Advances in Psychological Science ›› 2021, Vol. 29 ›› Issue (3): 472-480.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2021.00472
• Regular Articles • Previous Articles Next Articles
CHENG Yuhui, YUAN Xiangyong(), JIANG Yi
Received:
2020-05-14
Online:
2021-03-15
Published:
2021-01-26
Contact:
YUAN Xiangyong
E-mail:yuanxy@psych.ac.cn
CLC Number:
CHENG Yuhui, YUAN Xiangyong, JIANG Yi. The cognitive characteristics of and the brain mechanisms underlying social interaction processing from a third-person perspective[J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2021, 29(3): 472-480.
[1] |
Abassi, E., & Papeo, L. (2020). The representation of two-body shapes in the human visual cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience, 40(4), 852-863.
doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1378-19.2019 URL pmid: 31801812 |
[2] |
Arioli, M., & Canessa, N. (2019). Neural processing of social interaction: Coordinate-based meta-analytic evidence from human neuroimaging studies. Human Brain Mapping, 40(13), 3712-3737.
doi: 10.1002/hbm.24627 URL pmid: 31077492 |
[3] |
Arioli, M., Perani, D., Cappa, S., Proverbio, A. M., Zani, A., Falini, A., & Canessa, N. (2018). Affective and cooperative social interactions modulate effective connectivity within and between the mirror and mentalizing systems. Human Brain Mapping, 39(3), 1412-1427.
doi: 10.1002/hbm.23930 URL pmid: 29265483 |
[4] |
Canessa, N., Alemanno, F., Riva, F., Zani, A., Proverbio, A. M., Mannara, N., ... Cappa, S. F. (2012). The neural bases of social intention understanding: The role of interaction goals. PLoS ONE, 7(7), e42347.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0042347 URL pmid: 22848759 |
[5] |
Capozzi, F., & Ristic, J. (2018). How attention gates social interactions. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1426(1), 179-198.
doi: 10.1111/nyas.2018.1426.issue-1 URL |
[6] |
Caspers, S., Zilles, K., Laird, A. R., & Eickhoff, S. B. (2010). ALE meta-analysis of action observation and imitation in the human brain. Neuroimage, 50(3), 1148-1167.
URL pmid: 20056149 |
[7] |
Centelles, L., Assaiante, C., Etchegoyhen, K., Bouvard, M., & Schmitz, C. (2013). From action to interaction: Exploring the contribution of body motion cues to social understanding in typical development and in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43(5), 1140-1150.
doi: 10.1007/s10803-012-1655-0 URL |
[8] |
Costanzo, M., & Archer, D. (1989). Interperting the expressive behavior of others: The interpersonal perception task. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 13(4), 225-245.
doi: 10.1007/BF00990295 URL |
[9] |
Deen, B., Koldewyn, K., Kanwisher, N., & Saxe, R. (2015). Functional organization of social perception and cognition in the superior temporal sulcus. Cerebral Cortex, 25(11), 4596-4609.
URL pmid: 26048954 |
[10] |
Ding, X. W., Gao, Z. F., & Shen, M. W. (2017). Two equals one: Two human actions during social interaction are grouped as one unit in working memory. Psychological Science, 28(9), 1311-1320.
doi: 10.1177/0956797617707318 URL pmid: 28719763 |
[11] |
Downing, P. E., Peelen, M. V., Wiggett, A. J., & Tew, B. D. (2006). The role of the extrastriate body area in action perception. Social Neuroscience, 1(1), 52-62.
doi: 10.1080/17470910600668854 URL pmid: 18633775 |
[12] |
Farah, M. J., Tanaka, J. W., & Drain, H. M. (1995). What causes the face inversion effect? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21(3), 628-634.
doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.21.3.628 URL pmid: 7790837 |
[13] |
Fedorov, L. A., Chang, D. S., Giese, M. A., Bulthoff, H. H., & de la Rosa, S. (2018). Adaptation aftereffects reveal representations for encoding of contingent social actions. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115(29), 7515-7520.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1801364115 URL |
[14] |
Floyd, K., & Erbert, L. A. (2003). Relational message interpretations of nonverbal matching behavior: An application of the social meaning model. The Journal of Social Psychology, 143(5), 581-597.
doi: 10.1080/00224540309598465 URL pmid: 14609054 |
[15] |
Galazka, M., & Nyström, P. (2016). Infants’ preference for individual agents within chasing interactions. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 147, 53-70.
doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2016.02.010 URL pmid: 27017143 |
[16] |
Galazka, M. A., Roche, L., Nystrom, P., & Falck-Ytter, T. (2014). Human infants detect other people's interactions based on complex patterns of kinematic information. PLoS ONE, 9(11), e112432.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0112432 URL pmid: 25409449 |
[17] |
Gao, T., Scholl, B. J., & McCarthy, G. (2012). Dissociating the detection of intentionality from animacy in the right posterior superior temporal sulcus. The Journal of Neuroscience, 32(41), 14276-14280.
URL pmid: 23055497 |
[18] |
Georgescu, A. L., Kuzmanovic, B., Santos, N. S., Tepest, R., Bente, G., Tittgemeyer, M., & Vogeley, K. (2014). Perceiving nonverbal behavior: Neural correlates of processing movement fluency and contingency in dyadic interactions. Human Brain Mapping, 35(4), 1362-1378.
URL pmid: 23813661 |
[19] |
Goffaux, V., Peters, J. C., Haubrechts, J., Schiltz, C., Jansma, B., & Goebel, R. (2011). From coarse to fine? Spatial and temporal dynamics of cortical face processing. Cerebral Cortex, 21(2), 467-476.
doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq112 URL pmid: 20576927 |
[20] |
Hartmann, M., Mavrolampados, A., Allingham, E., Carlson, E., Burger, B., & Toiviainen, P. (2019). Kinematics of perceived dyadic coordination in dance. Scientific Reports, 9(1), 15594.
doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-52097-6 URL pmid: 31666586 |
[21] |
Hauser, M., & Wood, J. (2010). Evolving the capacity to understand actions, intentions, and goals. Annual Review of Psychology, 61(1), 303-324.
doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.100434 URL |
[22] |
Isik, L., Koldewyn, K., Beeler, D., & Kanwisher, N. (2017). Perceiving social interactions in the posterior superior temporal sulcus. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114(43), E9145-E9152.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1714471114 URL |
[23] |
Ji, H. C., Yin, J., Huang, Y. S., & Ding, X. W. (2020). Selective attention operates on the group level for interactive biological motion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 46(12), 1434-1442.
doi: 10.1037/xhp0000866 URL |
[24] |
Jiang, J., Dai, B. H., Peng, D. L., Zhu, C. Z., Liu, L., & Lu, C. M. (2012). Neural synchronization during face-to-face communication. Journal of Neuroscience, 32(45), 16064-16069.
doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2926-12.2012 URL |
[25] |
Keysers, C., & Gazzola, V. (2007). Integrating simulation and theory of mind: from self to social cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(5), 194-196.
doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.02.002 URL pmid: 17344090 |
[26] |
Kingsbury, L., & Hong, W. (2020). A multi-brain framework for social interaction. Trends in neurosciences, 43(9), 651-666.
URL pmid: 32709376 |
[27] |
Kuschefski, M., Falter-Wagner, C. M., Bente, G., Vogeley, K., & Georgescu, A. L. (2019). Inferring power and dominance from dyadic nonverbal interactions in autism spectrum disorder. Autism Research, 12(3), 505-516.
URL pmid: 30629333 |
[28] | Lahnakoski, J. M., Glerean, E., Salmi, J., Jaaskelainen, I. P., Sams, M., Hari, R., & Nummenmaa, L. (2012). Naturalistic fMRI mapping reveals superior temporal sulcus as the hub for the distributed brain network for social perception. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6(8), 233. |
[29] |
Liu, R., Yuan, X. Y., Chen, K. P., Jiang, Y., & Zhou, W. (2018). Perception of social interaction compresses subjective duration in an oxytocin-dependent manner. Elife, 7, e32100.
doi: 10.7554/eLife.32100 URL pmid: 29784084 |
[30] |
Manera, V., Becchio, C., Schouten, B., Bara, B. G., & Verfaillie, K. (2011). Communicative interactions improve visual detection of biological motion. PLoS ONE, 6(1), e14594.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0014594 URL pmid: 21297865 |
[31] |
Manera, V., Giudice, M. D., Bara, B. G., Verfaillie, K., & Becchio, C. (2011). The second-agent effect: Communicative gestures increase the likelihood of perceiving a second agent. PLoS ONE, 6(7), e22650.
URL pmid: 21829472 |
[32] |
Manera, V., Iani, F., Bourgeois, J., Haman, M., Okruszek, L. P., Rivera, S. M., ... Becchio, C. (2015). The Multilingual CID-5: A new tool to study the perception of communicative interactions in different languages. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1724.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01724 URL pmid: 26635651 |
[33] |
Manera, V., Schouten, B., Becchio, C., Bara, B. G., & Verfaillie, K. (2010). Inferring intentions from biological motion: A stimulus set of point-light communicative interactions. Behavior Research Methods, 42(1), 168-178.
doi: 10.3758/BRM.42.1.168 URL |
[34] |
Manera, V., Schouten, B., Verfaillie, K., & Becchio, C. (2013). Time will show: Real time predictions during interpersonal action perception. PLoS ONE, 8(1), e54949.
URL pmid: 23349992 |
[35] |
Manera, V., von der Lühe, T., Schilbach, L., Verfaillie, K., & Becchio, C. (2016). Communicative interactions in point-light displays: Choosing among multiple response alternatives. Behavior Research Methods, 48(4), 1580-1590.
doi: 10.3758/s13428-015-0669-x URL pmid: 26487054 |
[36] |
Marsh, K. L., Richardson, M. J., & Schmidt, R. C. (2009). Social connection through joint action and interpersonal coordination. Topics in Cognitive Science, 1(2), 320-339.
URL pmid: 25164936 |
[37] |
Mast, M. S., & Hall, J. A. (2004). Who is the boss and who is not? Accuracy of judging status. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 28(3), 145-165.
doi: 10.1023/B:JONB.0000039647.94190.21 URL |
[38] |
Matheson, H., Moore, C., & Akhtar, N. (2013). The development of social learning in interactive and observational contexts. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 114(2), 161-172.
doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2012.09.003 URL pmid: 23164286 |
[39] | Milinski, M. (2016). Reputation, a universal currency for human social interactions. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 371(1687), 20150100. |
[40] |
Neri, P., Luu, J. Y., & Levi, D. M. (2006). Meaningful interactions can enhance visual discrimination of human agents. Nature Neuroscience, 9(9), 1186-1192.
doi: 10.1038/nn1759 URL pmid: 16936721 |
[41] |
Okruszek, L., Haman, M., Kalinowski, K., Talarowska, M., Becchio, C., & Manera, V. (2015). Impaired recognition of communicative interactions from biological motion in schizophrenia. PLoS ONE, 10(2), e0116793.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0116793 URL pmid: 25664584 |
[42] |
Okruszek, L., Piejka, A., Wysokinski, A., Szczepocka, E., & Manera, V. (2018). Biological motion sensitivity, but not interpersonal predictive coding is impaired in schizophrenia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 127(3), 305-313.
doi: 10.1037/abn0000335 URL pmid: 29369645 |
[43] |
Okruszek, L., Piejka, A., Wysokiński, A., Szczepocka, E., & Manera, V. (2019). The second agent effect: Interpersonal predictive coding in people with schizophrenia. Social Neuroscience, 14(2), 208-213.
doi: 10.1080/17470919.2017.1415969 URL pmid: 29227757 |
[44] |
Papeo, L., & Abassi, E. (2019). Seeing social events: The visual specialization for dyadic human-human interactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 45(7), 877-888.
doi: 10.1037/xhp0000646 URL pmid: 30998069 |
[45] |
Papeo, L., Goupil, N., & Soto-Faraco, S. (2019). Visual search for people among people. Psychological Science, 30(10), 1483-1496.
URL pmid: 31532709 |
[46] |
Papeo, L., Stein, T., & Soto-Faraco, S. (2017). The two-body inversion effect. Psychological Science, 28(3), 369-379.
URL pmid: 28140764 |
[47] |
Peng, Y. J., Ichien, N., & Lu, H. J. (2020). Causal actions enhance perception of continuous body movements. Cognition, 194, 104060.
URL pmid: 31518908 |
[48] |
Pesquita, A., Whitwell, R. L., & Enns, J. T. (2018). Predictive joint-action model: A hierarchical predictive approach to human cooperation. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25(5), 1751-1769.
URL pmid: 29119405 |
[49] |
Quadflieg, S., Gentile, F., & Rossion, B. (2015). The neural basis of perceiving person interactions. Cortex, 70, 5-20.
doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2014.12.020 URL pmid: 25697049 |
[50] |
Quadflieg, S., & Koldewyn, K. (2017). The neuroscience of people watching: How the human brain makes sense of other people's encounters. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1396(1), 166-182.
URL pmid: 28405964 |
[51] |
Reed, C. L., Stone, V. E., Bozova, S., & Tanaka, J. (2003). The body-inversion effect. Psychological Science, 14(4), 302-308.
URL pmid: 12807401 |
[52] |
Schilbach, L., Timmermans, B., Reddy, V., Costall, A., Bente, G., Schlicht, T., & Vogeley, K. (2013). Toward a second- person neuroscience. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(4), 393-414.
doi: 10.1017/S0140525X12000660 URL |
[53] |
Schurz, M., Radua, J., Aichhorn, M., Richlan, F., & Perner, J. (2014). Fractionating theory of mind: A meta-analysis of functional brain imaging studies. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 42, 9-34.
doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.01.009 URL pmid: 24486722 |
[54] |
Shen, M. W., Yin, J., Ding, X. W., Shui, R., & Zhou, J. F. (2016). Deployment of attention on handshakes. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 681.
URL pmid: 27242595 |
[55] |
Sinke, C. B. A., Sorger, B., Goebel, R., & de Gelder, B. (2010). Tease or threat? Judging social interactions from bodily expressions. Neuroimage, 49(2), 1717-1727.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.09.065 URL |
[56] |
Sliwa, J., & Freiwald, W. A. (2017). A dedicated network for social interaction processing in the primate brain. Science, 356(6339), 745-745.
doi: 10.1126/science.aam6383 URL pmid: 28522533 |
[57] |
Su, J. Z., van Boxtel, J. J. A., & Lu, H. J. (2016). Social interactions receive priority to conscious perception. PLoS ONE, 11(8), e0160468.
URL pmid: 27509028 |
[58] |
Vestner, T., Gray, K. L. H., & Cook, R. (2020). Why are social interactions found quickly in visual search tasks? Cognition, 200, 104270.
URL pmid: 32220782 |
[59] |
Vestner, T., Tipper, S. P., Hartley, T., Over, H., & Rueschemeyer, S. A. (2019). Bound together: Social binding leads to faster processing, spatial distortion, and enhanced memory of interacting partners. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 148(7), 1251-1268.
doi: 10.1037/xge0000545 URL |
[60] |
von der Luhe, T., Manera, V., Barisic, I., Becchio, C., Vogeley, K., & Schilbach, L. (2016). Interpersonal predictive coding, not action perception, is impaired in autism. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B Biological Sciences, 371(1693), 20150373.
doi: 10.1098/rstb.2015.0373 URL |
[61] |
Walbrin, J., Downing, P., & Koldewyn, K. (2018). Neural responses to visually observed social interactions. Neuropsychologia, 112, 31-39.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.02.023 URL pmid: 29476765 |
[62] |
Walbrin, J., & Koldewyn, K. (2019). Dyadic interaction processing in the posterior temporal cortex. Neuroimage, 198, 296-302.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.05.027 URL pmid: 31100434 |
[63] |
Walter, H., Ciaramidaro, A., Adenzato, M., Vasic, N., Ardito, R. B., Erk, S., & Bara, B. G. (2009). Dysfunction of the social brain in schizophrenia is modulated by intention type: An fMRI study. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 4(2), 166-176.
URL pmid: 19287044 |
[64] |
Wang, Y., Wang, L., Xu, Q., Liu, D., Chen, L. H., Troje, N. F., ... Jiang, Y. (2018). Heritable aspects of biological motion perception and its covariation with autistic traits. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115(8), 1937-1942.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1714655115 URL |
[65] |
Wu, X. Y., Hua, R., Yang, Z. X., & Yin, J. (2018). The influence of intention and outcome on evaluations of social interaction. Acta Psychologica, 182, 75-81.
URL pmid: 29149691 |
[66] |
Yang, D. Y.-J., Rosenblau, G., Keifer, C., & Pelphrey, K. A. (2015). An integrative neural model of social perception, action observation, and theory of mind. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 51, 263-275.
URL pmid: 25660957 |
[67] |
Yin, J., Ding, X. W., Zhou, J. F., Shui, R., Li, X. Y., & Shen, M. W. (2013). Social grouping: Perceptual grouping of objects by cooperative but not competitive relationships in dynamic chase. Cognition, 129(1), 194-204.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2013.06.013 URL |
[68] |
Yin, J., Xu, H. K., Duan, J. P., & Shen, M. W. (2018). Object-based attention on social units: Visual selection of hands performing a social interaction. Psychological Science, 29(7), 1040-1048.
URL pmid: 29741989 |
[69] |
Zaini, H., Fawcett, J. M., White, N. C., & Newman, A. J. (2013). Communicative and noncommunicative point-light actions featuring high-resolution representation of the hands and fingers. Behavior Research Methods, 45(2), 319-328.
URL pmid: 23073730 |
[70] |
Zhou, C., Han, M., Liang, Q., Hu, Y. F., & Kuai, S. G. (2019). A social interaction field model accurately identifies static and dynamic social groupings. Nature Human Behaviour, 3(8), 847-855.
doi: 10.1038/s41562-019-0618-2 URL pmid: 31182793 |
[71] |
Zillekens, I. C., Brandi, M.-L., Lahnakoski, J. M., Koul, A., Manera, V., Becchio, C., & Schilbach, L. (2019). Increased functional coupling of the left amygdala and medial prefrontal cortex during the perception of communicative point-light stimuli. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 14(1), 97-107.
doi: 10.1093/scan/nsy105 URL pmid: 30481356 |
[1] | CHENG Ken, WANG Yifei, LIN Yinghui, WANG Jing. Observer reactions to unethical pro-organizational behavior and their feedback effects [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2022, 30(9): 1944-1954. |
[2] | WU Menghui, XIE Jiushu, DENG Zhu. The debate between inhibition and attribution of egocentric bias in visual perspective taking [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2022, 30(1): 179-187. |
[3] | KUANG Ziyi, ZHANG Yang, WANG Fuxing, YANG Xiaomeng, HU Xiangen. Can the presence of human teacher promote video learning? [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2021, 29(12): 2184-2194. |
[4] | LIU Qipeng, ZHAO Xiaoyun, WANG Cuiyan, XU Yiya, WANG Shuyan. The relationship between rumination and attention disengagement and the underlying neural mechanism [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2021, 29(1): 102-111. |
[5] | GUO Rong, FU Xinyuan. Social class signs and their effects on interpersonal social interactions [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2019, 27(7): 1268-1274. |
[6] | DU Bixuan, ZHANG Mingming, ZHANG Keye, REN Jie, HE Weiqi. Early development of the body shape and body movement in infancy [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2019, 27(10): 1703-1712. |
[7] | ZHANG Keye, ZHANG Mingming, LIU Tiantian, LUO Wenbo, HE Weiqi. The inversion effect of body recognition [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2019, 27(1): 27-36. |
[8] | YIN Shufei, LI Tian, ZHU Xinyi. Episodic memory performance and underlying brain mechanisms in elderly with subjective memory decline [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2019, 27(1): 51-59. |
[9] | LIU Didi, WANG Meiping, CHEN Pian, ZHANG Wenxin. Association between COMT Val158Met polymorphism and depression [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2018, 26(8): 1429-1437. |
[10] | SUN Yan, FANG Lin, WANG Tingyu, CUI Li. The influence factors and neural mechanisms of inhibitory control in autism spectrum disorders [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2018, 26(8): 1450-1464. |
[11] | ZHAO Shusong, ZHANG Yijie, ZHAO Jun. The third-party perspective of organizational justice: Research perspective, content and design [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2018, 26(12): 2216-2229. |
[12] | LIU Limin, TIAN Xiangjuan, ZHANG Wenxin, WANG Meiping. MAOA gene-environment interaction on antisocial behavior and its possible brain mechanism [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2017, 25(6): 970-979. |
[13] | QIAN Haoyue; ZHU Min; GAO Xiangping. Configural processing of faces in old adulthood [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2017, 25(2): 230-236. |
[14] | YUE Tong; HUANG Xiting. The neurobiological underpinnings of trait empathy [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2016, 24(9): 1368-1376. |
[15] | ZHU Yingying; RU Taotao; ZHOU Guofu. The Nonvisual Effect of Lighting and Its Neural Mechanism [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2015, 23(8): 1348-1360. |
Viewed | ||||||
Full text |
|
|||||
Abstract |
|
|||||