Advances in Psychological Science ›› 2020, Vol. 28 ›› Issue (10): 1688-1696.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2020.01688
• Regular Articles • Previous Articles Next Articles
LI Jiahui, LIU Qing, JIANG Duo()
Received:
2019-11-01
Online:
2020-10-15
Published:
2020-08-24
Contact:
JIANG Duo
E-mail:duo12322@szu.edu.cn
CLC Number:
LI Jiahui, LIU Qing, JIANG Duo. The cognitive mechanisms of decoy effect in decision making[J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2020, 28(10): 1688-1696.
[1] | 蒋多. (2015). 跨期决策策略研究. 博士学位论文. 浙江大学. |
[2] | 孙彦, 李纾, 殷晓莉. (2007). 决策与推理的双系统——启发式系统和分析系统. 心理科学进展, 15(5), 721-726. |
[3] | Ariely, D. (2008). Predictably irrational: The hidden forces that shape our decisions. New York: Harper Collins. |
[4] |
Bell, D. E. (1982). Regret in decision making under uncertainty. Operations Research, 30(5), 961-981.
doi: 10.1287/opre.30.5.961 URL |
[5] |
Cataldo, A. M., & Cohen, A. L. (2018). Reversing the similarity effect: The effect of presentation format. Cognition, 175, 141-156.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2018.02.003 URL pmid: 29525692 |
[6] |
Cataldo, A. M., & Cohen, A. L. (2019). The comparison process as an account of variation in the attraction, compromise, and similarity effects. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 26(3), 934-942.
doi: 10.3758/s13423-018-1531-9 URL pmid: 30264240 |
[7] |
Choplin, J. M., & Hummel, J. E. (2005). Comparison- induced decoy effects. Memory & Cognition, 33(2), 332-343.
doi: 10.3758/bf03195321 URL pmid: 16028587 |
[8] |
Chung, H. K., Sjöström, T., Lee, H. J., Lu, Y. T., Tsuo, F. Y., Chen, T. S., ... Huang, C. Y. (2017). Why do irrelevant alternatives matter? An fMRI-TMS study of context- dependent preferences. The Journal of Neuroscience, 37(48), 11647-11661.
doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2307-16.2017 URL pmid: 29109242 |
[9] | Connolly, T., Reb, J., & Kausel, E. E. (2013). Regret salience and accountability in the decoy effect. Judgment and Decision Making, 8(2), 136-149. |
[10] |
Ert, E., & Lejarraga, T. (2018). The effect of experience on context-dependent decisions. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 31(4), 535-546.
doi: 10.1002/bdm.v31.4 URL |
[11] |
Evans, N. J., Holmes, W. R., & Trueblood, J. S. (2019). Response-time data provide critical constraints on dynamic models of multi-alternative, multi-attribute choice. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 26(3), 901-933.
doi: 10.3758/s13423-018-1557-z URL pmid: 30737646 |
[12] |
Herne, K. (1997). Decoy alternatives in policy choices: Asymmetric domination and compromise effects. European Journal of Political Economy, 13(3), 575-589.
doi: 10.1016/S0176-2680(97)00020-7 URL |
[13] |
Highhouse, S. (1996). Context-dependent selection: The effects of decoy and phantom job candidates. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 65(1), 68-76.
doi: 10.1006/obhd.1996.0006 URL |
[14] |
Hu, J. P., & Yu, R. J. (2014). The neural correlates of the decoy effect in decisions. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 8, 271.
doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00271 URL pmid: 25147516 |
[15] |
Huber, J., Payne, J. W., & Puto, C. (1982). Adding asymmetrically dominated alternatives: Violations of regularity and the similarity hypothesis. Journal of Consumer Research, 9(1), 90-98.
doi: 10.1086/jcr.1982.9.issue-1 URL |
[16] |
Huber, J., & Puto, C. (1983). Market boundaries and product choice: Illustrating attraction and substitution effects. Journal of Consumer Research. 10(1), 31-44.
doi: 10.1086/jcr.1983.10.issue-1 URL |
[17] |
Kim, M. Y., & Park, B. I. (2017). The impact of country of origin on context effects in choice. International Marketing Review, 34(6), 706-734.
doi: 10.1108/IMR-03-2015-0074 URL |
[18] |
Lea, A. M., & Ryan, M. J. (2015). Irrationality in mate choice revealed by túngara frogs. Science, 349(6251), 964-966.
doi: 10.1126/science.aab2012 URL pmid: 26315434 |
[19] |
Liew, S. X., Howe, P. D. L., & Little, D. R. (2016). The appropriacy of averaging in the study of context effects. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23(5), 1639-1646.
doi: 10.3758/s13423-016-1032-7 URL pmid: 27025500 |
[20] |
Locatello, L., Poli, F., & Rasotto, M. B. (2015). Context- dependent evaluation of prospective mates in a fish. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 69(7), 1119-1126.
doi: 10.1007/s00265-015-1924-y URL pmid: 26097281 |
[21] |
Mao, W. (2016). When one desires too much of a good thing: The compromise effect under maximizing tendencies. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 26(1), 66-80.
doi: 10.1016/j.jcps.2015.04.007 URL |
[22] |
Mishra, S., Umesh, U. N., & Stem, D. E. (1993). Antecedents of the attraction effect: An information-processing approach. Journal of Marketing Research, 30(3), 331-349.
doi: 10.1177/002224379303000305 URL |
[23] |
Noguchi, T., & Stewart, N. (2014). In the attraction, compromise, and similarity effects, alternatives are repeatedly compared in pairs on single dimensions. Cognition, 132(1), 44-56.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.03.006 URL |
[24] | Parrish, A. E., Afrifa, E., & Beran, M. J. (2018). Exploring decoy effects on computerized task preferences in Rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Animal Behavior and Cognition, 5(2), 235-253. |
[25] | Parrish, A. E., Evans, T. A., & Beran, M. J. (2015). Rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) exhibit the decoy effect in a perceptual discrimination task. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 77(5), 1715-1725. |
[26] | Pettibone, J. C. (2012). Testing the effect of time pressure on asymmetric dominance and compromise decoys in choice. Judgment and Decision Making, 7(4), 513-521. |
[27] |
Pettibone, J. C., & Wedell, D. H. (2000). Examining models of nondominated decoy effects across judgment and choice. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 81(2), 300-328.
doi: 10.1006/obhd.1999.2880 URL pmid: 10706818 |
[28] |
Pratkanis, A. R., & Farquhar, P. H. (1992). A brief history of research on phantom alternatives: Evidence for seven empirical generalizations about phantoms. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 13(1), 103-122.
doi: 10.1207/s15324834basp1301_9 URL |
[29] |
Reb, J., Li, A., & Bagger, J. (2018). Decoy effect, anticipated regret, and preferences for work-family benefits. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 91(3), 441-464.
doi: 10.1111/joop.2018.91.issue-3 URL |
[30] |
Ronayne, D., & Brown, G. D. A. (2017). Multi-attribute decision by sampling: An account of the attraction, compromise and similarity effects. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 81, 11-27.
doi: 10.1016/j.jmp.2017.08.005 URL |
[31] |
Scarpi, D. (2011). The impact of phantom decoys on choices in cats. Animal Cognition, 14(1), 127-136.
doi: 10.1007/s10071-010-0350-9 URL |
[32] |
Sedikides, C., Ariely, D., & Olsen, N. (1999). Contextual and procedural determinants of partner selection: Of asymmetric dominance and prominence. Social Cognition, 17(2), 118-139.
doi: 10.1521/soco.1999.17.2.118 URL |
[33] |
Simonson, I. (1989). Choice based on reasons: The case of attraction and compromise effects. Journal of Consumer Research, 16(2), 158-174.
doi: 10.1086/jcr.1989.16.issue-2 URL |
[34] |
Simonson, I., & Tversky, A. (1992). Choice in context: Tradeoff contrast and extremeness aversion. Journal of Marketing Research, 29(3), 281-295.
doi: 10.1177/002224379202900301 URL |
[35] |
Spektor, M. S., Kellen, D., & Hotaling, J. M. (2018). When the good looks bad: An experimental exploration of the repulsion effect. Psychological Science, 29(8), 1309-1320.
doi: 10.1177/0956797618779041 URL pmid: 29792774 |
[36] |
Tan, K., Dong, S. H., Liu, X. W., Chen, W. W., Wang, Y. C., Oldroyd, B. P., & Latty, T. (2015). Phantom alternatives influence food preferences in the eastern honeybee A pis cerana. The Journal of Animal Ecology, 84(2), 509-517.
doi: 10.1111/1365-2656.12288 URL pmid: 25251672 |
[37] |
Trueblood, J. S. (2015). Reference point effects in riskless choice without loss aversion. Decision, 2(1), 13-26.
doi: 10.1037/dec0000015 URL |
[38] |
Trueblood, J. S., & Pettibone, J. C. (2017). The phantom decoy effect in perceptual decision making. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 30(2), 157-167.
doi: 10.1002/bdm.1930 URL |
[39] |
Tversky, A. (1972). Elimination by aspects: A theory of choice. Psychological Review, 79(4), 281-299.
doi: 10.1037/h0032955 URL |
[40] |
Tversky, A. (1977). Features of similarity. Psychological Review, 84(4), 327-352.
doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.84.4.327 URL |
[41] |
Tversky, A., & Simonson, I. (1993). Context-dependent preferences. Management Science, 39(10), 1179-1189.
doi: 10.1287/mnsc.39.10.1179 URL |
[42] |
Wang, Z., Jusup, M., Shi, L., Lee, J. H., Iwasa, Y., & Boccaletti, S. (2018). Exploiting a cognitive bias promotes cooperation in social dilemma experiments. Nature Communications, 9, 2954.
doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-05259-5 URL pmid: 30054460 |
[43] |
Wedell, D. H. (1991). Distinguishing among models of contextually induced preference reversals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17(4), 767-778.
doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.17.4.767 URL |
[44] |
Zhen, S. S., & Yu, R. J. (2016). The development of the asymmetrically dominated decoy effect in young children. Scientific Reports, 6, 22678.
doi: 10.1038/srep22678 URL pmid: 26935899 |
[1] | ZOU Di, LI Hong, WANG Fushun. An investigation into the definition of arousal and its cognitive neurophysiological basis [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2022, 30(9): 2020-2033. |
[2] | ZHANG Hang, MENG Le, ZHANG Jijia. Why musical emotion can be induced by harmony? The effect and cognitive mechanism of musical consonance [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2022, 30(4): 817-833. |
[3] | ZHANG Linlin, WEI Kunlin, LI Jing. Interpersonal motor synchronization in children [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2022, 30(3): 623-634. |
[4] | SUI Xue, SHI Hanwen, LI Yutong. Perspective taking and its cognitive mechanism in language processing [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2021, 29(6): 990-999. |
[5] | ZHANG Jingjing, LIANG Xiaoyue, CHEN Yidi, CHEN Qingrong. The cognitive mechanism of music syntactic processing and the influence of music structure on its processing [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2020, 28(6): 883-892. |
[6] | CHEN Lin, TIAN Xiaoming, DUAN Jinyun. The cognitive mechanism of advice taking [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2019, 27(1): 149-159. |
[7] | Huan HUANG, Bo LIU, Chenchen ZHOU, Ming JI. Mechanisms of commission errors in aftereffects of completed intentions [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2018, 26(9): 1600-1607. |
[8] | DU Weiwei, SONG Ting, LI Fuhong. Bivalency effect and its cognitive mechanism [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2018, 26(11): 1969-1975. |
[9] | FAN Wei, YANG Bo, LIU Juan, FU Xiaolan. Self-deception: For adjusting individual psychological states [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2017, 25(8): 1349-1359. |
[10] | YANG Yingkai; LIU Yanling. The cognitive neural mechanism of depressive rumination [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2016, 24(7): 1042-1049. |
[11] | LI Aimei; SUN Hailong; Xiong Guanxing; WANG Xiaotian; LI Bin. The effect and cognitive mechanism of “time poverty” on intertemporal choice and proactive behavior [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2016, 24(6): 874-884. |
[12] | LIANG Xiuling; LI Peng; CHEN Qingfei; LEI Yi; LI Hong. The Neural Correlates of Retrieval Practice in Learning and Memory Retention [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2015, 23(7): 1151-1159. |
[13] | LIU Lei;SU Ti;PENG Juan;GUO Yiqun;FENG Tingyong. The Cognitive and Neural Mechanism of the Delay Discounting: from the Trait and State Perspectives [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2014, 22(7): 1047-1061. |
[14] | . Cognitive Neuromechanisms of Optimism Bias [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2014, 22(1): 57-66. |
[15] | LI Aimei;LU Fanfan. The Experienced Utility of Well-being and the Deviation Mechanism of Irrational Decision-making [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2013, 21(6): 1059-1070. |
Viewed | ||||||
Full text |
|
|||||
Abstract |
|
|||||