Advances in Psychological Science ›› 2021, Vol. 29 ›› Issue (10): 1887-1900.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2021.01887
• Regular Articles • Previous Articles
Received:
2020-11-12
Online:
2021-10-15
Published:
2021-08-23
Contact:
XIAO Feng
E-mail:xiaofeng19850328@gmail.com
CLC Number:
LI Yi, XIAO Feng. Automatic perspective taking: The debate between implicit mentalizing and submentalizing[J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2021, 29(10): 1887-1900.
内隐心智化 | 潜心智化 | |
---|---|---|
定义 | 自动加工他人视角 | 不涉及心理状态的加工, 例如注意定向, 分心等 |
一致性效应 | 自动加工他人视角, 干扰自己视角判断 | 他人朝向方向和整体场景冲突 |
点观点采择:异我中心干扰效应 | 自动加工他人视角下的点数 | 身体朝向线索导致注意自动定向 |
社会Simon任务:空间一致性效应 | 自动加工他人想法 | 考虑同盟者的空间特征而非心理状态 |
模糊数字任务:线索一致性效应 | 自动加工他人视角数字 | 以自我为中心和以对象为中心的两套参照系空间编码冲突 |
预期注视范式:分心干扰效应 | 被试会首先、并更长时间地注视化身最后一次看到球的位置 | 被试被人物头部朝向干扰, 减少了对球的关注, 更少记忆球的移动 |
影响因素 | 社会相关性; 线索的视觉归因状态; 社会视角的获取 |
内隐心智化 | 潜心智化 | |
---|---|---|
定义 | 自动加工他人视角 | 不涉及心理状态的加工, 例如注意定向, 分心等 |
一致性效应 | 自动加工他人视角, 干扰自己视角判断 | 他人朝向方向和整体场景冲突 |
点观点采择:异我中心干扰效应 | 自动加工他人视角下的点数 | 身体朝向线索导致注意自动定向 |
社会Simon任务:空间一致性效应 | 自动加工他人想法 | 考虑同盟者的空间特征而非心理状态 |
模糊数字任务:线索一致性效应 | 自动加工他人视角数字 | 以自我为中心和以对象为中心的两套参照系空间编码冲突 |
预期注视范式:分心干扰效应 | 被试会首先、并更长时间地注视化身最后一次看到球的位置 | 被试被人物头部朝向干扰, 减少了对球的关注, 更少记忆球的移动 |
影响因素 | 社会相关性; 线索的视觉归因状态; 社会视角的获取 |
[1] | 陈友庆, 郭本禹. (2006). 聋儿的心理理论发展特点及影响因素. 心理科学进展, 14(3), 382-388. |
[2] | 鲁上, 刘烨, 傅小兰. (2013). 头部朝向在社会性注意转移中的作用. 心理科学进展, 21(2), 211-219. |
[3] | 潘威, 汪寅, 陈巍. (2017). 心智化社会认知观的演变及发展——来自潜心智化的思考. 心理科学, 40(5), 1274-1279. |
[4] | 王伟平, 苏彦捷. (2006). 孤独症儿童基于眼睛注视的社会性注意. 中国特殊教育, 6, 12-17. |
[5] | 王雨晴, 游旭群, 焦健, 谌鹏飞. (2015). 观点采择:基于自我的推理及其个体差异. 心理学报, 47(8), 1039-1049. |
[6] |
Apperly, I. A. (2013). What is "theory of mind"? Concepts, cognitive processes and individual differences. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(5), 825-839.
doi: 10.1080/17470218.2012.676055 URL |
[7] |
Apperly, I. A., & Butterfill, S. A. (2009). Do humans have two systems to track beliefs and belief-like states? Psychological Review, 116(4), 953-970.
doi: 10.1037/a0016923 pmid: 19839692 |
[8] |
Apperly, I. A., Samson, D., & Humphreys, G. W. (2005). Domain-specificity and theory of mind: Evaluating neuropsychological evidence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(12), 572-577.
pmid: 16271505 |
[9] |
Baker, L. J., Levin, D. T., & Saylor, M. M. (2016). The extent of default visual perspective taking in complex layouts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 42(4), 508-516.
doi: 10.1037/xhp0000164 URL |
[10] |
Boffel, C., & Musseler, J. (2018). Perceived ownership of avatars influences visual perspective taking. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 743.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00743 URL |
[11] |
Boffel, C., & Musseler, J. (2019a). Action effect consistency and body ownership in the avatar-Simon task. Plos One, 14(8), e0220817.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0220817 URL |
[12] |
Boffel, C., & Musseler, J. (2019b). Visual perspective taking for avatars in a Simon task. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 81(1), 158-172.
doi: 10.3758/s13414-018-1573-0 URL |
[13] |
Bukowski, H., Hietanen, J. K., & Samson, D. (2015). From gaze cueing to perspective taking: Revisiting the claim that we automatically compute where or what other people are looking at. Visual Cognition, 23(8), 1020-1042.
pmid: 26924936 |
[14] |
Bukowski, H., & Samson, D. (2015). Can emotions influence level-1 visual perspective taking? Cognitive Neuroscience, 7(1-4), 182-191.
doi: 10.1080/17588928.2015.1112773 URL |
[15] | Burnside, K., Severdija, V., & Poulin-Dubois, D. (2019). Infants attribute false beliefs to a toy crane. Developmental Science, 23(2), e12887. |
[16] | Capozzi, F., & Ristic, J. (2018). How attention gates social interactions. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1426(1), 179-198. |
[17] |
Capozzi, F., & Ristic, J. (2020). Attention and mentalizing? Reframing a debate on social orienting of attention. Visual Cognition, 28(2), 97-105.
doi: 10.1080/13506285.2020.1725206 URL |
[18] |
Carter, R. M., & Huettel, S. A. (2013). A nexus model of the temporal-parietal junction. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(7), 328-336.
doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.05.007 URL |
[19] |
Catmur, C., Santiesteban, I., Conway, J. R., Heyes, C., & Bird, G. (2016). Avatars and arrows in the brain. Neuroimage, 132(2016), 8-10.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.02.021 URL |
[20] |
Clements-Stephens, A. M., Vasiljevic, K., Murray, A. J., & Shelton, A. L. (2013). The role of potential agents in making spatial perspective taking social. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 497.
doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00497 pmid: 24046735 |
[21] |
Cole, G. G., Atkinson, M., Le, A. T. D., & Smith, D. T. (2016). Do humans spontaneously take the perspective of others? Acta Psychologica, 164(2016), 165-168.
doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2016.01.007 URL |
[22] |
Conway, J. R., Lee, D., Ojaghi, M., Catmur, C., & Bird, G. (2017). Submentalizing or mentalizing in a Level 1 perspective-taking task: A cloak and goggles test. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 43(3), 454-465.
doi: 10.1037/xhp0000319 URL |
[23] | Dolk, T., Hommel, B., Colzato, L. S., Schutz-Bosbach, S., Prinz, W., & Liepelt, R. (2011). How "social" is the social Simon effect? Frontiers in Psychology, 2(84), 1-9. |
[24] |
Eack, S. M., Wojtalik, J. A., Keshavan, M. S., & Minshew, N. J. (2017). Social-cognitive brain function and connectivity during visual perspective-taking in autism and schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 183, 102-109.
doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2017.03.009 URL |
[25] |
Elekes, F., Varga, M., & Király, I. (2016). Evidence for spontaneous level-2 perspective taking in adults. Consciousness and Cognition, 41(2016), 93-103.
doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2016.02.010 URL |
[26] |
Ferguson, H. J., Brunsdon, V. E. A., & Bradford, E. E. F. (2018). Age of avatar modulates the altercentric bias in a visual perspective-taking task: ERP and behavioral evidence. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 18, 1298-1319. Advance online publication. doi: 10.3758/s13415-018-0641-1
doi: 10.3758/s13415-018-0641-1 |
[27] |
Freundlieb, M., Kovacs, A. M., & Sebanz, N. (2016). When do humans spontaneously adopt another's visuospatial perspective? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 42(3), 401-412.
doi: 10.1037/xhp0000153 URL |
[28] |
Freundlieb, M., Kovács, Á. M., & Sebanz, N. (2018). Reading your mind while you are reading—Evidence for spontaneous visuospatial perspective taking during a semantic categorization task. Psychological Science, 29(4), 614-622.
doi: 10.1177/0956797617740973 pmid: 29447070 |
[29] |
Freundlieb, M., Sebanz, N., & Kovács, Á. M. (2017). Out of your sight, out of my mind: Knowledge about another person's visual access modulates spontaneous visuospatial perspective-taking. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 43(6), 1065-1072.
doi: 10.1037/xhp0000379 URL |
[30] |
Furlanetto, T., Becchio, C., Samson, D., & Apperly, I. (2016). Altercentric interference in level 1 visual perspective taking reflects the ascription of mental states, not submentalizing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 42(2), 158-163.
doi: 10.1037/xhp0000138 URL |
[31] |
Gardner, M. R., Bileviciute, A. P., & Edmonds, C. J. (2018). Implicit mentalising during level-1 visual perspective-taking indicated by dissociation with attention orienting. Vision, 2(1), 1-10.
doi: 10.3390/vision2010001 URL |
[32] |
Gobel, M. S., Tufft, M. R. A., & Richardson, D. C. (2017). Social beliefs and visual attention: How the social relevance of a cue influences spatial orienting. Cognitive Science, 42(S1), 161-185.
doi: 10.1111/cogs.12529 URL |
[33] |
Guagnano, D., Rusconi, E., & Umiltà, C. A. J. C. (2010). Sharing a task or sharing space? On the effect of the confederate in action coding in a detection task. Cognition, 114(3), 348-355.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.10.008 pmid: 19914615 |
[34] |
Gunalp, P., Moossaian, T., & Hegarty, M. (2019). Spatial perspective taking: Effects of social, directional, and interactive cues. Memory Cognition, 47(5), 1031-1043.
doi: 10.3758/s13421-019-00910-y URL |
[35] |
Hare, B., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2001). Do chimpanzees know what conspecifics know? Animal Behaviour, 61(1), 139-151.
doi: 10.1006/anbe.2000.1518 URL |
[36] |
Heyes, C. (2014). Submentalizing: I am not really reading your mind. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 9(2), 131-143.
doi: 10.1177/1745691613518076 URL |
[37] | Heyes, C. M., & Frith, C. D. (2014). The cultural evolution of mind reading. Science, 344(6190), 1357-1364. |
[38] |
Hommel, B. (1996). S-R compatibility effects without response uncertainty. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49(3), 546-571.
doi: 10.1080/713755643 URL |
[39] |
Ji, H., Wang, L., & Jiang, Y. (2020). Cross-category adaptation of reflexive social attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 149(11), 2145-2153.
doi: 10.1037/xge0000766 URL |
[40] |
Joyce, K., Schenke, K., Bayliss, A., & Bach, P. (2015). Looking ahead: Anticipatory cueing of attention to objects others will look at. Cognitive Neuroscience, 7(1-4), 74-81.
doi: 10.1080/17588928.2015.1112773 URL |
[41] |
Kaiser, S., Walther, S., Nennig, E., Kronmüller, K., Mundt, C., Weisbrod, M.,... Vogeley, K. (2008). Gender-specific strategy use and neural correlates in a spatial perspective taking task. Neuropsychologia, 46(10), 2524-2531.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.04.013 URL |
[42] |
Kano, F., Call, J., & Krupenye, C. (2020). Primates pass dynamically social Anticipatory-looking false-belief tests. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 24(10), 777-778.
doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2020.07.003 URL |
[43] |
Kano, F., Krupenye, C., Hirata, S., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2017). Submentalizing cannot explain belief-based action anticipation in apes. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21(9), 633-634.
doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2017.06.011 URL |
[44] |
Kano, F., Krupenye, C., Hirata, S., Tomonaga, M., & Call, J. (2019). Great apes use self-experience to anticipate an agent's action in a false-belief test. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 116(42), 20904-20909.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1910095116 URL |
[45] |
Kovacs, A. M., Teglas, E., & Endress, A. D. (2010). The social sense: Susceptibility to others' beliefs in human infants and adults. Science, 330(6012), 1830-1834.
doi: 10.1126/science.1190792 URL |
[46] |
Krupenye, C., Kano, F., Hirata, S., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2016). Great apes anticipate that other individuals will act according to false beliefs. Science, 354(6308), 110-114.
doi: 10.1126/science.aaf8110 URL |
[47] | Krupenye, C., Kano, F., Hirata, S., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2017). A test of the submentalizing hypothesis: Apes' performance in a false belief task inanimate control. Communicative & Integrative Biology, 10(4), e1343771. |
[48] | Kuhbandner, C. (2010). The role of positive and negative affect in the “mirroring” of other persons' actions. Cognition & Emotion, 24(7), 1182-1190. |
[49] |
Kuhn-Popp, N., Sodian, B., Sommer, M., Dohnel, K., & Meinhardt, J. (2013). Same or different? ERP correlates of pretense and false belief reasoning in children. Neuroscience, 248(2013), 488-498.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2013.06.017 URL |
[50] |
Langton, S. R. H. (2018). I don't see it your way: The dot perspective task does not gauge spontaneous perspective taking. Vision, 2(6), 1-17.
doi: 10.3390/vision2010001 URL |
[51] |
Marotta, A., Lupianez, J., Martella, D., & Casagrande, M. (2012). Eye gaze versus arrows as spatial cues: Two qualitatively different modes of attentional selection. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38(2), 326-335.
doi: 10.1037/a0023959 URL |
[52] |
Marshall, J., Gollwitzer, A., & Santos, L. R. (2018). Does altercentric interference rely on mentalizing?: Results from two level-1 perspective-taking tasks. Plos One, 13(3), e0194101.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0194101 URL |
[53] |
Martin, A. K., Dzafic, I., Ramdave, S., & Meinzer, M. (2017). Causal evidence for taskspecific involvement of the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex in human social cognition. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12(8), 1209-1218.
doi: 10.1093/scan/nsx063 URL |
[54] |
McCleery, J. P., Surtees, A. D. R., Graham, K. A., Richards, J. E., & Apperly, I. A. (2011). The neural and cognitive time course of theory of mind. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(36), 12849-12854.
doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1392-11.2011 pmid: 21900563 |
[55] |
Meinhardt, J., Sodian, B., Thoermer, C., Dohnel, K., & Sommer, M. (2011). True- and false-belief reasoning in children and adults: An event-related potential study of theory of mind. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 1(1), 67-76.
doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2010.08.001 pmid: 22436419 |
[56] | Meltzoff, A. N., & Brooks, R. (2007). Eyes wide shut: The importance of eyes in infant gaze following and understanding of other minds. In R. Flom, K. Lee & D. Muir (Eds.), Gaze following: Itsdevelopment and significance (pp.217-241). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. |
[57] |
Michael, J., Wolf, T., Letesson, C., Butterfill, S., Skewes, J., & Hohwy, J. (2018). Seeing it both ways using a dobule cueing task to investigate the role of spatial cueing in level-1 visual perspectiving taking. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 44(5), 693-702.
doi: 10.1037/xhp0000486 URL |
[58] |
Milgram, S., Bickman, L., & Berkowitz, L. (1969). Note on the drawing power of crowds of different size. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 13(2), 79-82.
doi: 10.1037/h0028070 URL |
[59] |
Millett, A. C., D'Souza, A. D. C., & Cole, G. G. (2019). Attribution of vision and knowledge in spontaneous perspective taking. Psychological Research, 84, 1758-1765. doi: 10.1007/s00426-019-01179-1
doi: 10.1007/s00426-019-01179-1 URL |
[60] |
Nielsen, M. K., Slade, L., Levy, J. P., & Holmes, A. (2015). Inclined to see it your way: Do altercentric intrusion effects in visual perspective taking reflect an intrinsically social process? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 68(10), 1931-1951.
doi: 10.1080/17470218.2015.1023206 URL |
[61] |
O'Grady, C., Scott-Phillips, T., Lavelle, S., & Smith, K. (2019). Perspective-taking is spontaneous but not automatic. https://doi.org/10.31219/osf.io/wzcqs
doi: https://doi.org/10.31219/osf.io/wzcqs |
[62] |
Peterson, C. C., & Siegal, M. (1999). Representing inner worlds: Theory of mind in autistic, deaf, and normal hearing children. Psychological Science, 10(2), 126-129.
doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00119 URL |
[63] |
Ramsey, R., Hansen, P., Apperly, I. A., & Samson, D. (2013). Seeing it my way or your way: Frontoparietal brain areas sustain viewpoint-independent perspective selection processes. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 25(5), 670-684.
doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00345 URL |
[64] |
Rosenblau, G., Kliemann, D., Heekeren, H. R., & Dziobek, I. (2015). Approximating implicit and explicit mentalizing with two naturalistic video-based tasks in typical development and autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45(4), 953-965.
doi: 10.1007/s10803-014-2249-9 pmid: 25267068 |
[65] |
Sabbagh, M. A., & Taylor, M. (2000). Neural correlates of theory-of-mind reasoning: An event-related potential study. Psychological Science, 11(1), 46-50.
pmid: 11228842 |
[66] |
Samson, D., Apperly, I. A., Braithwaite, J. J., Andrews, B. J., & Bodley Scott, S. E. (2010). Seeing it their way: Evidence for rapid and involuntary computation of what other people see. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36(5), 1255-1266.
doi: 10.1037/a0018729 URL |
[67] |
Santiesteban, I., Catmur, C., Hopkins, S. C., Bird, G., & Heyes, C. (2014). Avatars and arrows: Implicit mentalizing or domain-general processing? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40(3), 929-937.
doi: 10.1037/a0035175 URL |
[68] |
Santiesteban, I., Kaur, S., Bird, G., & Catmur, C. (2017). Attentional processes, not implicit mentalizing, mediate performance in a perspective-taking task: Evidence from stimulation of the temporoparietal junction. Neuroimage, 155(2017), 305-311.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.04.055 URL |
[69] |
Schneider, D., Lam, R., Bayliss, A. P., & Dux, P. E. (2012). Cognitive load disrupts implicit theory-of-mind processing. Psychological Science, 23(8), 842-847.
doi: 10.1177/0956797612439070 pmid: 22760885 |
[70] |
Schneider, D., Slaughter, V. P., Bayliss, A. P., & Dux, P. E. (2013). A temporally sustained implicit theory of mind deficit in autism spectrum disorders. Cognition, 129(2), 410-417.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2013.08.004 pmid: 23994318 |
[71] |
Schurz, M., Kronbichler, M., Weissengruber, S., Surtees, A., Samson, D., & Perner, J. (2015). Clarifying the role of theory of mind areas during visual perspective taking: Issues of spontaneity and domain-specificity. Neuroimage, 117(2015), 386-396.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.04.031 URL |
[72] |
Schurz, M., Radua, J., Aichhorn, M., Richlan, F., & Perner, J. (2014). Fractionating theory of mind: a meta-analysis of functional brain imaging studies. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 42(2014), 9-34.
doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.01.009 URL |
[73] | Sebanz, N., Knoblich, G. N., & Prinz, W. (2003). Representing others' actions: Just like one's own? Cognition, 88(3), B11-B21. |
[74] |
Senju, A., Southgate, V., White, S., & Frith, U. (2009). Mindblind eyes: An absence of spontaneous theory of mind in Asperger syndrome. Science, 325(5942), 883-885.
doi: 10.1126/science.1176170 URL |
[75] |
Seow, T., & Fleming, S. M. (2019). Perceptual sensitivity is modulated by what others can see. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 81(6), 1979-1990.
doi: 10.3758/s13414-019-01724-5 URL |
[76] |
Simon, J. R. (1969). Reactions toward the source of stimulation. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 81(1), 174-176.
pmid: 5812172 |
[77] |
Simpson, A. J., & Todd, A. R. (2017). Intergroup visual perspective-taking: Shared group membership impairs self-perspective inhibition but may facilitate perspective calculation. Cognition, 166(2017), 371-381.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2017.06.003 URL |
[78] |
Surtees, A., Samson, D., & Apperly, I. A. (2016). Unintentional perspective-taking calculates whether something is seen, but not how it is seen. Cognition, 148(2016), 97-105.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2015.12.010 URL |
[79] |
Teufel, C., Alexis, D. M., Clayton, N. S., & Davis, G. (2010). Mental-state attribution drives rapid, reflexive gaze following. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 72(3), 695-705.
doi: 10.3758/APP.72.3.695 URL |
[80] |
Teufel, C., Fletcher, P. C., & Davis, G. (2010). Seeing other minds: Attributed mental states influence perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(8), 376-382.
doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.05.005 URL |
[81] |
Todd, A. R., Cameron, C. D., & Simpson, A. J. (2017). Dissociating processes underlying level-1 visual perspective taking in adults. Cognition, 159(2017), 97-101.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2016.11.010 URL |
[82] |
Todd, A. R., Forstmann, M., Burgmer, P., Brooks, A. W., & Galinsky, A. D. (2015). Anxious and egocentric: How specific emotions influence perspective taking. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 144(2), 374-391.
doi: 10.1037/xge0000048 URL |
[83] |
Todd, A. R., & Simpson, A. J. (2016). Anxiety impairs spontaneous perspective calculation: Evidence from a level-1 visual perspective-taking task. Cognition, 156(2016), 88-94.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2016.08.004 URL |
[84] | van Overwalle, F., & Vandekerckhove, M. (2013). Implicit and explicit social mentalizing: Dual processes driven by a shared neural network. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7(560), 1-6. |
[85] |
Vogeley, K., May, M., Ritzl, A., Falkai, P., Zilles, K., & Fink, G. R. (2004). Neural correlates of first-person perspective as one constituent of human self-consciousness. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(5), 817-827.
pmid: 15200709 |
[86] |
von Salm-Hoogstraeten, S., & Musseler, J. (2020). Human cognition in interaction with robots: Taking the robot's perspective into account. Human Factors, https://doi.org/10.1177/0018720820933764
doi: https://doi.org/10.1177/0018720820933764 |
[87] |
Wang, J. J., Tseng, P., Juan, C. H., Frisson, S., & Apperly, I. A. (2019). Perspective-taking across cultures: Shared biases in Taiwanese and British adults. Royal Society Open Science, 6(11), 190540.
doi: 10.1098/rsos.190540 URL |
[88] |
Wilson, C. J., Soranzo, A., & Bertamini, M. (2017). Attentional interference is modulated by salience not sentience. Acta Psychologica, 178(2017), 56-65.
doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2017.05.010 URL |
[89] |
Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception. Cognition, 13(1983), 103-128.
doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(83)90004-5 URL |
[1] | YANG Chao, DONG Zhijie, WEN Haoyan, ZHOU Yizhou, WANG Yanli, YUAN Fangzhou. The determinants and consequences of positive body image and its promotion strategies [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2023, 31(5): 815-826. |
[2] | YANG Lei, CHEN Weiyang, BAI Baoyu, ZHONG Nian. Experimental evidence for internal mechanisms of cumulative cultural evolution [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2023, 31(5): 866-886. |
[3] | GUO Ying, TIAN Xin, HU Dong, BAI Shulin, ZHOU Shuxi. The effects of shame on prosocial behavior: A systematic review and three-level meta-analysis [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2023, 31(3): 371-385. |
[4] | CHENG Jieting, SHI Mengwei. Perceived warmth and competence: The role of physiological cues in social cognition [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2023, 31(3): 443-454. |
[5] | ZHANG Haotian, YU Feng, XU Liying, XUAN Zheli. Monism and pluralism in morality: Origins, connotations and debates [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2023, 31(2): 301-314. |
[6] | ZHANG Jing, SUN Qing-Zhou, WU Bao. Risk communication between doctors and patients: Matching role and information [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2023, 31(1): 99-107. |
[7] | ZHENG Yinbo, LI Xin, HUANG Huadong, LI Bin, LI Aimei. The double-edged sword effect of mortality cues on employee performance: A perspective of transactional stress theory [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2023, 31(1): 116-126. |
[8] | KOU Dongxiao, WANG Xiaoyu. The influence of power on interpersonal sensitivity [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2023, 31(1): 108-115. |
[9] | YANG Qun, ZHU Bing, YU Yiming, ZHANG Jingmin, XUE Mengmeng. Prosociality increases under stress: Evidence from different types of prosocial preferences [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2022, 30(12): 2809-2824. |
[10] | LI Song, LIU Junjun, BAO Xiuqin, CHEN Xu. The impacts of interpersonal gratitude on relationship maintenance [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2022, 30(11): 2586-2594. |
[11] | LUAN Mo, WU Shuang. How does decision making process signal social status? A maximizing decision making perspective [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2022, 30(10): 2194-2205. |
[12] | HU Xiaoyong, DU Tangyan, LI Lanyu, WANG Tiantian. Neural mechanisms underlying the effect of low socioeconomic status on self-regulation [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2022, 30(10): 2278-2290. |
[13] | LI Yusu, ZHANG Kun, BI Yanling, ZHANG Baoshan. Psychological challenge and its explanation of first-generation college students: A perspective from cultural mismatch theory [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2022, 30(10): 2338-2355. |
[14] | TANG Hui, LI Xinyu, WEI Yifan, LI Xiaocai, CHEN Liuyan, ZHANG Yao. The impact of partner choice on cooperative behavior and its mechanisms [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2022, 30(10): 2356-2371. |
[15] | HU Xiaoyong, LI Lanyu, DU Tangyan, WANG Tiantian, YANG Jing. The protective effects of the “shift-and-persist” strategy on the health of the lower class and their mechanisms [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2022, 30(9): 2088-2099. |
Viewed | ||||||
Full text |
|
|||||
Abstract |
|
|||||