Advances in Psychological Science ›› 2020, Vol. 28 ›› Issue (12): 2000-2007.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2020.02000
• Regular Articles • Previous Articles Next Articles
DONG Bo1, WANG Chengyu2, ZHANG Xiuling2(), ZHANG Tianyang3()
Received:
2019-07-28
Online:
2020-12-15
Published:
2020-10-26
Contact:
ZHANG Xiuling,ZHANG Tianyang
E-mail:journeyonstars@126.com;tyzhang@suda.edu.cn
CLC Number:
DONG Bo, WANG Chengyu, ZHANG Xiuling, ZHANG Tianyang. Sequential surface integration process hypothesis of the ground-surface reference frame[J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2020, 28(12): 2000-2007.
[1] | 白向丽. (2020). 现代战斗机自动防撞地系统发展与应用研究. 飞行力学, 38(3), 1-6. |
[2] | 焦军. (2011). 空间距离知觉中地表表征的SSIP假说——来自空间地表遮挡的证据 (硕士学位论文). 西南大学, 重庆. |
[3] | 李晶, 张侃. (2011). 空间记忆的内在参照系理论及研究进展. 华东师范大学学报(教育科学版), 29(3), 49-54. |
[4] |
李英武, 于宙, 韩笑. (2015). 空间参照框架的产生机制及与认知机能的关系. 心理科学进展, 23(2), 192-201.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2015.00192 URL |
[5] | 张爱珍, 王垚, 李晶. (2019). 路径知识习得的认知老化效应. 心理科学进展, 27(2), 242-250. |
[6] | 张家鑫, 海拉干, 李会杰. (2019). 空间导航的测量及其在认知老化中的应用. 心理科学进展, 27(12), 2019-2033. |
[7] | 周柳. (2015). 基于固有偏差的视觉距离知觉研究(博士学位论文). 华东师范大学, 上海. |
[8] | 周佩灵, 黎安娟. (2011). 距离知觉中的SSIP理论. 科教导刊, (4), 242-243. |
[9] | 周荣刚, 张侃. (2004). 空间知识记忆和提取的理论模型. 心理科学进展, 12(3), 330-339. |
[10] | 周荣刚, 张侃. (2008). 基于场景记忆的参照系整合过程中的物体方位判断. 心理学报, 40(12), 1229-1239. |
[11] |
左婷婷, 胡清芬. (2015). 空间认知风格及其与空间能力的关系. 心理科学进展, 23(6), 959-966.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2015.00959 URL |
[12] | Baldwin, J., Burleigh, A., Pepperell, R., & Ruta, N. (2016). The perceived size and shape of objects in peripheral vision. i-Perception, 7(4), 1-23. |
[13] |
Bian, Z., Braunstein, M. L., & Andersen, G. J. (2005). The ground dominance effect in the perception of 3-D layout. Perception and Psychophysics, 67(5), 802-815.
doi: 10.3758/bf03193534 URL pmid: 16334053 |
[14] |
Bian, Z., Braunstein, M. L., & Andersen, G. J. (2006). The ground dominance effect in the perception of relative distance in 3-D scenes is mainly due to characteristics of the ground surface. Perception & Psychophysics, 68(8), 1297-1309.
doi: 10.3758/bf03193729 URL pmid: 17378416 |
[15] |
Bian,Z., & Andersen,G. J. (2010). The advantage of a ground surface in the representation of visual scenes. Journal of Vision, 10(8), 1-36.
doi: 10.1167/10.8.1 URL pmid: 20884576 |
[16] |
Bian,Z., & Andersen,G. J. (2011). Environmental surfaces and the compression of perceived visual space. Journal of Vision, 11(7), 4. https://doi.org/10.1167/11.7.4
doi: 10.1167/11.7.4 URL pmid: 21669858 |
[17] |
Blake, R., & Sekuler, R. (2006). Seeing a three-dimensional world. In R. Blake & R. Sekuler (Eds.), Perception (5th) (pp. 273-315). New York, USA: McGraw-Hill Higher Education.
doi: 10.2196/18818 URL pmid: 32638703 |
[18] |
Creem-Regehr, S. H., Willemsen, P., Gooch, A. A., & Thompson, W. B. (2005). The influence of restricted viewing conditions on egocentric distance perception: Implications for real and virtual indoor environments. Perception, 34(2), 191-204.
doi: 10.1068/p5144 URL pmid: 15832569 |
[19] |
Eby, D. W., & Loomis,J. M. (1987). A study of visually directed throwing in the presence of multiple distance cues. Perception & Psychophysics, 41(4), 308-312.
doi: 10.3758/bf03208231 URL pmid: 3588227 |
[20] |
Eckart, Z., & Markus, L. (2016). Visual space constructed by saccade motor maps. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 10, 1-11.
doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00001 URL pmid: 26858619 |
[21] |
Erkelens, C. J. (2015). The extent of visual space inferred from perspective angles. i-Perception, 6(1), 5-14.
doi: 10.1068/i0673 URL pmid: 26034567 |
[22] |
Feria, C. S., Braunstein, M. L., & Andersen, G. J. (2003). Judging distance across texture discontinuities. Perception, 32(12), 1423-1440.
doi: 10.1068/p5019 URL pmid: 14992421 |
[23] | Gibson, J. J. (1950). The perception of the visual world. Cambridge, USA: The Riverside Press. |
[24] | Hajnal, A., Bunch, D. A., & Kelty-Stephen, D. G. (2014). Going for distance and going for speed: Effort and optical variables shape information for distance perception from observation to response. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 76(4), 1015-1035. |
[25] |
He, Z. J., & Ooi,T. L. (2000). Perceiving binocular depth with reference to a common surface. Perception, 29(11), 1313-1334.
doi: 10.1068/p3113 URL pmid: 11219987 |
[26] |
He, Z. J., Wu, B., Ooi, T. L., Yarbrough, G., & Wu, J. (2004). Judging egocentric distance on the ground: Oclusion and surface integration. Perception, 33(7), 789-806.
doi: 10.1068/p5256a URL pmid: 15460507 |
[27] |
Hecht, H., Ramdohr, M., & von Castell, C. (2018). Underestimation of large distances in active and passive locomotion. Experimental Brain Research, 236(6), 1603-1609.
doi: 10.1007/s00221-018-5245-z URL pmid: 29582108 |
[28] | Higashiyama, A., & Yamazaki, T. (2015). Anisotropic perception of slant from texture gradient: Size contrast hypothesis. Attention Perception & Psychophysics, 78(2), 647-662. |
[29] |
Jansen, S. E. M., Toet, A., & Werkhoven, P. J. (2011). Human locomotion through a multiple obstacle environment: Strategy changes as a result of visual field limitation. Experimental Brain Research, 212(3), 449-456.
doi: 10.1007/s00221-011-2757-1 URL |
[30] |
Jenkins, M. P., Gross, G. A., Bisantz, A. M., & Nagi, R. (2015). Towards context aware data fusion: Modeling and integration of situationally qualified human observations to manage uncertainty in a hard + soft fusion process. Information Fusion, 21, 130-144.
doi: 10.1016/j.inffus.2013.04.011 URL |
[31] | Kavšek,M., & Granrud,C. E. (2013). The ground is dominant in infants’ perception of relative distance. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 75(2), 341-348. |
[32] | Kunz, B. R., Wouters, L., Smith, D., Thompson, W. B., & Creem-Regehr, S. H. (2009). Revisiting the effect of quality of graphics on distance judgments in virtual environments: A comparison of verbal reports and blind walking. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 71(6), 1284-1293. |
[33] |
Loomis, J. M., da Silva, J. A., Philbeck, J. W., & Fukusima, S. S. (1996). Visual perception of location and distance. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 5(3), 72-77.
doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.ep10772783 URL |
[34] | Loomis, J. M. (2014). Three theories for reconciling the linearity of egocentric distance perception with distortion of shape on the ground plane. Psychology & Neuroscience, 7(3), 245-251. |
[35] |
Loyola, M. (2018). The influence of the availability of visual cues on the accurate perception of spatial dimensions in architectural virtual environments. Virtual Reality, 22(3), 235-243.
doi: 10.1007/s10055-017-0331-2 URL |
[36] |
Murphy, A. P., Ban, H., & Welchman, A. E. (2013). Integration of texture and disparity cues to surface slant in dorsal visual cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology, 110(1), 190-203.
doi: 10.1152/jn.01055.2012 URL pmid: 23576705 |
[37] |
Nakashima, R., & Kumada, T. (2017). Peripersonal versus extrapersonal visual scene information for egocentric direction and position perception. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 71(5), 1-28.
doi: 10.1177/1747021817740774 URL |
[38] |
Norman, J. F., Adkins, O. C., Norman, H. F., Cox, A. G., & Rogers, C. E. (2015). Aging and the visual perception of exocentric distance. Vision Research, 109, 52-58.
doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2015.02.007 URL pmid: 25720533 |
[39] |
Ooi, T. L., & He,Z. J. (2006). Elucidating the ground-based mechanisms underlying space perception in the intermediate distance range. Cognitive Processing, 7(1 Suppl.), 75-76.
doi: 10.1007/s10339-006-0073-7 URL |
[40] |
Ooi, T. L., & He,Z. J. (2007). A distance judgment function based on space perception mechanisms: Revisiting Gilinsky's (1951) equation. Psychological Review, 114(2), 441-454.
doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.114.2.441 URL pmid: 17500634 |
[41] |
Ooi, T. L., Wu, B., & He, Z. J. (2001). Distance determined by the angular declination below the horizon. Nature, 414(6860), 197-200.
doi: 10.1038/35102562 URL pmid: 11700556 |
[42] |
Ooi, T. L., Wu, B., & He, Z. J. (2006). Perceptual space in the dark affected by the intrinsic bias of the visual system. Perception, 35(5), 605-624.
doi: 10.1068/p5492 URL pmid: 16836053 |
[43] |
Ooi, T. L., & He,Z. J. (2015). Space perception of strabismic observers in the real world environment. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, 56(3), 1761-1768.
doi: 10.1167/iovs.14-15741 URL pmid: 25698702 |
[44] |
Philbeck, J. W., & Loomis,J. M. (1997). Comparison of two indicators of perceived egocentric distance under full-cue and reduced-cue conditions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23(1), 72-85.
doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.23.1.72 URL pmid: 9090147 |
[45] |
Proffitt, D. R. (2006). Distance perception. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15(3), 131-135.
doi: 10.1111/j.0963-7214.2006.00422.x URL |
[46] |
Proulx, M. J., Todorov, O. S., Amanda, T. A., & de Sousa,, A. A. (2016). Corrigendum: Where am I? who am I? The relation between spatial cognition, social cognition, and individual differences in the built environment. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1-23.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00001 URL pmid: 26858668 |
[47] | Sedgwick, H. A. (2005). Visual Space Perception. In E. B. Goldstein (Ed.), Blackwell Handbook of Sensation and Perception (pp. 128-167). Malden, USA: Blackwell Publishing. |
[48] | Šikl, Radovan., & Šimeček, Michal. (2015). Visual space perception at different levels of depth description. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 77(6), 2098-2107. |
[49] |
Sinai, M. J., Ooi, T. L., & He, Z. J. (1998). Terrain influences the accurate judgement of distance. Nature, 395(6701), 497-500.
doi: 10.1038/26747 URL pmid: 9774104 |
[50] | Teng, D. W., Eddy, C. L., & Kelty-Stephen, D. G. (2016). Non-visually-guided distance perception depends on matching torso fluctuations between training and test. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 78(8), 2320-2328. |
[51] |
Thompson, W. B., Dilda, V., & Creem-Regehr, S. H. (2007). Absolute distance perception to locations off the ground plane. Perception, 36(11), 1559-1571.
doi: 10.1068/p5667 URL pmid: 18265839 |
[52] |
Thomson, J. A. (1983). Is continuous visual monitoring necessary in visually guided locomotion?. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 9(3), 427-443.
doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.9.3.427 URL pmid: 6223981 |
[53] |
Wu, B., Ooi, T. L., & He, Z. J. (2004). Perceiving distance accurately by a directional process of integrating ground information. Nature, 428(6978), 73-77.
doi: 10.1038/nature02350 URL pmid: 14999282 |
[54] |
Wu, B., He, Z. J., & Ooi, T. L. (2007). Inaccurate representation of the ground surface beyond a texture boundary. Perception, 36(5), 703-721.
doi: 10.1068/p5693 URL pmid: 17624117 |
[55] |
Wu, J., He, Z. J., & Ooi, T. L. (2008). Perceived relative distance on the ground affected by the selection of depth information. Perception & Psychophysics, 70(4), 707-713.
doi: 10.3758/pp.70.4.707 URL pmid: 18556932 |
[56] |
Wu, J., He, Z. J., & Ooi, T. L. (2014). The visual system's intrinsic bias influences space perception in the impoverished environment. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception & Performance, 40(2), 626-638.
doi: 10.1037/a0033034 URL pmid: 23750965 |
[57] |
Wu, J., Zhou, L., Shi, P., He, Z. J., & Ooi, T. L. (2015). The visible ground surface as a reference frame for scaling binocular depth of a target in midair. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 41(1), 111-126.
doi: 10.1037/a0038287 URL pmid: 25384237 |
[58] |
Yang, Z. Y., & Purves, D. (2003). A statistical explanation of visual space. Nature Neuroscience, 6(6), 632-640.
doi: 10.1038/nn1059 URL pmid: 12754512 |
[59] |
Zhou, L., Deng, C. L., Ooi, T. L., & He, Z. J. J. (2016). Attention modulates perception of visual space. Nature Human Behaviour, 1(1), 0004.
doi: 10.1038/s41562-016-0004 URL |
[60] |
Zhou, L., He, Z. J., & Ooi, T. L. (2013). The visual system’s intrinsic bias and knowledge of size mediate perceived size and location in the dark. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 39(6), 1930-1942.
doi: 10.1037/a0033088 URL pmid: 23751007 |
[61] |
Zhou, L., Ooi, T. L., & He, Z. J. (2016). Intrinsic spatial knowledge about terrestrial ecology favors the tall for judging distance. Science Advances, 2(8), e1501070.
doi: 10.1126/sciadv.1501070 URL pmid: 27602402 |
[1] | Liu Zhou; Teng Leng Ooi; Zijiang J. He. An Ambient Attention for Visual Space Perception [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2016, 24(Suppl.): 49-. |
Viewed | ||||||
Full text |
|
|||||
Abstract |
|
|||||