心理科学进展 ›› 2022, Vol. 30 ›› Issue (3): 623-634.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2022.00623
收稿日期:
2021-04-13
出版日期:
2022-03-15
发布日期:
2022-01-25
通讯作者:
李晶
E-mail:lij@psych.ac.cn
基金资助:
ZHANG Linlin1,2, WEI Kunlin3, LI Jing1,2()
Received:
2021-04-13
Online:
2022-03-15
Published:
2022-01-25
Contact:
LI Jing
E-mail:lij@psych.ac.cn
摘要:
人际运动同步是一种“社会胶粘剂”, 能促进儿童产生更多的亲社会行为。儿童的人际运动同步能力随着年龄的增长不断提升, 其同步表现受到运动方式、节奏刺激类型、同步对象等因素影响。在人际运动同步过程中, 时间感知、运动计划和运动执行三大认知因素参与其中, 既制约着个体同步能力的发展, 也影响个体同步的表现。孤独症作为一种神经发育性障碍, 孤独症儿童表现出异常的人际运动同步。未来的研究不仅要探讨孤独症儿童人际运动同步的异常机制, 还要更多关注人际运动同步训练在孤独症群体中的实际应用和干预效果。
中图分类号:
张琳琳, 魏坤琳, 李晶. (2022). 儿童的人际运动同步. 心理科学进展 , 30(3), 623-634.
ZHANG Linlin, WEI Kunlin, LI Jing. (2022). Interpersonal motor synchronization in children. Advances in Psychological Science, 30(3), 623-634.
[1] | 潘禄. (2016). 肢体同步运动对节奏感知中注意同步化的影响 (博士学位论文). 浙江大学, 杭州. |
[2] | 万群, 林苗, 钱秀莹. (2010). 时间知觉的脑机制: 时钟模型的困境和新导向. 心理科学进展, 18, 394-402. |
[3] | American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-5) (5th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing. |
[4] |
Anzulewicz, A., Sobota, K., & Delafield-Butt, J. T. (2016). Toward the autism motor signature: Gesture patterns during smart tablet gameplay identify children with autism. Scientific Reports, 6, 1-13.
doi: 10.1038/s41598-016-0001-8 URL |
[5] |
Aschersleben, G., & Bertelson, P. (2003). Temporal ventriloquism: Crossmodal interaction on the time dimension: 2. Evidence from sensorimotor synchronization. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 50, 157-163.
pmid: 14511843 |
[6] |
Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (1995). Synchronizing actions with events: The role of sensory information. Perception and Psychophysics, 57, 305-317.
doi: 10.3758/bf03213056 pmid: 7770322 |
[7] |
Baruch, C., & Drake, C. (1997). Tempo discrimination in infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 20, 573-577.
doi: 10.1016/S0163-6383(97)90049-7 URL |
[8] |
Bernieri, F. J., Reznick, J. S., & Rosenthal, R. (1988). Synchrony, pseudosynchrony, and dissynchrony: Measuring the entrainment process in mother-infant interactions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 243.
doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.2.243 URL |
[9] |
Białuńska, A., Dalla Bella, S., & Jaśkowski, P. (2011). Increasing stimulus intensity does not affect sensorimotor synchronization. Psychological Research, 75, 43-53.
doi: 10.1007/s00426-010-0282-7 pmid: 20364268 |
[10] |
Blakemore, S.-J., & Decety, J. (2001). From the perception of action to the understanding of intention. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2, 561-567.
pmid: 11483999 |
[11] |
Bloch, C., Vogeley, K., Georgescu, A. L., & Falter-Wagner, C. M. (2019). Intrapersonal synchrony as constituent of interpersonal synchrony and its relevance for autism spectrum disorder. Frontiers in Robotics and AI, 6, 73.
doi: 10.3389/frobt.2019.00073 URL |
[12] |
Brezis, R.-S., Noy, L., Alony, T., Gotlieb, R., Cohen, R., Golland, Y., & Levit-Binnun, N. (2017). Patterns of joint improvisation in adults with autism spectrum disorder. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1790.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01790 URL |
[13] | Brown, S. (2000). Evolutionary models of music:From sexual selection to group selection. In François Tonneau, & Nicholas S. Thompson (Eds.), Perspectives in Ethology (pp.231-281). Boston, American: Springer US. |
[14] |
Carson, R. G., Yalchin, O., Stephan, R., & Jan, L. (2009). Artificial gravity reveals that economy of action determines the stability of sensorimotor coordination. Plos One, 4(4), e5248-. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0005248
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0005248 URL |
[15] |
Casartelli, L., Molteni, M., & Ronconi, L. (2016). So close yet so far: Motor anomalies impacting on social functioning in autism spectrum disorder. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 63, 98-105.
doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.02.001 pmid: 26855233 |
[16] |
Cirelli, L. K. (2018). How interpersonal synchrony facilitates early prosocial behavior. Current Opinion in Psychology, 20, 35-39.
doi: S2352-250X(17)30121-5 pmid: 28830004 |
[17] |
Cirelli, L. K., Einarson, K. M., & Trainor, L. J. (2014). Interpersonal synchrony increases prosocial behavior in infants. Developmental Science, 17, 1003-1011.
pmid: 25513669 |
[18] |
Clizbe, D., & Getchell, N. (2010). The development of period correction processes in motor coordination: Adaptation to temporal perturbation. Motor Control, 14, 59-67.
pmid: 20237403 |
[19] | Cook, J. L., Blakemore, S.-J., & Press, C. (2013). Atypical basic movement kinematics in autism spectrum conditions. Brain, 136, 2816-2824. |
[20] |
Cross, L., Turgeon, M., & Atherton, G. (2019). How moving together binds us together: The social consequences of interpersonal entrainment and group processes. Open Psychology, 1, 273-302.
doi: 10.1515/psych-2018-0018 URL |
[21] |
Cuadros, Z., Hurtado, E., & Cornejo, C. (2020). Infant-adult synchrony in spontaneous and nonspontaneous interactions. Plos One, 15(12), e0244138. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0244138
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0244138 URL pmid: 33338070 |
[22] |
Curioni, A., Minio-Paluello, I., Sacheli, L. M., Candidi, M., & Aglioti, S. M. (2017). Autistic traits affect interpersonal motor coordination by modulating strategic use of role-based behavior. Molecular Autism, 8, 1-13.
doi: 10.1186/s13229-016-0115-7 URL |
[23] |
Dahan, A., Noy, L., Hart, Y., Mayo, A., & Alon, U. (2016). Exit from synchrony in joint improvised motion. Plos One, 11(10), e0160747. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0160747
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0160747 URL |
[24] |
Doumas, M., McKenna, R., & Murphy, B. (2016). Postural control deficits in autism spectrum disorder: The role of sensory integration. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46, 853-861.
doi: 10.1007/s10803-015-2621-4 URL |
[25] |
Dowd, A. M., McGinley, J. L., Taffe, J. R., & Rinehart, N. J. (2012). Do planning and visual integration difficulties underpin motor dysfunction in autism? A kinematic study of young children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42, 1539-1548.
doi: 10.1007/s10803-011-1385-8 URL |
[26] |
Drake, C., Jones, M. R., & Baruch, C. (2000). The development of rhythmic attending in auditory sequences: Attunement, referent period, focal attending. Cognition, 77, 251-288.
pmid: 11018511 |
[27] |
Dufek, J. S., Eggleston, J. D., Harry, J. R., & Hickman, R. A. (2017). A comparative evaluation of gait between children with autism and typically developing matched controls. Medical Sciences, 5, 1.
doi: 10.3390/medsci5010001 URL |
[28] |
Eggleston, J. D., Landers, M. R., Bates, B. T., Nagelhout, E., & Dufek, J. S. (2018). Examination of gait parameters during perturbed over-ground walking in children with autism spectrum disorder. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 74, 50-56.
doi: S0891-4222(18)30010-6 pmid: 29366924 |
[29] |
Endedijk, H. M., Ramenzoni, V. C., Cox, R. F., Cillessen, A. H., Bekkering, H., & Hunnius, S. (2015). Development of interpersonal coordination between peers during a drumming task. Developmental Psychology, 51, 714.
doi: 10.1037/a0038980 pmid: 25775110 |
[30] |
Faisal, A. A., Selen, L. P., & Wolpert, D. M. (2008). Noise in the nervous system. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9, 292-303.
doi: 10.1038/nrn2258 pmid: 18319728 |
[31] |
Falter, C. M., Noreika, V., Wearden, J. H., & Bailey, A. J. (2012). More consistent, yet less sensitive: Interval timing in autism spectrum disorders. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65, 2093-2107.
doi: 10.1080/17470218.2012.690770 URL |
[32] |
Feldman, R. (2007). Mother-infant synchrony and the development of moral orientation in childhood and adolescence: Direct and indirect mechanisms of developmental continuity. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 77, 582-597.
doi: 10.1037/0002-9432.77.4.582 URL |
[33] |
Feniger-Schaal, R., Schoenherr, D., Altmann, U., & Strauss, B. (2021). Movement synchrony in the mirror game. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 45, 107-126.
doi: 10.1007/s10919-020-00341-3 URL |
[34] |
Fitzpatrick, P., Frazier, J. A., Cochran, D. M., Mitchell, T., Coleman, C., & Schmidt, R. (2016). Impairments of social motor synchrony evident in autism spectrum disorder. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1323.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01323 pmid: 27630599 |
[35] |
Fitzpatrick, P., Romero, V., Amaral, J. L., Duncan, A., Barnard, H., Richardson, M. J., & Schmidt, R. C. (2017). Evaluating the importance of social motor synchronization and motor skill for understanding autism. Autism Research, 10, 1687-1699.
doi: 10.1002/aur.1808 pmid: 28590041 |
[36] |
Fitzpatrick, P., Schmidt, R., & Lockman, J. J. (1996). Dynamical patterns in the development of clapping. Child Development, 67, 2691-2708.
doi: 10.2307/1131747 URL |
[37] |
Fournier, K. A., Hass, C. J., Naik, S. K., Lodha, N., & Cauraugh, J. H. (2010). Motor coordination in autism spectrum disorders: A synthesis and meta-analysis. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40, 1227-1240.
doi: 10.1007/s10803-010-0981-3 URL |
[38] |
Friston, K. (2010). The free-energy principle: A unified brain theory? Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11, 127-138.
doi: 10.1038/nrn2787 pmid: 20068583 |
[39] | Fujii, S., Hirashima, M., Kudo, K., Ohtsuki, T., Nakamura, Y., & Oda, S. (2011). Synchronization error of drum kit playing with a metronome at different tempi by professional drummers. Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 28, 491-503. |
[40] | Gallese, V., Rochat, M. J., & Berchio, C. (2013). The mirror mechanism and its potential role in autism spectrum disorder. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 55, 15-22. |
[41] |
Getchell, N. (2007). Developmental aspects of perception- action coupling in multi-limb coordination: Rhythmic sensorimotor synchronization. Motor Control, 11, 1.
pmid: 17392564 |
[42] | Ghaziuddin, M., & Butler, E. (1998). Clumsiness in autism and Asperger syndrome: A further report. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 42, 43-48. |
[43] |
Glazebrook, C. M., Elliott, D., & Lyons, J. (2006). A kinematic analysis of how young adults with and without autism plan and control goal-directed movements. Motor Control, 10, 244-264.
pmid: 17106133 |
[44] |
Glazebrook, C. M., Gonzalez, D., Hansen, S., & Elliott, D. (2009). The role of vision for online control of manual aiming movements in persons with autism spectrum disorders. Autism, 13, 411-433.
doi: 10.1177/1362361309105659 pmid: 19535469 |
[45] |
Glenberg, A. M., Mann, S., Altman, L., Forman, T., & Procise, S. (1989). Modality effects in the coding reproduction of rhythms. Memory and Cognition, 17, 373-383.
pmid: 2761398 |
[46] |
Gong, L., Liu, Y., Yi, L., Fang, J., Yang, Y., & Wei, K. (2020). Abnormal gait patterns in autism spectrum disorder and their correlations with social impairments. Autism Research, 13, 1215-1226.
doi: 10.1002/aur.v13.7 URL |
[47] |
Gonzalez, D. A., Glazebrook, C. M., Studenka, B., & Lyons, J. (2013). Motor interactions with another person: Do individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorder plan ahead? Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 7, 23.
doi: 10.3389/fnint.2013.00023 pmid: 23616751 |
[48] |
Gowen, E., & Hamilton, A. (2013). Motor abilities in autism: A review using a computational context. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43, 323-344.
doi: 10.1007/s10803-012-1574-0 pmid: 22723127 |
[49] |
Hagen, E. H., & Bryant, G. A. (2003). Music and dance as a coalition signaling system. Human Nature, 14, 21.
doi: 10.1007/s12110-003-1015-z URL |
[50] |
Hannon, E. E., & Johnson, S. P. (2005). Infants use meter to categorize rhythms and melodies: Implications for musical structure learning. Cognitive Psychology, 50, 354-377.
pmid: 15893524 |
[51] |
Hoehl, S., Fairhurst, M., & Schirmer, A. (2021). Interactional synchrony: Signals, mechanisms and benefits. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 16, 5-18.
doi: 10.1093/scan/nsaa024 URL |
[52] |
Hove, M. J., Keller, P. E., & Krumhansl, C. L. (2007). Sensorimotor synchronization with chords containing tone-onset asynchronies. Perception and Psychophysics, 69, 699-708.
doi: 10.3758/BF03193772 URL |
[53] |
Hove, M. J., & Risen, J. L. (2009). It's all in the timing: Interpersonal synchrony increases affiliation. Social Cognition, 27, 949-960.
doi: 10.1521/soco.2009.27.6.949 URL |
[54] |
Hove, M. J., Spivey, M. J., & Krumhansl, C. L. (2010). Compatibility of motion facilitates visuomotor synchronization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36, 1525.
doi: 10.1037/a0019059 URL |
[55] |
Hu, Y., Hu, Y., Li, X., Pan, Y., & Cheng, X. (2017). Brain-to-brain synchronization across two persons predicts mutual prosociality. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12, 1835-1844.
doi: 10.1093/scan/nsx118 URL |
[56] |
Isaksson, S., Salomäki, S., Tuominen, J., Arstila, V., Falter-Wagner, C. M., & Noreika, V. (2018). Is there a generalized timing impairment in Autism Spectrum Disorders across time scales and paradigms? Journal of Psychiatric Research, 99, 111-121.
doi: S0022-3956(17)30800-2 pmid: 29438910 |
[57] |
Isenhower, R. W., Marsh, K. L., Richardson, M. J., Helt, M., Schmidt, R., & Fein, D. (2012). Rhythmic bimanual coordination is impaired in young children with autism spectrum disorder. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 6, 25-31.
doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2011.08.005 URL |
[58] |
Jansen-Osmann, P., Richter, S., Konczak, J., & Kalveram, K.-T. (2002). Force adaptation transfers to untrained workspace regions in children. Experimental Brain Research, 143, 212-220.
doi: 10.1007/s00221-001-0982-8 URL |
[59] |
Jansiewicz, E. M., Goldberg, M. C., Newschaffer, C. J., Denckla, M. B., Landa, R., & Mostofsky, S. H. (2006). Motor signs distinguish children with high functioning autism and Asperger’s syndrome from controls. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 613-621.
doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0109-y pmid: 16609826 |
[60] |
Jones, K. E., Hamilton, A. F. d. C., & Wolpert, D. M. (2002). Sources of signal-dependent noise during isometric force production. Journal of Neurophysiology, 88, 1533-1544.
doi: 10.1152/jn.2002.88.3.1533 URL |
[61] |
Kaur, M., Srinivasan, S. M., & Bhat, A. N. (2018). Comparing motor performance, praxis, coordination, and interpersonal synchrony between children with and without Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD). Research in Developmental Disabilities, 72, 79-95.
doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2017.10.025 URL |
[62] |
Kirschner, S., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Joint drumming: Social context facilitates synchronization in preschool children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 102, 299-314.
doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2008.07.005 URL |
[63] |
Kleinspehn-Ammerlahn, A., Riediger, M., Schmiedek, F., von Oertzen, T., Li, S.-C., & Lindenberger, U. (2011). Dyadic drumming across the lifespan reveals a zone of proximal development in children. Developmental Psychology, 47, 632.
doi: 10.1037/a0021818 pmid: 21219070 |
[64] |
Koehne, S., Hatri, A., Cacioppo, J. T., & Dziobek, I. (2016). Perceived interpersonal synchrony increases empathy: Insights from autism spectrum disorder. Cognition, 146, 8-15.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2015.09.007 URL |
[65] |
Konvalinka, I., Vuust, P., Roepstorff, A., & Frith, C. D. (2010). Follow you, follow me: Continuous mutual prediction and adaptation in joint tapping. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 2220-2230.
doi: 10.1080/17470218.2010.497843 URL |
[66] |
Kragness, H. E., & Cirelli, L. K. (2021). A syncing feeling: Reductions in physiological arousal in response to observed social synchrony. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 16, 177-184.
doi: 10.1093/scan/nsaa116 pmid: 33449119 |
[67] |
Kuhtz-Buschbeck, J., Stolze, H., Boczek-Funcke, A., Jöhnk, K., Heinrichs, H., & Illert, M. (1998). Kinematic analysis of prehension movements in children. Behavioural Brain Research, 93, 131-141.
pmid: 9659995 |
[68] |
Kurgansky, A., & Shupikova, E. (2011). Visuomotor synchronization in adults and seven-to eight-year-old children. Human Physiology, 37, 526-536.
doi: 10.1134/S0362119711050100 URL |
[69] |
Landa, R. J., Holman, K. C., O’Neill, A. H., & Stuart, E. A. (2011). Intervention targeting development of socially synchronous engagement in toddlers with autism spectrum disorder: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52, 13-21.
doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02288.x URL |
[70] | Launay, J., Dean, R. T., & Bailes, F. (2013). Synchronization can influence trust following virtual interaction. In Andreas B. Eder & Christian Frings (Eds.), Experimental psychology (pp.53-63). Hogrefe. |
[71] |
Launay, J., Tarr, B., & Dunbar, R. I. (2016). Synchrony as an adaptive mechanism for large‐scale human social bonding. Ethology, 122, 779-789.
doi: 10.1111/eth.12528 URL |
[72] |
Lorås, H., Sigmundsson, H., Talcott, J., Öhberg, F., & Stensdotter, A. (2012). Timing continuous or discontinuous movements across effectors specified by different pacing modalities and intervals. Experimental Brain Research, 220, 335-347.
doi: 10.1007/s00221-012-3142-4 pmid: 22710620 |
[73] | Macpherson, M., Fay, N., & Miles, L. (2020). Seeing synchrony: A replication of the effects of task-irrelevant social information on perceptions of interpersonal coordination. Acta Psychologica, 209, 103-140. |
[74] |
Madison, G., Karampela, O., Ullén, F., & Holm, L. (2013). Effects of practice on variability in an isochronous serial interval production task: Asymptotical levels of tapping variability after training are similar to those of musicians. Acta Psychologica, 143, 119-128.
doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2013.02.010 pmid: 23558155 |
[75] |
Manning, F. C., Harris, J., & Schutz, M. (2016). Temporal prediction abilities are mediated by motor effector and rhythmic expertise. Experimental Brain Research, 235, 1-11.
doi: 10.1007/s00221-016-4768-4 URL |
[76] | Marsh, K. L., Isenhower, R. W., Richardson, M. J., Helt, M., Verbalis, A. D., Schmidt, R. C., & Fein, D. (2013). Autism and social disconnection in interpersonal rocking. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 7, 4. |
[77] |
McAuley, J. D., Jones, M. R., Holub, S., Johnston, H. M., & Miller, N. S. (2006). The time of our lives: Life span development of timing and event tracking. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 135, 348.
doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.135.3.348 URL |
[78] |
Ménoret, M., Varnet, L., Fargier, R., Cheylus, A., Curie, A., des Portes, V., Nazir, T. A., & Paulignan, Y. (2014). Neural correlates of non-verbal social interactions: A dual-EEG study. Neuropsychologia, 55, 85-97.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.10.001 URL |
[79] |
Meyer, M., Bekkering, H., Haartsen, R., Stapel, J., & Hunnius, S. (2015). The role of action prediction and inhibitory control for joint action coordination in toddlers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 139, 203-220.
doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2015.06.005 pmid: 26150055 |
[80] | Meyer, M., Bekkering, H., Paulus, M., & Hunnius, S. (2010). Joint action coordination in 2½-and 3-year-old children. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 4, 220. |
[81] | Meyer, M., & Hunnius, S. (2020). Becoming better together: The early development of interpersonal coordination. New Perspectives on Early Social-Cognitive Development, 254, 187. |
[82] |
Mills, P. F., Harry, B., Stevens, C. J., Knoblich, G., & Keller, P. E. (2019). Intentionality of a co-actor influences sensorimotor synchronisation with a virtual partner. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 72, 1478-1492.
doi: 10.1177/1747021818796183 URL |
[83] |
Minshew, N. J., Sung, K., Jones, B. L., & Furman, J. M. (2004). Underdevelopment of the postural control system in autism. Neurology, 63, 2056-2061.
pmid: 15596750 |
[84] |
Mogan, R., Fischer, R., & Bulbulia, J. A. (2017). To be in synchrony or not? A meta-analysis of synchrony's effects on behavior, perception, cognition and affect. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 72, 13-20.
doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2017.03.009 URL |
[85] |
Mostofsky, S. H., Dubey, P., Jerath, V. K., Jansiewicz, E. M., Goldberg, M. C., & Denckla, M. B. (2006). Developmental dyspraxia is not limited to imitation in children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 12, 314.
pmid: 16903124 |
[86] |
Mu, Y., Huang, Y., Ji, C., Gu, L., & Wu, X. (2018). Auditory over visual advantage of sensorimotor synchronization in 6-to 7-year-old children but not in 12-to 15-year-old children and adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 44, 818.
doi: 10.1037/xhp0000500 URL |
[87] |
Pan, C.-Y., Tsai, C.-L., & Chu, C.-H. (2009). Fundamental movement skills in children diagnosed with autism spectrum disorders and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39, 1694.
doi: 10.1007/s10803-009-0813-5 URL |
[88] | Parma, V., & de Marchena, A. B. (2016). Motor signatures in autism spectrum disorder: The importance of variability. In Jan-Marino Ramirez. In Jan-Marino (Eds.), Journal of neurophysiology (pp.1081-1748). The American Physiological Society. |
[89] |
Patel, A. D. (2006). Musical rhythm, linguistic rhythm, and human evolution. Music Perception, 24, 99-104.
doi: 10.1525/mp.2006.24.1.99 URL |
[90] |
Provasi, J., & Bobin-Bègue, A. (2003). Spontaneous motor tempo and rhythmical synchronisation in 2½-and 4-year-old children. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 27, 220-231.
doi: 10.1080/01650250244000290 URL |
[91] |
Rabinowitch, T.-C., & Knafo-Noam, A. (2015). Synchronous rhythmic interaction enhances children’s perceived similarity and closeness towards each other. Plos One, 10(4), e0120878. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0120878
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0120878 URL |
[92] |
Rabinowitch, T.-C., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2017). Synchronized movement experience enhances peer cooperation in preschool children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 160, 21-32.
doi: S0022-0965(17)30149-2 pmid: 28412556 |
[93] | Rauchbauer, B., & Grosbras, M.-H. (2020). Developmental trajectory of interpersonal motor alignment: Positive social effects and link to social cognition. In Giovanni Laviola, S. Chamberlain & E. Choleris (Eds.), Neuroscience and biobehavioral reviews (pp.411-425). Elsevier. |
[94] |
Reddish, P., Fischer, R., & Bulbulia, J. (2013). Let's dance together: Synchrony, shared intentionality and cooperation. Plos One, 8(8), e71182-e71182. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0071182
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0071182 URL |
[95] | Rennung, M., & Göritz, A. S. (2016). Prosocial consequences of interpersonal synchrony. In Edgar Erdfelder (Eds.), Hotspots in psychology (pp.168-189). Hogrefe Publishing. |
[96] |
Repp, B. H. (2005). Sensorimotor synchronization: A review of the tapping literature. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 12, 969-992.
doi: 10.3758/BF03206433 URL |
[97] | Repp, B. H., & Penel, A. (2004). Rhythmic movement is attracted more strongly to auditory than to visual rhythms. Psychological Research, 68, 252-270. |
[98] |
Repp, B. H., & Su, Y.-H. (2013). Sensorimotor synchronization: A review of recent research (2006-2012). Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 20, 403-452.
doi: 10.3758/s13423-012-0371-2 URL |
[99] |
Rose, P. (2016). Effects of movement, tempo, and gender on steady beat performance of kindergarten children. International Journal of Music Education, 34, 104-115.
doi: 10.1177/0255761414533309 URL |
[100] |
Sebanz, N., & Knoblich, G. (2021). Progress in joint-action research. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 30, 138-143.
doi: 10.1177/0963721420984425 URL |
[101] | Semin, G. R., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2008). From embodied representation to co-regulation. In Pineda J. A. (Eds), Mirror neuron systems (pp.107-120). Humana Press. |
[102] |
Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., Saporta, N., Marton-Alper, I. Z., & Gvirts, H. Z. (2019). Herding brains: A core neural mechanism for social alignment. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 23, 174-186.
doi: S1364-6613(19)30016-6 pmid: 30679099 |
[103] |
Southgate, V., Johnson, M. H., Karoui, I. E., & Csibra, G. (2010). Motor system activation reveals infants’ on-line prediction of others’ goals. Psychological Science, 21, 355-359.
doi: 10.1177/0956797610362058 pmid: 20424068 |
[104] |
Stapel, J. C., Hunnius, S., van Elk, M., & Bekkering, H. (2010). Motor activation during observation of unusual versus ordinary actions in infancy. Social Neuroscience, 5, 451-460.
doi: 10.1080/17470919.2010.490667 pmid: 20602285 |
[105] |
Studenka, B. E., & Zelaznik, H. N. (2008). The influence of dominant versus non-dominant hand on event and emergent motor timing. Human Movement Science, 27, 29-52.
doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2007.08.004 pmid: 18191491 |
[106] |
Su, W.-C., Culotta, M., Mueller, J., Tsuzuki, D., Pelphrey, K., & Bhat, A. (2020). Differences in cortical activation patterns during action observation, action execution, and interpersonal synchrony between children with or without Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD): An fNIRS pilot study. Plos One, 15(10), e0240301. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0240301
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0240301 URL |
[107] |
Tarr, B., Launay, J., & Dunbar, R. I. (2016). Silent disco: Dancing in synchrony leads to elevated pain thresholds and social closeness. Evolution and Human Behavior, 37, 343-349.
doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.02.004 URL |
[108] |
Toiviainen, P., Luck, G., & Thompson, M. R. (2010). Embodied meter: Hierarchical eigenmodes in music-induced movement. Music Perception, 28, 59-70.
doi: 10.1525/mp.2010.28.1.59 URL |
[109] | Trainor, L. J., & Cirelli, L. (2015). Rhythm and interpersonal synchrony in early social development. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1337, 45-52. |
[110] |
Tranchant, P., Vuvan, D. T., & Peretz, I. (2016). Keeping the beat: A large sample study of bouncing and clapping to music. Plos One, 11(7), e0160178. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0160178
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0160178 URL |
[111] |
Tsapakidou, A., Zachopoulou, E., & Gini, V. (2001). Complexity of rhythmic ability as measured in preschool children. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 92, 777-785.
doi: 10.2466/pms.2001.92.3.777 URL |
[112] |
Tunçgenç, B., Cohen, E., & Fawcett, C. (2015). Rock with me: The role of movement synchrony in infants' social and nonsocial choices. Child Development, 86, 976-984.
doi: 10.1111/cdev.12354 pmid: 25702860 |
[113] |
Valdesolo, P., & Desteno, D. (2011). Synchrony and the social tuning of compassion. Emotion, 11, 262-266.
doi: 10.1037/a0021302 URL |
[114] |
van Beers, R. J. (2009). Motor learning is optimally tuned to the properties of motor noise. Neuron, 63, 406-417.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.06.025 URL |
[115] |
Vesper, C., Butterfill, S., Knoblich, G., & Sebanz, N. (2010). A minimal architecture for joint action. Neural Networks, 23, 998-1003.
doi: 10.1016/j.neunet.2010.06.002 URL |
[116] |
Vicaria, I. M., & Dickens, L. (2016). Meta-analyses of the intra-and interpersonal outcomes of interpersonal coordination. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 40, 335-361.
doi: 10.1007/s10919-016-0238-8 URL |
[117] |
Wiltermuth, S. S., & Heath, C. (2009). Synchrony and cooperation. Psychological Science, 20, 1-5.
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02253.x pmid: 19152536 |
[118] | Wohlschläger, A., & Koch, R. (2000). Synchronization error: An error in time perception. In P. Desain, & L. Windsor (Eds.), Rhythm perception and production (pp.115-127). Lisse: Swets. |
[119] |
Wolpert, D. M. (2007). Probabilistic models in human sensorimotor control. Human Movement Science, 26, 511-524.
pmid: 17628731 |
[120] |
Wolpert, D. M., & Flanagan, J. R. (2001). Motor prediction. Current Biology, 11, 729-732.
pmid: 11566114 |
[121] |
Wolpert, D. M., & Ghahramani, Z. (2000). Computational principles of movement neuroscience. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 1212-1217.
doi: 10.1038/81497 URL |
[122] |
Wolpert, D. M., & Kawato, M. (1998). Multiple paired forward and inverse models for motor control. Neural Networks, 11, 1317-1329.
pmid: 12662752 |
[123] |
Yan, J. H., & Thomas, J. R. (2002). Arm movement control: Differences between children with and without attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 73, 10-18.
doi: 10.1080/02701367.2002.10608987 URL |
[124] |
Yoo, G. E., & Kim, S. J. (2018). Dyadic drum playing and social skills: Implications for rhythm-mediated intervention for children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Music Therapy, 55, 340-375.
doi: 10.1093/jmt/thy013 URL |
[125] | Yu, L., & Myowa, M. (2021). The early development of tempo adjustment and synchronization during joint drumming: A study of 18-to 42-month-old children. In Bremner Gavin (Eds.), Infancy (pp.533-659). Wiley. |
[126] |
Zentner, M., & Eerola, T. (2010). Rhythmic engagement with music in infancy. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107, 5768-5773.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1000121107 URL |
[1] | 郭英, 田鑫, 胡东, 白书琳, 周蜀溪. 羞愧对亲社会行为影响的三水平元分析[J]. 心理科学进展, 2023, 31(3): 371-385. |
[2] | 邹迪, 李红, 王福顺. 唤醒定义探析及其认知神经生理基础[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(9): 2020-2033. |
[3] | 张航, 孟乐, 张积家. 为什么和声能诱发音乐情感?——音乐协和性的作用及其认知机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(4): 817-833. |
[4] | 刘敏, 胡洋, 刘巧云. 孤独症儿童潜在早期识别标志——发声异常及原因探析[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(3): 635-647. |
[5] | 刘萍, 张荣伟, 李丹. 自我超越价值观对持久幸福感的作用及机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(3): 660-669. |
[6] | 侯文文, 苏怡. 注意和记忆对孤独症儿童词汇发展滞后的影响[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(11): 2558-2569. |
[7] | 荆伟, 张婕, 付锦霞, 田琳, 赵微. 婴幼儿面孔注意偏向:先天倾向与发展轨迹——来自正常和孤独症婴幼儿的证据[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(7): 1216-1230. |
[8] | 王琳, 王志丹, 王泓婧. 孤独症儿童动作发展障碍的神经机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(7): 1239-1250. |
[9] | 隋雪, 史汉文, 李雨桐. 语言加工过程中的观点采择及其认知机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(6): 990-999. |
[10] | 魏真瑜, 邓湘树, 赵治瀛. 亲社会行为中的从众效应[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(3): 531-539. |
[11] | 李馨, 刘培, 肖晨洁, 王笑天, 李爱梅. 组织中权力如何促进亲社会行为?责任感知的中介作用[J]. 心理科学进展, 2020, 28(9): 1586-1598. |
[12] | 张晶晶, 梁啸岳, 陈伊笛, 陈庆荣. 音乐句法加工的认知机制与音乐结构的影响模式[J]. 心理科学进展, 2020, 28(6): 883-892. |
[13] | 毕小彬, 范晓壮, 米文丽, 贺荟中. 高风险婴儿前瞻性纵向研究与孤独症谱系障碍早期识别[J]. 心理科学进展, 2020, 28(3): 443-455. |
[14] | 陈晓雯, 蔡文淑, 谢桐, 傅世敏. 孤独症谱系障碍者视觉定向与视觉搜索的特点及神经机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2020, 28(1): 98-109. |
[15] | 石荣, 刘昌. 基于直觉的亲社会性:来自社会启发式假设的思考[J]. 心理科学进展, 2019, 27(8): 1468-1477. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||