心理科学进展 ›› 2022, Vol. 30 ›› Issue (7): 1626-1636.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2022.01626
收稿日期:
2021-04-15
出版日期:
2022-07-15
发布日期:
2022-05-17
通讯作者:
崔丽莹
E-mail:cui720926@163.com
Received:
2021-04-15
Online:
2022-07-15
Published:
2022-05-17
Contact:
CUI Liying
E-mail:cui720926@163.com
摘要:
作为两种常见且易被混淆的自我意识情绪, 诸多研究者对内疚和羞耻的产生前因及影响后效进行研究, 尤其是近年来的研究逐渐关注两者对合作行为的影响。我们通过对已有研究进行梳理发现, 内疚和羞耻对合作行为的影响及其机制存在差异, 内疚的影响更为稳定及具有普适性, 羞耻的影响具有复杂性和多样性, 更容易受到其他因素的影响。内疚和羞耻对合作行为的不同影响可从动机机制和调节机制两方面进行解释。首先, 动机机制包括内疚对他人的补偿、修复动机以及羞耻对自我的保护、修复动机。内疚对他人的补偿、修复动机主要体现在维护社会互动中的公平; 弥补对他人造成的伤害, 降低自己和他人的痛苦; 修复人际关系, 重新获得他人或自己的接纳与认同。羞耻对自我的保护、修复动机主要体现在释放消极的情绪状态; 维护积极的自我形象和声誉, 修复自我; 保护自我, 防止后续伤害及不良后果。总体而言, 内疚者关注过失行为及后果, 侧重于对受害他人的补偿或对与他人关系的修复, 而羞耻者关注整体自我的受损, 侧重于对自我的保护和对自己社会形象的修复, 基于此, 我们在前人研究的基础上构建了内疚与羞耻影响合作行为的动机机制对比模型图。其次, 调节机制涉及到个体因素和情境因素的调节作用, 其中个体因素包括认知因素、社会价值取向、自我控制能力、情绪特质和情绪状态, 情境因素包括暴露情境和掩蔽情境、实验情境和日常情境、相关情境和不相关情境。具体而言, 内疚对合作行为的影响的较少受情境的限制, 而羞耻受情境因素限制较多, 在暴露情境、相关情境中更能促进合作行为的产生, 而在掩蔽情境、不相关情境中, 羞耻较难影响个体的合作抉择, 且无论是实验室情境还是日常情境, 内疚似乎都比羞耻更易影响合作。最后, 鉴于内疚与羞耻的诱发和测量方法的有效性仍需检验, 两种情绪对合作行为影响的过程机制和生理机制仍不明晰, 个体因素影响的研究尚不充足以及跨文化研究较为缺乏, 未来研究可以从内疚和羞耻诱发与测量方法的有效性, 情绪作用发生的内在过程和生理机制, 情绪影响的个体和文化差异这几方面进行扩展和深挖。
中图分类号:
郝娜, 崔丽莹. (2022). 补偿他人还是保护自己?内疚与羞耻情绪对合作行为的影响差异. 心理科学进展 , 30(7), 1626-1636.
HAO Na, CUI Liying. (2022). Compensate others or protect oneself ? The difference of the effects of guilt and shame on cooperative behavior. Advances in Psychological Science, 30(7), 1626-1636.
[1] | 丁芳, 曹令敏, 张琛琛. (2013). 小学儿童羞耻情绪对其合作行为的影响. 山东省团校学报:青少年研究, (1), 19- 24. |
[2] | 丁芳, 周鋆, 胡雨. (2014). 初中生内疚情绪体验的发展及其对公平行为的影响. 心理科学, 37(5), 1154-1159. |
[3] | 丁毅, 纪婷婷, 陈旭. (2015). 社会两难情境下的合作选择:自我利益和集体利益间的权衡. 心理学探新, 35(2), 158-163. |
[4] | 范伟, 任梦梦, 肖俊泽, 简增郸, 杜晓明, 傅小兰. (2019). 羞耻情绪对欺骗行为的影响:自我控制的作用. 心理学报, 51(9), 992-1006. |
[5] | 高学德. (2013). 羞耻研究:概念、结构及其评定. 心理科学进展, 21(8), 1450-1456. |
[6] | 高学德, 周爱保. (2009). 内疚和羞耻的关系——来自反事实思维的验证. 心理科学, 32(1), 126-129. |
[7] | 关元, 何嘉梅. (2018). 自我控制:内涵、理论及展望. 心理技术与应用, 6(2), 118-128. |
[8] | 何华容, 丁道群. (2016). 内疚:一种有益的负性情绪. 心理研究, 9(1), 3-8. |
[9] | 蓝玳琪. (2020). 内疚、羞耻情绪对不同奉献感大学生亲社会行为的影响 (硕士学位论文), 上海师范大学. |
[10] | 李晶, 朱莉琪. (2014). 高功能孤独症儿童的合作行为. 心理学报, 46(9), 1301-1316. |
[11] | 李赛琦, 王柳生. (2020). 内源性羞耻与群体身份对亲社会行为的影响. 心理科学, 43(4), 957-962. |
[12] | 刘长江, 郝芳. (2014). 社会困境问题的理论架构与实验研究. 心理科学进展, 22(9), 1475-1484. |
[13] | 施承孙, 钱铭怡. (1999). 羞耻和内疚的差异. 心理科学进展, 7(1), 35-38. |
[14] | 汤明, 李伟强, 刘福会, 袁博. (2019). 内疚与亲社会行为的关系:来自元分析的证据. 心理科学进展, 27(5), 773-788. |
[15] | 王柳生, 桑标. (2019). 羞耻罗盘扩展模型的初步建构. 心理研究, 12(5), 394-400. |
[16] | 王煜, 李梦菊. (2020). 建设性还是破坏性?羞愧的双路径模型. 心理科学进展, 28(8), 1325-1336. |
[17] | 吴晨阳, 何贵兵. (2011). 自我控制行为中的情绪因素. 心理科学, 34(2), 317-321. |
[18] | 俞国良, 赵军燕. (2009). 自我意识情绪:聚焦于自我的道德情绪研究. 心理发展与教育, 25(2), 116-120. |
[19] | 张琨, 方平, 姜媛, 于悦, 欧阳恒磊. (2014). 道德视野下的内疚. 心理科学进展, 22(10), 1628-1636. |
[20] | 张晓贤, 桑标. (2012). 儿童内疚情绪对其亲社会行为的影响. 心理科学, 35(2), 314-320. |
[21] |
Baumann, D. J., Cialdini, R. B., & Kendrick, D. T. (1981). Altruism as hedonism: Helping and self-gratification as equivalent responses. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40(6), 1039-1046.
doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.40.6.1039 URL |
[22] |
Baumeister, R. F., Stillwell, A. M., & Heatherton, T. F. (1994). Guilt: An interpersonal approach. Psychological Bulletin, 115(2), 243-267.
pmid: 8165271 |
[23] |
Behrendt, H., & Ben-Ari, R. (2012). The positive side of negative emotion: The role of guilt and shame in coping with interpersonal conflict. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 56(6), 1116-1138.
doi: 10.1177/0022002712445746 URL |
[24] | Bond, R. M. (2019). Low-cost, high-impact altruistic punishment promotes cooperation cascades in human social networks. Scientific Reports, 9(1), 2061. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-38323-7 |
[25] |
Burmeister, A., Fasbender, U., & Gerpott, F. H. (2019). Consequences of knowledge hiding: The differential compensatory effects of guilt and shame. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 92(2), 281-304.
doi: 10.1111/joop.12249 |
[26] |
Cheung, M. S.-P., Gilbert, P., & Irons, C. (2004). An exploration of shame, social rank and rumination in relation to depression. Personality and Individual Differences, 36(5), 1143-1153.
doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(03)00206-X URL |
[27] |
Cohen, T. R., Wolf, S. T., Panter, A. T., & Insko, C. A. (2011). Introducing the GASP scale: A new measure of guilt and shame proneness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100(5), 947-966.
doi: 10.1037/a0022641 pmid: 21517196 |
[28] |
Cottrell, C. A., & Neuberg, S. L. (2005). Different emotional reactions to different groups: A sociofunctional threat-based approach to "prejudice". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88(5), 770-789.
doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.88.5.770 URL |
[29] |
Declerck, C. H., Boone, C., & Kiyonari, T. (2014). No place to hide: When shame causes proselfs to cooperate. Journal of Social Psychology, 154(1), 74-88.
doi: 10.1080/00224545.2013.855158 URL |
[30] |
de Hooge, I. E. (2012). The exemplary social emotion guilt: Not so relationship-oriented when another person repairs for you. Cognition and Emotion, 26(7), 1189-1207.
doi: 10.1080/02699931.2011.640663 URL |
[31] |
de Hooge, I. E., Breugelmans, S. M., & Zeelenberg, M. (2008). Not so ugly after all: When shame acts as a commitment device. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95(4), 933-943.
doi: 10.1037/a0011991 URL |
[32] |
de Hooge, I. E., Nelissen, R. M. A., Breugelmans, S. M., & Zeelenberg, M. (2011). What is moral about guilt? Acting "prosocially" at the disadvantage of others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100(3), 462-473.
doi: 10.1037/a0021459 URL |
[33] |
de Hooge, I. E., Zeelenberg, M., & Breugelmans, S. M. (2007). Moral sentiments and cooperation: Differential influences of shame and guilt. Cognition and Emotion, 21(5), 1025-1042.
doi: 10.1080/02699930600980874 URL |
[34] |
de Hooge, I. E., Zeelenberg, M., & Breugelmans, S. M. (2010). Restore and protect motivations following shame. Cognition and Emotion, 24(1), 111-127.
doi: 10.1080/02699930802584466 URL |
[35] |
Dorfman, A., Eyal, T., & Bereby-Meyer, Y. (2014). Proud to cooperate: The consideration of pride promotes cooperation in a social dilemma. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 55, 105-109.
doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2014.06.003 URL |
[36] | Duan, L., Feng, Q., & Xu, P. (2020). Using functional near-infrared spectroscopy to assess brain activation evoked by guilt and shame. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 14, Article 197. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2020.00197 |
[37] |
Furukawa, Y., Nakashima, K., & Morinaga, Y. (2019). Guilt signals a crisis of rejection: Two types of individual differences related to social rejection have dissimilar effects on guilt and compensatory behavior. Japanese Psychological Research, 61(1), 1-11.
doi: 10.1111/jpr.12199 |
[38] |
Haran, U. (2019). May the best man lose: Guilt inhibits competitive motivation. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 154, 15-33.
doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2019.07.003 URL |
[39] |
Henrich, J., & Henrich, N. (2006). Culture, evolution and the puzzle of human cooperation. Cognitive Systems Research, 7(2-3), 220-245.
doi: 10.1016/j.cogsys.2005.07.001 URL |
[40] |
Ho, S.-Y., Tong, E. M. W., & Jia, L. (2016). Authentic and hubristic pride: Differential effects on delay of gratification. Emotion, 16(8), 1147-1156.
doi: 10.1037/emo0000179 URL |
[41] | Hoffman, M. L. (1982). Development of prosocial motivation:Empathy and guilt. In N. Eisenberg (Ed.), The development of prosocial behavior (pp.218-231). New York: Academic Press. |
[42] |
Hofmann, W., & Fisher, R. R. (2012). How guilt and pride shape subsequent self-control. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 3(6), 682-690.
doi: 10.1177/1948550611435136 URL |
[43] |
Hofmann, W., Schmeichel, B. J., & Baddeley, A. D. (2012). Executive functions and self-regulation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(3), 174-180.
doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.01.006 URL |
[44] |
Jacquet, J., Hauert, C., Traulsen, A., & Milinski, M. (2011). Shame and honour drive cooperation. Biology Letters, 7(6), 899-901.
doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2011.0367 URL |
[45] |
Ketelaar, T., & Au, W. T. (2003). The effects of feelings of guilt on the behaviour of uncooperative individuals in repeated social bargaining games: An affect-as- information interpretation of the role of emotion in social interaction. Cognition and Emotion, 17(3), 429-453.
doi: 10.1080/02699930143000662 pmid: 29715746 |
[46] |
Leach, C. W., & Cidam, A. (2015). When is shame linked to constructive approach orientation? A meta-analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 109(6), 983-1002.
doi: 10.1037/pspa0000037 URL |
[47] |
Lee, J., & Min, S. (2021). The effects of repeated induction of emotions on cooperation and punishment. Economic Inquiry, 59(3), 925-943.
doi: 10.1111/ecin.12974 URL |
[48] | Lewis, M. (1997). The self in self-conscious emotions. Annuals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 818(1), 118-142. |
[49] |
Miranda, G. A., Welbourne, J. L., & Sariol, A. M. (2020). Feeling shame and guilt when observing workplace incivility: Elicitors and behavioral responses. Human Resource Development Quarterly, 31(4), 371-392.
doi: 10.1002/hrdq.21395 URL |
[50] |
Murphy, R. O., & Ackermann, K. A. (2014). Social value orientation: Theoretical and measurement issues in the study of social preferences. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 18(1), 13-41.
doi: 10.1177/1088868313501745 pmid: 24065346 |
[51] | Nathanson, D. L. (1992). Shame and pride: Affect, sex, and the birth of the self. New York: Norton. |
[52] |
Nelissen, R. M. A., Dijker, A. J. M., & deVries, N. K. (2007). How to turn a hawk into a dove and vice versa: Interactions between emotions and goals in a give-some dilemma game. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43(2), 280-286.
doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2006.01.009 URL |
[53] |
Olthof, T. (2012). Anticipated feelings of guilt and shame as predictors of early adolescents’ antisocial and prosocial interpersonal behaviour. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9(3), 371-388.
doi: 10.1080/17405629.2012.680300 URL |
[54] |
Patrick, V. M., Chun, H. H., & Macinnis, D. J. (2009). Affective forecasting and self-control: Why anticipating pride wins over anticipating shame in a self-regulation context. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 19(3), 537- 545.
doi: 10.1016/j.jcps.2009.05.006 URL |
[55] |
Pletzer, J. L., Balliet, D., Joireman, J., Kuhlman, D. M., Voelpel, S. C., & van Lange, P. A. M. (2018). Social value orientation, expectations, and cooperation in social dilemmas: A meta-analysis. European Journal of Personality, 32(1), 62-83.
doi: 10.1002/per.2139 URL |
[56] |
Roberts, W., Strayer, J., & Denham, S. (2014). Empathy, anger, guilt: Emotions and prosocial behaviour. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science/Revue canadienne des sciences du comportement, 46(4), 465-474.
doi: 10.1037/a0035057 URL |
[57] |
Robertson, T. E., Sznycer, D., Delton, A. W., Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2018). The true trigger of shame: Social devaluation is sufficient, wrongdoing is unnecessary. Evolution and Human Behavior, 39(5), 566-573.
doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2018.05.010 URL |
[58] |
Sherman, R. A., Rauthmann, J. F., Brown, N. A., Serfass, D. G., & Jones, A. B. (2015). The independent effects of personality and situations on real-time expressions of behavior and emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 109(5), 872-888.
doi: 10.1037/pspp0000036 pmid: 25915131 |
[59] | Skatova, A., Spence, A., Leygue, C., & Ferguson, E. (2017). Guilty repair sustains cooperation, angry retaliation destroys it. Scientific Reports, 7, Article 46709. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep46709 |
[60] |
Smith, A., Pedersen, E. J., Forster, D. E., Mccullough, M. E., & Lieberman, D. (2017). Cooperation: The roles of interpersonal value and gratitude. Evolution and Human Behavior, 38(6), 695-703.
doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2017.08.003 URL |
[61] |
Stuewig, J., Tangney, J. P., Kendall, S., Folk, J. B., Meyer, C. R., & Dearing, R. L. (2014). Children’s proneness to shame and guilt predict risky and illegal behaviors in young adulthood. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 46(2), 217-227.
doi: 10.1007/s10578-014-0467-1 URL |
[62] |
Sznycer, D. (2019). Forms and functions of the self- conscious emotions. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 23(2), 143-157.
doi: S1364-6613(18)30272-9 pmid: 30583948 |
[63] |
Sznycer, D., Tooby, J., Cosmides, L., Porat, R., Shalvi, S., & Halperin, E. (2016). Shame closely tracks the threat of devaluation by others, even across cultures. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 113(10), 2625-2630.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1514699113 pmid: 26903649 |
[64] |
Tangney, J. P., Miller, R. S., Flicker, L., & Barlow, D. H. (1996). Are shame, guilt, and embarrassment distinct emotions? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70(6), 1256-1269.
pmid: 8667166 |
[65] |
Tangney, J. P., Stuewig, J., & Martinez, A. G. (2014). Two faces of shame: The roles of shame and guilt in predicting recidivism. Psychological Science, 25(3), 799-805.
doi: 10.1177/0956797613508790 pmid: 24395738 |
[66] |
Tangney, J. P., Stuewig, J., & Mashek, D. J. (2007). Moral emotions and moral behavior. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 345-372.
pmid: 16953797 |
[67] |
Tracy, J. L., & Robins, R. W. (2004). Putting the self into self-conscious emotions: A theoretical model. Psychological Inquiry, 15(2), 103-125.
doi: 10.1207/s15327965pli1502_01 URL |
[68] |
Tracy, J. L., & Robins, R. W. (2006). Appraisal antecedents of shame and guilt: Support for a theoretical model. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32(10), 1339-1351.
doi: 10.1177/0146167206290212 URL |
[69] |
Vaish, A. (2018). The prosocial functions of early social emotions: The case of guilt. Current Opinion in Psychology, 20, 25-29.
doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.08.008 URL |
[70] |
Watanabe, E., & Ohtsubo, Y. (2012). Costly apology and self-punishment after an unintentional transgression. Journal of Evolutionary Psychology, 10(3), 87-105.
doi: 10.1556/JEP.10.2012.3.1 URL |
[71] |
Wietzker, A., Buysse, A., Loeys, T., & Brondeel, R. (2012). Easing the conscience: Feeling guilty makes people cooperate in divorce negotiations. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 29(3), 324-336.
doi: 10.1177/0265407511431180 URL |
[72] |
Zhu, P., Guo, H., Zhang, H., Han, Y., Wang, Z., & Chu, C. (2020). The role of punishment in the spatial public goods game. Nonlinear Dynamics, 102, 2959-2968.
doi: 10.1007/s11071-020-05965-0 URL |
[73] |
Zhu, R., Feng, C., Zhang, S., Mai, X., & Liu, C. (2019). Differentiating guilt and shame in an interpersonal context with univariate activation and multivariate pattern analyses. NeuroImage, 186, 476-486.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.11.012 URL |
[74] |
Zhu, R., Wu, H., Xu, Z., Tang, H., Shen, X., Mai, X., & Liu, C. (2017). Early distinction between shame and guilt processing in an interpersonal context. Social Neuroscience, 14(1), 53-66.
doi: 10.1080/17470919.2017.1391119 URL |
[1] | 王阳, 温忠麟, 王惠惠, 管芳. 第二类有中介的调节模型[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(9): 2131-2142. |
[2] | 唐辉, 李鑫宇, 魏一帆, 李晓彩, 陈柳燕, 张曜. 伙伴选择对合作行为的影响作用与机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(10): 2356-2371. |
[3] | 吴琴, 崔丽莹. 合作行为中的“眼睛效应”:解释机制与限制因素[J]. 心理科学进展, 2020, 28(6): 994-1003. |
[4] | 汤明, 李伟强, 刘福会, 袁博. 内疚与亲社会行为的关系:来自元分析的证据[J]. 心理科学进展, 2019, 27(5): 773-788. |
[5] | 倪旭东, 贺爽爽. 子团队利弊作用的调节机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2018, 26(5): 910-921. |
[6] | 张颖, 杨付. 主动性人格:机制与未来走向[J]. 心理科学进展, 2017, 25(9): 1544-1551. |
[7] | 殷 融, 张菲菲, 王元元, 许志红. 群体内疚:界定、心理机制、行动倾向及干预策略[J]. 心理科学进展, 2017, 25(6): 1058-1068. |
[8] | 严瑜;吴霞. 从信任违背到信任修复:道德情绪的作用机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2016, 24(4): 633-642. |
[9] | 冷冰冰, 王香玲, 高贺明, 李富洪. 内疚的认知和情绪活动及其脑区调控[J]. 心理科学进展, 2015, 23(12): 2064-2071. |
[10] | 徐晓惠;李晶;朱莉琪. 婴幼儿对合作行为共享性特征的理解[J]. 心理科学进展, 2014, 22(9): 1404-1412. |
[11] | 张书维;许志国;徐岩. 社会公正与政治信任:民众对政府的合作行为机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2014, 22(4): 588-595. |
[12] | 张琨;方平;姜媛;于悦;欧阳恒磊. 道德视野下的内疚[J]. 心理科学进展, 2014, 22(10): 1628-1636. |
[13] | 高学德. 羞耻研究:概念、结构及其评定[J]. 心理科学进展, 2013, 21(8): 1450-1456. |
[14] | 石伟;闫现洋;刘杰. 对不公正历史事件的情绪反应—— 群体内疚[J]. 心理科学进展, 2011, 19(2): 224-232. |
[15] | 张婍;王二平. 社会困境下政治信任对公众态度和合作行为的影响[J]. 心理科学进展, 2010, 18(10): 1620-1627. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||