心理科学进展 ›› 2022, Vol. 30 ›› Issue (2): 291-307.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2022.00291
收稿日期:
2021-06-03
出版日期:
2022-02-15
发布日期:
2021-12-24
通讯作者:
罗劲
E-mail:luoj@psych.ac.cn
基金资助:
LI Ziyi, ZHANG Ze, ZHANG Ying, LUO Jing()
Received:
2021-06-03
Online:
2022-02-15
Published:
2021-12-24
Contact:
LUO Jing
E-mail:luoj@psych.ac.cn
摘要:
酝酿效应是指当人们遭遇无法解决的难题时, 暂时将其搁置, 转而去完成其他无关任务, 反而有利于原有问题解决的现象。近年来, 研究者对创造性问题解决过程中的酝酿效应及其影响因素进行了广泛的研究, 同时提出了较多理论试图进一步解释酝酿效应。酝酿效应的代表性理论可分为“选择性遗忘理论”、“激活扩散和线索同化理论”、“无意识加工理论”等, 它们从不同角度解释了酝酿效应的机制, 并对其影响因素和有关现象各有预测。对心智游移和睡眠这些特殊形式的酝酿的研究进一步揭示了酝酿效应的机制。认知神经研究表明, 酝酿期的基本认知成分, 即表征重构和激活扩散, 涉及到前额叶、纹状体、海马体等多个脑区的协同合作。未来研究需要以深化理论建设为基础, 利用认知神经方法进一步揭示酝酿效应的机制, 从而更为深刻地认识创造性酝酿的本质。
中图分类号:
李子逸, 张泽, 张莹, 罗劲. (2022). 创造性思维的酝酿效应. 心理科学进展 , 30(2), 291-307.
LI Ziyi, ZHANG Ze, ZHANG Ying, LUO Jing. (2022). The incubation effect of creative thinking. Advances in Psychological Science, 30(2), 291-307.
注意撤回理论 | 遗忘固着理论 | 激活扩散与线索同化理论 | 无意识加工理论 | |
---|---|---|---|---|
理论简介 | 酝酿的作用是转移注意力, 消除由错误假设导致的固着(Segal, | 酝酿期促进了对固着的遗忘(Smith et al., | 酝酿效应来自外部线索提示和内部激活扩散或问题表征的交互(Yaniv et al., | 在酝酿期存在无意识思维对问题解决方案的持续联想性检索(Dijksterhuis et al., |
酝酿期长度 | 酝酿效应来自错误假设的突然消失, 因此酝酿时间无影响(Segal, | 酝酿期越长, 固着越可能被遗忘(Smith et al., | 酝酿期长度需要适中, 其长度和酝酿效应大小之间是倒U型关系(Yang et al., | |
准备期长度 | 对于视觉任务, 准备期长则更能意识到形成新的问题表征的必要性(Sio et al., | 在准备期达到僵局后开始酝酿期, 可以最大化线索提示的作用(Moss et al., | 准备期可能允许被试穷尽问题空间, 在再次尝试时转换问题表征, 但也可能使固着增强(Sio et al., | |
目标任务 | 如果目标任务具有多个答案, 则酝酿效应更明显(Sio et al., | |||
内插任务 | 高认知负荷的内插任务更能转移注意力, 因此酝酿效应更大(Segal, | 低认知负荷的内插任务更有利于对线索的同化(Sio et al., | 内插任务和目标任务的类型最好不同(Gilhooly et al., | |
有益线索 | 酝酿期呈现答案相关线索促进酝酿效应(Yaniv et al., | |||
误导性线索 | 误导性线索更多影响言语创造性任务的酝酿(Sio et al., | 立即酝酿能减弱误导性线索的影响(Chiang et al., | ||
结果维度 | 酝酿期提升答案新颖性而非有效性(Tsenn et al., |
表1 酝酿效应理论及相关实证研究
注意撤回理论 | 遗忘固着理论 | 激活扩散与线索同化理论 | 无意识加工理论 | |
---|---|---|---|---|
理论简介 | 酝酿的作用是转移注意力, 消除由错误假设导致的固着(Segal, | 酝酿期促进了对固着的遗忘(Smith et al., | 酝酿效应来自外部线索提示和内部激活扩散或问题表征的交互(Yaniv et al., | 在酝酿期存在无意识思维对问题解决方案的持续联想性检索(Dijksterhuis et al., |
酝酿期长度 | 酝酿效应来自错误假设的突然消失, 因此酝酿时间无影响(Segal, | 酝酿期越长, 固着越可能被遗忘(Smith et al., | 酝酿期长度需要适中, 其长度和酝酿效应大小之间是倒U型关系(Yang et al., | |
准备期长度 | 对于视觉任务, 准备期长则更能意识到形成新的问题表征的必要性(Sio et al., | 在准备期达到僵局后开始酝酿期, 可以最大化线索提示的作用(Moss et al., | 准备期可能允许被试穷尽问题空间, 在再次尝试时转换问题表征, 但也可能使固着增强(Sio et al., | |
目标任务 | 如果目标任务具有多个答案, 则酝酿效应更明显(Sio et al., | |||
内插任务 | 高认知负荷的内插任务更能转移注意力, 因此酝酿效应更大(Segal, | 低认知负荷的内插任务更有利于对线索的同化(Sio et al., | 内插任务和目标任务的类型最好不同(Gilhooly et al., | |
有益线索 | 酝酿期呈现答案相关线索促进酝酿效应(Yaniv et al., | |||
误导性线索 | 误导性线索更多影响言语创造性任务的酝酿(Sio et al., | 立即酝酿能减弱误导性线索的影响(Chiang et al., | ||
结果维度 | 酝酿期提升答案新颖性而非有效性(Tsenn et al., |
遗忘 | 线索同化 | 无意识加工 | 心智游移 | 睡眠 | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
简述 | 酝酿期刷新工作记忆, 促进错误信息的遗忘 | 酝酿期问题表征持续激活, 与有益线索互动 | 酝酿期存在对目标任务的持续无意识加工 | 酝酿期存在心智游移, 增加与目标任务有关的无意识联结 | 睡眠时发生了记忆表征的重组; 睡眠时重新激活了相关记忆, 发生了激活扩散 |
认知成分 | 解除准备期导致的思维定势, 改变问题表征 | 将线索与已有问题表征结合 | 表征转换 | 形成远距离联结 | 形成远距离联结, 改变问题表征 |
神经机制 | 背外侧前额叶调控海马体的记忆编码活动(Anderson & Hulbert, 2020; Hanslmayr et al., | 右侧额叶保持问题表征(唐晓晨 等, | 纹状体-海马体-前额叶网络在意识之外的神经再激活(Creswell et al., | 内侧颞叶、默认网络和执行控制网络的交互(Christoff et al., | 尚缺乏关于睡眠酝酿效应的脑影像学证据 |
总结 | 在酝酿期, 背外侧前额叶调控海马体等记忆系统, 打破错误假设导致的固着; 右侧额叶、颞叶、顶叶的协同合作有利于个体同化有益线索; 纹状体-海马体-前额叶网络在酝酿期持续催化表征重构; 酝酿期的心智游移通过内侧颞叶, 默认网络和执行网络的交互促进顿悟。对睡眠的研究可以反映现实生活中长期酝酿的机制, 未来研究可进一步考察不同睡眠阶段的作用和相应脑机制 |
表2 各视角下酝酿期认知神经机制
遗忘 | 线索同化 | 无意识加工 | 心智游移 | 睡眠 | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
简述 | 酝酿期刷新工作记忆, 促进错误信息的遗忘 | 酝酿期问题表征持续激活, 与有益线索互动 | 酝酿期存在对目标任务的持续无意识加工 | 酝酿期存在心智游移, 增加与目标任务有关的无意识联结 | 睡眠时发生了记忆表征的重组; 睡眠时重新激活了相关记忆, 发生了激活扩散 |
认知成分 | 解除准备期导致的思维定势, 改变问题表征 | 将线索与已有问题表征结合 | 表征转换 | 形成远距离联结 | 形成远距离联结, 改变问题表征 |
神经机制 | 背外侧前额叶调控海马体的记忆编码活动(Anderson & Hulbert, 2020; Hanslmayr et al., | 右侧额叶保持问题表征(唐晓晨 等, | 纹状体-海马体-前额叶网络在意识之外的神经再激活(Creswell et al., | 内侧颞叶、默认网络和执行控制网络的交互(Christoff et al., | 尚缺乏关于睡眠酝酿效应的脑影像学证据 |
总结 | 在酝酿期, 背外侧前额叶调控海马体等记忆系统, 打破错误假设导致的固着; 右侧额叶、颞叶、顶叶的协同合作有利于个体同化有益线索; 纹状体-海马体-前额叶网络在酝酿期持续催化表征重构; 酝酿期的心智游移通过内侧颞叶, 默认网络和执行网络的交互促进顿悟。对睡眠的研究可以反映现实生活中长期酝酿的机制, 未来研究可进一步考察不同睡眠阶段的作用和相应脑机制 |
[1] | 沈汪兵, 刘昌, 罗劲, 余洁. (2012). 顿悟问题思维僵局早期觉察的脑电研究. 心理学报, 44(7), 924-935. |
[2] | 唐晓晨, 庞娇艳, 罗劲. (2009). 顿悟中的蔡格尼克效应: 左右脑在解题失败与提示信息加工时的活动差异. 科学通报, 54(22), 3464-3474. |
[3] |
Allen, A. P., & Thomas, K. E. (2011). A dual process account of creative thinking. Creativity Research Journal, 23(2), 109-118.
doi: 10.1080/10400419.2011.571183 URL |
[4] | Al-Shorachi, E., Sasasmit, K., & Gonçalves, M. (2015). Creativity intervention:Using storytelling and math problems as intervening tasks for inducing incubation. In DS 80-11 Proceedings of the 20th International Conference on Engineering Design (ICED 15) Vol 11: Human Behaviour in Design, Design Education; Milan, Italy, 27-30.07. 15 (pp. 081-090). |
[5] |
Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Reidler, J. S., Huang, C., & Buckner, R. L. (2010). Evidence for the dsefault network's role in spontaneous cognition. Journal of Neurophysiology, 104, 322-335.
doi: 10.1152/jn.00830.2009 pmid: 20463201 |
[6] |
Baird, B., Smallwood, J., Mrazek, M. D., Kam, J. W. Y., Franklin, M. S., & Schooler, J. W. (2012). Inspired by distraction: Mind wandering facilitates creative incubation. Psychological Science, 23(10), 1117-1122.
doi: 10.1177/0956797612446024 URL |
[7] |
Bargh, J. A., Raymond, P., Pryor, J. B., & Strack, F. (1995). Attractiveness of the underling: An automatic power→ sex association and its consequences for sexual harassment and aggression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68(5), 768.
pmid: 7776181 |
[8] | Beijamini, F., Valentin, A., Jäger, R., Born, J., & Diekelmann, S. (2021). Sleep facilitates problem solving with no additional gain through targeted memory reactivation. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 15, 30. |
[9] |
Binder, J. R., Frost, J. A., Hammeke, T. A., Bellgowan, P. S. F., Rao, S. M., & Cox, R. W. (1999). Conceptual processing during the conscious resting state: A functional MRI study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 11, 80-83.
pmid: 9950716 |
[10] |
Bos, M. W., Dijksterhuis, A., & van Baaren, R. B. V. (2008). On the goal-dependency of unconscious thought. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44(4), 1114-1120.
doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2008.01.001 URL |
[11] |
Bos, M. W., Dijksterhuis, A., & van Baaren, R. B. V. (2011). The benefits of “sleeping on things”: Unconscious thought leads to automatic weighting. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 21(1), 4-8.
doi: 10.1016/j.jcps.2010.09.002 URL |
[12] |
Bowden, E. M., & Jung-Beeman, M. (2003). Normative data for 144 compound remote associate problems. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 35(4), 634-639.
doi: 10.3758/BF03195543 URL |
[13] | Brandmeyer, T., & Delorme, A. (2020). Meditation and the wandering mind: A theoretical framework of underlying neurocognitive mechanisms. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1745691620917340. |
[14] |
Breslin, D. (2019). Off-Task social breaks and group creativity. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 53(4), 496-507.
doi: 10.1002/jocb.v53.4 URL |
[15] | Brodt, S., Pöhlchen, D., Täumer, E., Gais, S., & Schönauer, M. (2018). Incubation, not sleep, aids problem-solving. Sleep, 41(10), zsy155. |
[16] | Buckner, R. L., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., & Schacter, D. L. (2008). The brain’s default network anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1124, 1-38. |
[17] |
Bursley, J. K., Nestor, A., Tarr, M. J., & Creswell, J. D. (2016). Awake, offline processing during associative learning. Plos One, 11(4), e0127522.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0127522 URL |
[18] |
Cai, D. J., Mednick, S. A., Harrison, E. M., Kanady, J. C., & Mednick, S. C. (2009). REM, not incubation, improves creativity by priming associative networks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(25), 10130-10134.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.0900271106 URL |
[19] |
Cerruti, C., & Schlaug, G. (2009). Anodal transcranial direct current stimulation of the prefrontal cortex enhances complex verbal associative thought. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(10), 1980-1987.
doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.21143 pmid: 18855556 |
[20] |
Chiang, N. C., & Chen, M. L. (2017). Benefits of incubation on divergent thinking. Creativity Research Journal, 29(3), 282-291.
doi: 10.1080/10400419.2017.1360058 URL |
[21] |
Choi, H., & Smith, S. M. (2005). Incubation and the resolution of tip-of-the-tongue states. Journal of General Psychology, 132(4), 365-376.
doi: 10.3200/GENP.132.4.365-376 URL |
[22] |
Christoff, K., Gordon, A. M., Smallwood, J., Smith, R., & Schooler, J. W. (2009). Experience sampling during fMRI reveals default network and executive system contributions to mind wandering. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106, 8719-8724.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.0900234106 URL |
[23] |
Cousins, J. N., El-Deredy, W., Parkes, L. M., Hennies, N., & Lewis, P. A. (2016). Cued reactivation of motor learning during sleep leads to overnight changes in functional brain activity and connectivity. Plos Biology, 14(5), e1002451.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1002451 URL |
[24] |
Creswell, J. D., Bursley, J. K., & Satpute, A. B. (2013). Neural reactivation links unconscious thought to decision-making performance. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8(8), 863-869.
doi: 10.1093/scan/nst004 pmid: 23314012 |
[25] |
Darsaud, A., Wagner, U., Balteau, E., Desseilles, M., Sterpenich, V., Vandewalle, G., … Maquet, P. (2011). Neural precursors of delayed insight. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(8), 1900-1910.
doi: 10.1162/jocn.2010.21550 URL |
[26] | Deshmukh, V. D. (2013). Cognitive pause-and-unload hypothesis of meditation and creativity. Journal of Alternative Medicine Research, 5(3), 217. |
[27] |
Dijksterhuis, A. (2004). Think different: The merits of unconscious thought in preference development and decision making. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87(5), 586.
pmid: 15535773 |
[28] |
Dijksterhuis, A. (2013). First neural evidence for the unconscious thought process. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8(8), 845.
doi: 10.1093/scan/nst036 pmid: 24249812 |
[29] |
Dijksterhuis, A., & Meurs, T. (2006). Where creativity resides: The generative power of unconscious thought. Consciousness and Cognition, 15(1), 135-146.
pmid: 16019229 |
[30] |
Dijksterhuis, A., & Nordgren, L. F. (2006). A theory of unconscious thought. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1(2), 95-109.
doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6916.2006.00007.x pmid: 26151465 |
[31] |
Fox, K. C., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., & Christoff, K. (2016). The neurobiology of self-generated thought from cells to systems: Integrating evidence from lesion studies, human intracranial electrophysiology, neurochemistry, and neuroendocrinology. Neuroscience, 335, 134-150.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.08.020 URL |
[32] |
Fox, K. C., & Beaty, R. E. (2019). Mind-wandering as creative thinking: Neural, psychological, and theoretical considerations. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 27, 123-130.
doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2018.10.009 URL |
[33] |
Fox, K. C., Spreng, R. N., Ellamil, M., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., & Christoff, K. (2015). The wandering brain: Meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies of mind-wandering and related spontaneous thought processes. Neuroimage, 111, 611-621.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.02.039 URL |
[34] |
Frith, E., Ponce, P., & Loprinzi, P. D. (2021). Active or inert? An experimental comparison of creative ideation across incubation periods. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 55(1), 5-14.
doi: 10.1002/jocb.v55.1 URL |
[35] |
Gao, Y., & Zhang, H. (2014). Unconscious processing modulates creative problem solving: Evidence from an electrophysiological study. Consciousness and Cognition, 26, 64-73.
doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2014.03.001 URL |
[36] |
Gilhooly, K. J. (2016). Incubation and intuition in creative problem solving. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1076.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01076 pmid: 27499745 |
[37] |
Gilhooly, K. J. (2017). Response: Commentary: Incubation and intuition in creative problem solving. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 465.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00465 pmid: 28424642 |
[38] | Gilhooly, K. J., Georgiou, G., & Devery, U. (2013). Incubation and creativity: Do something different. Thinking & Reasoning, 19(2), 137-149. |
[39] |
Gilhooly, K. J., Georgiou, G. J., Garrison, J., Reston, J. D., & Sirota, M. (2012). Don’t wait to incubate: Immediate versus delayed incubation in divergent thinking. Memory & Cognition, 40(6), 966-975.
doi: 10.3758/s13421-012-0199-z URL |
[40] |
Goel, V., & Vartanian, O. (2005). Dissociating the roles of right ventral lateral and dorsal lateral prefrontal cortex in generation and maintenance of hypotheses in set-shift problems. Cerebral Cortex, 15(8), 1170-1177.
doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhh217 URL |
[41] |
Golchert, J., Smallwood, J., Jefferies, E., Seli, P., Huntenburg, J. M., Liem, F., … Margulies, D. S. (2017). Individual variation in intentionality in the mind-wandering state is reflected in the integration of the default-mode, fronto-parietal, and limbic networks. Neuroimage, 146, 226-235.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.11.025 URL |
[42] | Haarmann, H. J., George, T., Smaliy, A., & Dien, J. (2012). Remote associates test and alpha brain waves. The Journal of Problem Solving, 4(2), 5. |
[43] |
Hanslmayr, S., Staudigl, T., & Fellner, M. C. (2012). Oscillatory power decreases and long-term memory: The information via desynchronization hypothesis. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 74.
doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00074 pmid: 22514527 |
[44] |
Hao, N., Ku, Y. X., Liu, M. G., Hu, Y., Grabner, R. H., & Fink, A. (2014). Enhancing verbal creativity via brief interventions during an incubation interval. Creativity Research Journal, 26(1), 30-38.
doi: 10.1080/10400419.2014.873658 URL |
[45] |
Hao, N., Liu, M. G., Ku, Y. X., Hu, Y., & Runco, M. A. (2015). Verbal divergent thinking facilitated by a pleasurable incubation interval. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 9(3), 286.
doi: 10.1037/a0038851 URL |
[46] |
Henok, N., Vallée-Tourangeau, F., & Vallée-Tourangeau, G. (2020). Incubation and interactivity in insight problem solving. Psychological Research, 84(1), 128-139.
doi: 10.1007/s00426-018-0992-9 URL |
[47] |
Hołda, M., Głodek, A., Dankiewicz-Berger, M., Skrzypińska, D., & Szmigielska, B. (2020). Ill-defined problem solving does not benefit from daytime napping. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 559.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00559 URL |
[48] |
Hélie, S., & Sun, R. (2010). Incubation, insight, and creative problem solving: A unified theory and a connectionist model. Psychological Review, 117(3), 994.
doi: 10.1037/a0019532 URL |
[49] |
Johannessen, K. B., & Berntsen, D. (2010). Current concerns in involuntary and voluntary autobiographical memories. Consciousness and Cognition, 19, 847-860.
doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2010.01.009 pmid: 20188597 |
[50] | Kershaw, T. C., & Ohlsson, S. (2004). Multiple causes of difficulty in insight: The case of the nine-dot problem. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30, 3-13. |
[51] |
Killingsworth, M. A., & Gilbert, D. T. (2010). A wandering mind is an unhappy mind. Science, 330, 932.
doi: 10.1126/science.1192439 pmid: 21071660 |
[52] |
Kounios, J., Fleck, J. I., Green, D. L., Payne, L., Stevenson, J. L., Bowden, E. M., & Jung-Beeman, M. (2008). The origins of insight in resting-state brain activity. Neuropsychologia, 46, 281-291.
pmid: 17765273 |
[53] |
Kounios, J., Frymiare, J. L., Bowden, E. M., Fleck, J. I., Subramaniam, K., Parrish, T. B., & Jung-Beeman, M. (2006). The prepared mind: Neural activity prior to problem presentation predicts subsequent solution by sudden insight. Psychological Science, 17, 882-890.
pmid: 17100789 |
[54] |
Leszczynski, M., Chaieb, L., Reber, T. P., Derner, M., Axmacher, N., & Fell, J. (2017). Mind wandering simultaneously prolongs reactions and promotes creative incubation. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 1-9.
doi: 10.1038/s41598-016-0028-x URL |
[55] |
Luo, J., & Niki, K. (2003). Function of hippocampus in “insight” of problem solving. Hippocampus, 13(3), 316-323.
doi: 10.1002/(ISSN)1098-1063 URL |
[56] |
Madjar, N., Shalley, C. E., & Herndon, B. (2019). Taking time to incubate: The moderating role of ‘what you do’ and ‘when you do it’ on creative performance. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 53(3), 377-388.
doi: 10.1002/jocb.v53.3 URL |
[57] |
Mazoyer, B., Zago, L., Mellet, E., Bricogne, S., Etard, O., Houde, O., … Tzourio-Mazoyer, N. (2001). Cortical networks for working memory and executive functions sustain the conscious resting state in man. Brain Research Bulletin, 54, 287-298.
pmid: 11287133 |
[58] |
McCormick, C., Rosenthal, C. R., Miller, T. D., & Maguire, E. A. (2018). Mind-wandering in people with hippocampal damage. Journal of Neuroscience, 38(11), 2745-2754.
doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1812-17.2018 pmid: 29440532 |
[59] |
Meyer, D. E., & Schvaneveldt, R. W. (1971). Facilitation in recognizing pairs of words: Evidence of a dependence between retrieval operations. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 90(2), 227.
pmid: 5134329 |
[60] |
Milivojevic, B., Vicente-Grabovetsky, A., & Doeller, C. F. (2015). Insight reconfigures hippocampal-prefrontal memories. Current Biology, 25(7), 821-830.
doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.01.033 pmid: 25728693 |
[61] |
Morsella, E., Ben-Zeev, A., Lanska, M., & Bargh, J. A. (2010). The spontaneous thoughts of the night: How future tasks breed intrusive cognitions. Social Cognition, 28, 641-650.
doi: 10.1521/soco.2010.28.5.641 URL |
[62] |
Moss, J., Kotovsky, K., & Cagan, J. (2007). The influence of open goals on the acquisition of problem-relevant information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33(5), 876.
doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.33.5.876 URL |
[63] |
Moss, J., Kotovsky, K., & Cagan, J. (2011). The effect of incidental hints when problems are suspended before, during, or after an impasse. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37(1), 140.
doi: 10.1037/a0021206 URL |
[64] |
Mullally, S. L., & O'Mara, S. M. (2013). Suppressing the encoding of new information in memory: A behavioral study derived from principles of hippocampal function. Plos One, 8(1), e50814.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0050814 URL |
[65] |
Nordgren, L. F., Bos, M. W., & Dijksterhuis, A. (2011). The best of both worlds: Integrating conscious and unconscious thought best solves complex decisions. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47(2), 509-511.
doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2010.12.007 URL |
[66] |
Payne, J. D. (2011). Sleep on it!: Stabilizing and transforming memories during sleep. Nature Neuroscience, 14(3), 272-274.
doi: 10.1038/nn0311-272 URL |
[67] | Poincaré, H. (1910). Mathematical creation. The Monist, 321-335. |
[68] |
Ren, J., Huang, Z. H., Luo, J., Wei, G. X., Ying, X. P., Ding, Z. G., … Luo, F. (2011). Meditation promotes insightful problem-solving by keeping people in a mindful and alert conscious state. Science China Life Sciences, 54(10), 961-965.
doi: 10.1007/s11427-011-4233-3 URL |
[69] | Ritter, S. M., & Dijksterhuis, A. (2014). Creativity-the unconscious foundations of the incubation period. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 215. |
[70] | Sahakyan, L., Delaney, P. F., Foster, N. L., & Abushanab, B. (2013). List-method directed forgetting in cognitive and clinical research: A theoretical and methodological review. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 59, 131-189. |
[71] |
Sanders, K. E., Osburn, S., Paller, K. A., & Beeman, M. (2019). Targeted memory reactivation during sleep improves next-day problem solving. Psychological Science, 30(11), 1616-1624.
doi: 10.1177/0956797619873344 URL |
[72] |
Sandkühler, S., & Bhattacharya, J. (2008). Deconstructing insight: EEG correlates of insightful problem solving. PLoS One, 3(1), e1459.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0001459 URL |
[73] |
Schönauer, M., Brodt, S., Pöhlchen, D., Breßmer, A., Danek, A. H., & Gais, S. (2018). Sleep does not promote solving classical insight problems and magic tricks. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 12, 72.
doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00072 pmid: 29535620 |
[74] | Seifert, M. C., Meyer, D. E., Davidson, N., Patalano, A. L., & Yaniv, I. (1995). Demystification of cognitive insight:Opportunistic assimilation and the prepared-mind perspective. In R. J.Sternberg & J. E.Davidson(Eds.), The nature of insight (pp.65-124). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. |
[75] |
Segal, E. (2004). Incubation in insight problem solving. Creativity Research Journal, 16(1), 141-148.
doi: 10.1207/s15326934crj1601_13 URL |
[76] |
Seyed-Allaei, S., Avanaki, Z. N., Bahrami, B., & Shallice, T. (2017). Major thought restructuring: The roles of different prefrontal cortical regions. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 29(7), 1147-1161.
doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01109 pmid: 28253076 |
[77] | Shaw, S. (2020). Creative problem-solving in mathematics: Immersion, impasse, incubation, and insight (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of California, Los Angeles. |
[78] |
Shen, W. B., Tong, Y., Li, F., Yuan, Y., Hommel, B., Liu, C., & Luo, J. (2018). Tracking the neurodynamics of insight: A meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Biological Psychology, 138, 189-198.
doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2018.08.018 URL |
[79] |
Shen, W. B., Yuan, Y., Liu, C., Zhang, X., Luo, J., & Gong, Z. (2016). Is creative insight task-specific? A coordinate- based meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies on insightful problem solving. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 110, 81-90.
doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2016.10.001 URL |
[80] |
Shen, W. B., Yuan, Y., Liu, C., & Luo, J. (2017). The roles of the temporal lobe in creative insight: An integrated review. Thinking and Reasoning, 23(4), 321-375.
doi: 10.1080/13546783.2017.1308885 URL |
[81] |
Sio, U. N., Kotovsky, K., & Cagan, J. (2017). Interrupted: The roles of distributed effort and incubation in preventing fixation and generating problem solutions. Memory & Cognition, 45(4), 553-565.
doi: 10.3758/s13421-016-0684-x URL |
[82] |
Sio, U. N., Monaghan, P., & Ormerod, T. (2013). Sleep on it, but only if it is difficult: Effects of sleep on problem solving. Memory & Cognition, 41(2), 159-166.
doi: 10.3758/s13421-012-0256-7 URL |
[83] |
Sio, U. N., & Ormerod, T. C. (2009). Does incubation enhance problem solving? A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 135(1), 94.
doi: 10.1037/a0014212 pmid: 19210055 |
[84] | Sio, U. N., & Ormerod, T. C. (2015). Incubation and cueing effects in problem-solving: Set aside the difficult problems but focus on the easy ones. Thinking & Reasoning, 21(1), 113-129. |
[85] |
Smallwood, J., & Schooler, J. W. (2006). The restless mind. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 946-958.
doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.6.946 pmid: 17073528 |
[86] | Smallwood, J., & Schooler, J. W. (2013). The restless mind. Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice, 1(S), 130-149. |
[87] | Smeekens, B. A. (2013). The role of working memory capacity and mind wandering in creativity and insight. The University of North Carolina at Greensboro. |
[88] |
Smeekens, B. A., & Kane, M. J. (2016). Working memory capacity, mind wandering, and creative cognition: An individual-differences investigation into the benefits of controlled versus spontaneous thought. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 10(4), 389.
doi: 10.1037/aca0000046 pmid: 28458764 |
[89] | Smith, C. M., Bushouse, E., & Lord, J. (2010). Individual and group performance on insight problems: The effects of experimentally induced fixation. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 13(1), 91-99. |
[90] | Smith, S. M. (1995). Getting into and out of mental ruts:A theory of fixation, incubation, and insight. In R. J.Sternberg & J. E.Davidson(Eds.), The Nature of Insight (pp.121-149). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. |
[91] |
Smith, S. M., & Beda, Z. (2020). Old problems in new contexts: The context-dependent fixation hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 149(1), 192.
doi: 10.1037/xge0000615 URL |
[92] |
Smith, S. M., & Blankenship, S. E. (1989). Incubation effects. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 27(4), 311-314.
doi: 10.3758/BF03334612 URL |
[93] | Smith, S. M., & Blankenship, S. E. (1991). Incubation and the persistence of fixation in problem solving. The American Journal of Psychology, 61-87. |
[94] | Smith, S. M., & Dodds, R. A. (1999). Incubation. In M. A.Runco & S. R.Pritzker(Eds.), Encyclopedia of creativity (pp.39-43). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. |
[95] | Smith, S. M., Sifonis, C. M., & Angello, G. (2012). Clue insensitivity in remote associates test problem solving. The Journal of Problem Solving, 4(2), 3. |
[96] |
Snyder, A., Mitchell, J., Ellwood, S., Yates, A., & Pallier, G. (2004). Nonconscious idea generation. Psychological Reports, 94(3_suppl), 1325-1330.
doi: 10.2466/pr0.94.3c.1325-1330 URL |
[97] | Steindorf, L., Hammerton, H. A., & Rummel, J. (2020). Mind wandering outside the box-About the role of off-task thoughts and their assessment during creative incubation. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts. |
[98] |
Stickgold, R. (2005). Sleep-dependent memory consolidation. Nature, 437(7063), 1272-1278.
doi: 10.1038/nature04286 URL |
[99] |
Storm, B. C., & Angello, G. (2010). Overcoming fixation: Creative problem solving and retrieval-induced forgetting. Psychological Science, 21(9), 1263-1265.
doi: 10.1177/0956797610379864 URL |
[100] |
Strick, M., Dijksterhuis, A., Bos, M. W., Sjoerdsma, A., van Baaren, R. B., & Nordgren, L. F. (2011). A meta-analysis on unconscious thought effects. Social Cognition, 29(6), 738-762.
doi: 10.1521/soco.2011.29.6.738 URL |
[101] |
Tan, T. T., Zou, H., Chen, C. S., & Luo, J. (2015). Mind wandering and the incubation effect in insight problem solving. Creativity Research Journal, 27(4), 375-382.
doi: 10.1080/10400419.2015.1088290 URL |
[102] |
Tsenn, J., Atilola, O., McAdams, D. A., & Linsey, J. S. (2014). The effects of time and incubation on design concept generation. Design Studies, 35(5), 500-526.
doi: 10.1016/j.destud.2014.02.003 URL |
[103] |
Verleger, R., Rose, M., Wagner, U., Yordanova, J., & Kolev, V. (2013). Insights into sleep's role for insight: Studies with the number reduction task. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 9(4), 160.
doi: 10.2478/v10053-008-0143-8 pmid: 24605175 |
[104] |
Wagner, U., Gais, S., Haider, H., Verleger, R., & Born, J. (2004). Sleep inspires insight. Nature, 427(6972), 352-355.
doi: 10.1038/nature02223 URL |
[105] | Wallas, G. (1926). The Art of Thought. New York, NY: Harcourt Brace. |
[106] |
Watkins, E. R. (2010). Level of construal, mind wandering, and repetitive thought: Reply to McVay and Kane. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 198-201.
doi: 10.1037/a0018563 URL |
[107] | Weisberg, R. W. (2006). Creativity: Understanding innovation in problem solving, science, invention, and the arts. John Wiley & Sons. |
[108] |
Weisberg, R. W. (2013). On the “demystification” of insight: A critique of neuroimaging studies of insight. Creativity Research Journal, 25(1), 1-14.
doi: 10.1080/10400419.2013.752178 URL |
[109] |
Yamaoka, A., & Yukawa, S. (2016). Mind-wandering enhances creative problem solving. Shinrigaku Kenkyu: The Japanese Journal of Psychology, 87(5), 506-512.
pmid: 29630183 |
[110] | Yamaoka, A., & Yukawa, S. (2017). The relationship between mind-wandering or awareness and creativity. Japanese Journal of Social Psychology, 32, 151-162. |
[111] |
Yamaoka, A., & Yukawa, S. (2020). Does mind wandering during the thought incubation period improve creativity and worsen mood?. Psychological Reports, 123(5), 1785-1800.
doi: 10.1177/0033294119896039 pmid: 31856642 |
[112] |
Yang, H., Chattopadhyay, A., Zhang, K., & Dahl, D. W. (2012). Unconscious creativity: When can unconscious thought outperform conscious thought?. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 22(4), 573-581.
doi: 10.1016/j.jcps.2012.04.002 URL |
[113] |
Yaniv, I., & Meyer, D. E. (1987). Activation and meta-cognition of inaccessible stored information: Potential bases for incubation effects in problem solving. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13(2), 187-205.
doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.13.2.187 URL |
[114] |
Yuan, Y., & Shen, W. (2016). Commentary: Incubation and intuition in creative problem solving. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1807.
pmid: 27899908 |
[115] |
Zhang, Z., Zhang, W. T., Wu, X. F., Tan, T. T., & Luo, J. (2019). Incubation optimizes the promoting effects of rewards on creativity. PsyCh Journal, 8(2), 271-272.
doi: 10.1002/pchj.251 pmid: 30408842 |
[116] |
Zhong, C. B., Dijksterhuis, A., & Galinsky, A. D. (2008). The merits of unconscious thought in creativity. Psychological Science, 19(9), 912-918.
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02176.x URL |
[117] |
Zhou, X., Zhai, H. K., Delidabieke, B., Zeng, H., Cui, Y. X., & Cao, X. (2019). Exposure to Ideas, Evaluation Apprehension, and Incubation Intervals in Collaborative Idea Generation. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 1459.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01459 URL |
[1] | 李亚丹, 杜颖, 谢聪, 刘春宇, 杨毅隆, 李阳萍, 邱江. 语义距离与创造性思维关系的元分析[J]. 心理科学进展, 2023, 31(4): 519-534. |
[2] | 姚海娟, 王琦, 李兆卿. 情绪调节中的认知重评创造力[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(3): 601-612. |
[3] | 尹俊婷, 王冠, 罗俊龙. 威胁对创造力的影响:认知与情绪双加工路径[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(5): 815-826. |
[4] | 叶超群, 林郁泓, 刘春雷. 创造力产生过程中的神经振荡机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(4): 697-706. |
[5] | 张亚坤, 陈宁, 陈龙安, 施建农. 让智慧插上创造的翅膀:创造动力系统的激活及其条件[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(4): 707-722. |
[6] | 王博韬, 魏萍. 道德情绪:探寻道德与创造力关系的新视角[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(2): 268-275. |
[7] | 张舜, 杨晓蕾, 任佳文, 张景焕. 多巴胺相关基因甲基化、家庭环境与创造力的关系[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(11): 1911-1919. |
[8] | 李铭泽, 叶慧莉, 张光磊. 自恋型领导对团队创造力形成过程的多视角研究[J]. 心理科学进展, 2020, 28(9): 1437-1453. |
[9] | 黄崇蓉, 胡瑜. 组织内信任与创造力的关系:元分析的证据[J]. 心理科学进展, 2020, 28(7): 1118-1132. |
[10] | 杨洁, 张露, 黄勇. 互联网企业玩兴氛围对创新行为的跨层次作用机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2020, 28(4): 523-534. |
[11] | 白新文, 齐舒婷, 明晓东, 周意勇, 黄明权. 骏马易见, 伯乐难寻:决策者心智模式影响创意识别的机制及边界条件[J]. 心理科学进展, 2019, 27(4): 571-586. |
[12] | 车敬上, 孙海龙, 肖晨洁, 李爱梅. 为什么信息超载损害决策?基于有限认知资源的解释[J]. 心理科学进展, 2019, 27(10): 1758-1768. |
[13] | 韩建涛, 刘文令, 庞维国. 创造力测评中的评分者效应[J]. 心理科学进展, 2019, 27(1): 171-180. |
[14] | 黄林洁琼, 刘慧瀛, 安蕾, 刘亚楠, 张舒, 金彩玉. 多元文化经历促进创造力[J]. 心理科学进展, 2018, 26(8): 1511-1520. |
[15] | 张建卫, 李海红, 刘玉新, 赵辉. 家长式领导对多层面创造力的作用机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2018, 26(7): 1319-1330. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||