2020). A model for two-digit number processing based on a joint Garner and system factorial technology analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology General, 149(4), 676-700.
doi: 10.1037/xge0000679 URL |
[43] |
Fumarola, A., Prpic, V., Da Pos, O., Murgia, M., Umiltà, C., & Agostini, T. (2014). Automatic spatial association for luminance. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 76(3), 759-765.
doi: 10.3758/s13414-013-0614-y URL |
[44] |
Galton, F. (1880). Visualised numerals. Nature, 21, 252-256.
doi: 10.1038/021252a0 URL |
[45] |
Gevers, W., Caessens, B., & Fias, W. (2005). Towards a common processing architecture underlying Simon and SNARC effects. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 17(5), 659-673.
doi: 10.1080/09541440540000112 URL |
[46] |
Gevers, W., Lammertyn, J., Notebaert, W., Verguts, T., & Fias, W. (2006). Automatic response activation of implicit spatial information: Evidence from the SNARC effect. Acta Psychologica (Amst), 122(3), 221-233.
doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2005.11.004 URL |
[47] |
Gevers, W., Ratinckx, E., De Baene, W., & Fias, W. (2006). Further evidence that the SNARC effect is processed along a dual-route architecture: Evidence from the lateralized readiness potential. Experimental Psychology, 53(1), 58-68.
pmid: 16610273 |
[48] | Gevers, W., Reynvoet, B., & Fias, W. (2003). The mental representation of ordinal sequences is spatially organized. Cognition, 87(3), B87-B95. |
[49] |
Gevers, W., Santens, S., Dhooge, E., Chen, Q., Van den Bossche, L., Fias, W., & Verguts, T. (2010). Verbal-spatial and visuospatial coding of number-space interactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139(1), 180-190.
doi: 10.1037/a0017688 URL |
[50] |
Gevers, W., Verguts, T., Reynvoet, B., Caessens, B., & Fias, W. (2006). Numbers and space: a computational model of the SNARC effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32(1), 32-44.
doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.32.1.32 URL |
[51] | Ginsburg, V., & Gevers, W. (2015). Spatial coding of ordinal information in short- and long-term memory. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9(8), 8. |
[52] | Giorgio, E. D., Lunghi, M., Rugani, R., Regolin, L., Dalla Barba, B., Vallortigara, G., & Simion, F. (2019). A mental number line in human newborns. Developmental Science, 22(6), e12801. |
[53] |
Gut, M., Szumska, I., Wasilewska, M., & Jaskowśki, P. (2012). Are low and high number magnitudes processed differently while resolving the conflict evoked by the SNARC effect? International Journal of Psychophysiology, 85(1), 7-16.
doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2012.02.007 URL |
[54] |
Hartmann, M., Gashaj, V., Stahnke, A., & Mast, F. W. (2014). There is more than "more is up": Hand and foot responses reverse the vertical association of number magnitudes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40(4), 1401-1414.
doi: 10.1037/a0036686 URL |
[55] |
He, D. X., He, X. Y., Zhao, T. T., Wang, J., Li, L. Z., & Louwerse, M. (2020). Does Number Perception Cause Automatic Shifts of Spatial Attention? A Study of the Att-SNARC Effect in Numbers and Chinese Months. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 680.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00680 URL |
[56] |
Herrera, A., Macizo, P., & Semenza, C. (2008). The role of working memory in the association between number magnitude and space. Acta Psychologica (Amst), 128(2), 225-237.
doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2008.01.002 URL |
[57] |
Hirsch, P., Nolden, S., & Koch, I. (2017). Higher-order cognitive control in dual tasks: Evidence from task-pair switching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 43(3), 569-580.
doi: 10.1037/xhp0000309 URL |
[58] |
Hung, Y. H., Hung, D. L., Tzeng, O. J.-L., & Wu, D. H. (2008). Flexible spatial mapping of different notations of numbers in Chinese readers. Cognition, 106(3), 1441-1450.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.04.017 URL |
[59] |
Ito, Y., & Hatta, T. (2004). Spatial structure of quantitative representation of numbers: Evidence from the SNARC effect. Memory & Cognition, 32(4), 662-673.
doi: 10.3758/BF03195857 URL |
[60] |
Keus, I. M., Jenks, K. M., & Schwarz, W. (2005). Psychophysiological evidence that the SNARC effect has its functional locus in a response selection stage. Cognitive Brain Research, 24(1), 48-56.
doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.12.005 URL |
[61] |
Keus, I. M., & Schwarz, W. (2005). Searching for the functional locus of the SNARC effect: Evidence for a response-related origin. Memory & Cognition, 33(4), 681-695.
doi: 10.3758/BF03195335 URL |
[62] |
Kong, F., Zhao, J. J., & You, X. Q. (2012). Components representation of negative numbers: Evidence from auditory stimuli detection and number classification tasks. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(4), 691-701.
doi: 10.1080/17470218.2011.622048 URL |
[63] |
Kornblum, S., Hasbroucq, T., & Osman, A. (1990). Dimensional overlap: Cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility--A model and taxonomy. Psychological Review, 97(2), 253-270.
pmid: 2186425 |
[64] |
Krause, F., Lindemann, O., Toni, I., & Bekkering, H. (2014). Different brains process numbers differently: Structural bases of individual differences in spatial and nonspatial number representations. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 26(4), 768-776.
doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00518 URL |
[65] |
Leuthold, H. (2011). The Simon effect in cognitive electrophysiology: A short review. Acta Psychologica (Amst), 136(2), 203-211.
doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.08.001 URL |
[66] |
Li, Q., Nan, W. Z., Wang, K., & Liu, X. (2014). Independent processing of stimulus-stimulus and stimulus-response conflicts. Plos One, 9(2), e89249.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0089249 URL |
[67] |
Lindemann, O., Abolafia, J. M., Pratt, J., & Bekkering, H. (2008). Coding strategies in number space: Memory requirements influence spatial-numerical associations. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61(4), 515-524.
doi: 10.1080/17470210701728677 URL |
[68] |
Liu, X., Park, Y., Gu, X. S., & Fan, J. (2010). Dimensional overlap accounts for independence and integration of stimulus-response compatibility effects. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72(6), 1710-1720.
doi: 10.3758/APP.72.6.1710 URL |
[69] |
Lonnemann, J., Linkersdorfer, J., Nagler, T., Hasselhorn, M., & Lindberg, S. (2013). Spatial representations of numbers and letters in children. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 544.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00544 pmid: 23970877 |
[70] | Mapelli, D., Rusconi, E., & Umiltà, C. (2003). The SNARC effect: An instance of the Simon effect? Cognition, 88(3), B1-B10. |
[71] |
Marois, R., Larson, J. M., Chun, M. M., & Shima, D. (2006). Response-specific sources of dual-task interference in human pre-motor cortex. Psychological Research, 70(6), 436-447.
doi: 10.1007/s00426-005-0022-6 URL |
[72] |
Moro, S. B., Dell'Acqua, R., & Cutini, S. (2018). The SNARC effect is not a unitary phenomenon. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25(2), 688-695.
doi: 10.3758/s13423-017-1408-3 URL |
[73] |
Moyer, R. S., & Landauer, T. K. (1967). Time required for judgements of numerical inequality. Nature, 215, 1519-1520.
doi: 10.1038/2151519a0 URL |
[74] |
Müller, D., & Schwarz, W. (2007). Exploring the mental number line: Evidence from a dual-task paradigm. Psychological Research, 71(5), 598-613.
doi: 10.1007/s00426-006-0070-6 URL |
[75] |
Myachykov, A., Cangelosi, A., Ellis, R., & Fischer, M. H. (2015). The oculomotor resonance effect in spatial-numerical mapping. Acta Psychologica (Amst), 161, 162-169.
doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2015.09.006 URL |
[76] |
Nan, W. Z., Yan, L. Z., Yang, G. C., Liu, X., & Fu, S. M. (2021). Two processing stages of the SNARC effect. Psychological Research. doi: 10.1007/s00426-021-01506-5
doi: 10.1007/s00426-021-01506-5 |
[77] |
Nuerk, H. C., Iversen, W., & Willmes, K. (2004). Notational modulation of the SNARC and the MARC (linguistic markedness of response codes) effect. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A: Human Experimental Psychology, 57(5), 835-863.
doi: 10.1080/02724980343000512 URL |
[78] |
Nuerk, H. C., Wood, G., & Willmes, K. (2005). The universal SNARC effect: The association between number magnitude and space is amodal. Experimental Psychology, 52(3), 187-194.
doi: 10.1027/1618-3169.52.3.187 URL |
[79] |
Oberauer, K., Awh, E., & Sutterer, D. W. (2017). The role of long-term memory in a test of visual working memory: Proactive facilitation but no proactive interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 43(1), 1-22.
doi: 10.1037/xlm0000302 URL |
[80] |
Pinto, M., Pellegrino, M., Marson, F., Lasaponara, S., & Doricchi, F. (2019). Reconstructing the origins of the space-number association: Spatial and number-magnitude codes must be used jointly to elicit spatially organised mental number lines. Cognition, 190, 143-156.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2019.04.032 URL |
[81] |
Prado, J., Van der Henst, J. B., & Noveck, I. A. (2008). Spatial associations in relational reasoning: Evidence for a SNARC-like effect. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61(8), 1143-1150.
doi: 10.1080/17470210801954777 URL |
[82] |
Pressigout, A., Charvillat, A., Mersad, K., & Doré-Mazars, K. (2019). Time dependency of the SNARC effect for different number formats: Evidence from saccadic responses. Psychological Research, 83(7), 1485-1495.
doi: 10.1007/s00426-018-1010-y pmid: 29633009 |
[83] |
Proctor, R. W., & Cho, Y. S. (2006). Polarity correspondence: A general principle for performance of speeded binary classification tasks. Psychological Bulletin, 132(3), 416-442.
pmid: 16719568 |
[84] |
Prpic, V., Fumarola, A., De Tommaso, M., Luccio, R., Murgia, M., & Agostini, T. (2016). Separate mechanisms for magnitude and order processing in the spatial-numerical association of response codes (SNARC) effect: The strange case of musical note values. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 42(8), 1241-1251.
doi: 10.1037/xhp0000217 URL |
[85] |
Ratinckx, E., & Brysbaert, M. (2002). Interhemispheric stroop-like interference in number comparison: Evidence for strong interhemispheric integration of semantic number information. Neuropsychology, 16(2), 217-229.
pmid: 11949714 |
[86] | Riello, M., & Rusconi, E. (2011). Unimanual SNARC Effect: Hand Matters. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 372. |
[87] |
Rusconi, E., Bueti, D., Walsh, V., & Butterworth, B. (2011). Contribution of frontal cortex to the spatial representation of number. Cortex, 47(1), 2-13.
doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2009.08.005 pmid: 19762013 |
[88] |
Scerrati, E., Lugli, L., Nicoletti, R., & Umiltà, C. (2017). Comparing Stroop-like and Simon Effects on Perceptual Features. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 17815.
doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-18185-1 pmid: 29259275 |
[89] |
Schuller, A.-M., Hoffmann, D., Goffaux, V., & Schiltz, C. (2014). Shifts of spatial attention cued by irrelevant numbers: Electrophysiological evidence from a target discrimination task. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 27(4), 442-458.
doi: 10.1080/20445911.2014.946419 URL |
[90] |
Schwarz, W., & Keus, I. M. (2004). Moving the eyes along the mental number line: Comparing SNARC effects with saccadic and manual responses. Perception & Psychophysics, 66(4), 651-664.
doi: 10.3758/BF03194909 URL |
[91] |
Seron, X., Pesenti, M., Noël, M. P., Deloche, G., & Cornet, J. A. (1992). Images of numbers, or “When 98 is upper left and 6 sky blue”. Cognition, 44(1-2), 159-196.
doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(92)90049-N URL |
[92] |
Shaki, S., Fischer, M. H., & Petrusic, W. M. (2009). Reading habits for both words and numbers contribute to the SNARC effect. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16(2), 328-331.
doi: 10.3758/PBR.16.2.328 URL |
[93] |
Shaki, S., & Gevers, W. (2011). Cultural Characteristics Dissociate Magnitude and Ordinal Information Processing. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 42(4), 639-650.
doi: 10.1177/0022022111406100 URL |
[94] |
Spinelli, G., & Lupker, S. J. (2020). Item-specific control of attention in the stroop task: Contingency learning is not the whole story in the item-specific proportion-congruent effect. Memory & Cognition, 48(3), 426-435.
doi: 10.3758/s13421-019-00980-y URL |
[95] |
Sternberg, S. (1969). The discovery of processing stages: Extensions of Donders' method. Acta Psychologica (Amst), 30, 276-315.
doi: 10.1016/0001-6918(69)90055-9 URL |
[96] |
Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18(6), 643-662.
doi: 10.1037/h0054651 URL |
[97] |
Tlauka, M. (2002). Switching imagined viewpoints: the effects of viewing angle and layout size. British Journal of Psychology, 93(2), 193-201.
doi: 10.1348/000712602162535 URL |
[98] | Tombu, M. N., Asplund, C. L., Dux, P. E., Godwin, D., Martin, J. W., & Marois, R. (2011). A Unified attentional bottleneck in the human brain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108(33), 13426-13431. |
[99] |
Toomarian, E. Y., & Hubbard, E. M. (2018). On the genesis of spatial-numerical associations: Evolutionary and cultural factors co-construct the mental number line. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 90, 184-199.
doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.04.010 URL |
[100] |
Treccani, B., Cubelli, R., Della Sala, S., & Umiltà, C. (2009). Flanker and Simon effects interact at the response selection stage. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62(9), 1784-1804.
doi: 10.1080/17470210802557751 URL |
[101] |
Treccani, B., Milanese, N., & Umilta, C. (2010). Influence on Simon and SNARC effects of a nonspatial stimulus- response mapping: Between-task logical recoding. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36(5), 1239-1254.
doi: 10.1037/a0019239 URL |
[102] |
Tschentscher, N., Hauk, O., Fischer, M. H., & Pulvermuller, F. (2012). You can count on the motor cortex: Finger counting habits modulate motor cortex activation evoked by numbers. Neuroimage, 59(4), 3139-3148.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.11.037 pmid: 22133748 |
[103] |
Vallortigara, G. (2017). Comparative cognition of number and space: The case of geometry and of the mental number line. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B Biological Sciences, 373(1740), 20170120.
doi: 10.1098/rstb.2017.0120 URL |
[104] |
van Dijck, J. P., & Fias, W. (2011). A working memory account for spatial-numerical associations. Cognition, 119(1), 114-119.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2010.12.013 URL |
[105] |
van Dijck, J. P., Gevers, W., & Fias, W. (2009). Numbers are associated with different types of spatial information depending on the task. Cognition, 113(2), 248-253.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.08.005 URL |
[106] |
van Galen, M. S., & Reitsma, P. (2008). Developing access to number magnitude: A study of the SNARC effect in 7-to 9-year-olds. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 101(2), 99-113.
doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2008.05.001 URL |
[107] |
van Opstal, F., & Verguts, T. (2011). The origins of the numerical distance effect: The same-different task. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 23(1), 112-120.
doi: 10.1080/20445911.2011.466796 URL |
[108] |
Verguts, T., Fias, W., & Stevens, M. (2005). A model of exact small-number representation. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12(1), 66-80.
doi: 10.3758/BF03196349 URL |
[109] |
Viarouge, A., Hubbard, E. M., & Dehaene, S. (2014). The organization of spatial reference frames involved in the SNARC effect. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(8), 1484-1499.
doi: 10.1080/17470218.2014.897358 URL |
[110] |
Vu, K.-P. L., & Proctor, R. W. (2001). Determinants of right-left and top-bottom prevalence for two-dimensional spatial compatibility. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27(4), 813-828.
doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.27.4.813 URL |
[111] |
Wang, X. T., Du, F., He, X. S., & Zhang, K. (2014). Enhanced spatial stimulus-response mapping near the hands: The Simon effect is modulated by hand-stimulus proximity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40(6), 2252-2265.
doi: 10.1037/a0038140 URL |
[112] |
Weis, T., Estner, B., Krick, C. M., Reith, W., & Lachmann, T. (2015). SNARC meets SPARC in fMRI--Interdependence of compatibility effects depends on semantic content. Neuropsychologia, 77, 331-338.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.09.018 URL |
[113] |
Weis, T., Estner, B., van Leeuwen, C., & Lachmann, T. (2016). SNARC (spatial-numerical association of response codes) meets SPARC (spatial-pitch association of response codes): Automaticity and interdependency in compatibility effects. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69(7), 1366-1383.
doi: 10.1080/17470218.2015.1082142 URL |
[114] |
Wiegand, K., & Wascher, E. (2005). Dynamic aspects of stimulus-response correspondence: Evidence for two mechanisms involved in the Simon effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31(3), 453-464.
doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.31.3.453 URL |
[115] |
Wiegand, K., & Wascher, E. (2007). Response coding in the Simon task. Psychological Research, 71(4), 401-410.
doi: 10.1007/s00426-005-0027-1 URL |
[116] |
Yan, L. Z., Yang, G. C., Nan, W. Z., Liu, X., & Fu, S. M. (2021). The SNARC effect occurs in the response-selection stage. Acta Psychologica (Amst), 215, 103292.
doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2021.103292 URL |
[117] |
Yang, G. C., Nan, W. Z., Zheng, Y., Wu, H. Y., Li, Q., & Liu, X. (2017). Distinct cognitive control mechanisms as revealed by modality-specific conflict adaptation effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 43(4), 807-818.
doi: 10.1037/xhp0000351 URL |
[118] | Zhang, P., Cao, B. H., & Li, F. H. (2020). SNARC effect modulated by central executive control: revealed in a cue-based trisection task. Psychological Research. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-020-01407-z |
[119] |
Zhao, T. T., He, X. Y., Zhao, X. R., Huang, J. R., Zhang, W., Wu, S., & Chen, Q. (2018). The influence of time units on the flexibility of the spatial numerical association of response codes effect. British Journal of Psychology, 109(2), 299-320.
doi: 10.1111/bjop.2018.109.issue-2 URL |
[120] |
Zhou, D. D., Luo, J., Yi, Z. Z., Li, Y., Yang, S. T., Verguts, T., & Chen, Q. (2020). The hand-lateralization of spatial associations in working memory and long-term memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 73(8), 1150-1161.
doi: 10.1177/1747021819899533 URL |
[121] |
Zorzi, M., Priftis, K., & Umiltà, C. (2002). Neglect disrupts the mental number line. Nature, 417, 138-139.
doi: 10.1038/417138a URL |
[1] | YIN Kui, ZHANG Kaili, XING Lu, ZHAO Jing. Impact of ambidextrous leadership on ambidextrous performance: Based on Human Capital Theory [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2020, 28(5): 692-710. |
[2] | ZHOU Qing, WU Qi. Evolution of intergroup bias: Adaptation to the threats of violence and disease [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2019, 27(12): 2084-2096. |
[3] | LI Heng. Implicit space-time mappings on the front and back axis and their influencing factors [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2018, 26(6): 975-983. |
[4] | DENG Zhijun, WU Huizhong, CHEN Yinghe. The working memory account of the spatial-numerical associations [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2017, 25(9): 1492-1502. |
[5] | CHEN Jie, LIU Lei, WANG Rong, SHEN Haizhou. The effect of musical training on executive functions [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2017, 25(11): 1854-1864. |
[6] | XIE Ji-Hong;LIU Hua-Shan. Behavioral Characteristics, Mechanism and Influencing Factors of Nonconscious Goal Pursuit [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2012, 20(12): 2069-2078. |
[7] | LEI Ming;DAI Yan;XIAO Xiao;ZENG Can;ZHANG Qing-Lin. The Mechanism of Resilience: Evidence from Trait-Resilient Individual [J]. , 2011, 19(6): 874-882. |
[8] | JIANG Jun;CHEN Xue-Fei;CHEN An-Tao. The Effect of Positive Mood on Visual Attention and Its Mechanism [J]. , 2011, 19(5): 701-711. |
[9] | TANG Yuan-Hong;LU Xie-Feng;LI Ke. The Concept, Measuring Strategy and Applying of Person-Organization Fit: Based on the Reflection of Interactivity and Flexibility [J]. , 2010, 18(11): 1762-1770. |
[10] | Xu Xiaodong;Liu Chang. The Spatial Character of Number [J]. , 2006, 14(6): 851-858. |
Viewed | ||||||
Full text |
|
|||||
Abstract |
|
|||||