心理科学进展 ›› 2020, Vol. 28 ›› Issue (7): 1164-1171.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2020.01164
收稿日期:
2019-04-04
出版日期:
2020-07-15
发布日期:
2020-05-21
通讯作者:
佐斌
E-mail:zuobin@mail.ccnu.edu.cn
基金资助:
WEN Fangfang, ZUO Bin(), MA Shuhan, XIE Zhijie
Received:
2019-04-04
Online:
2020-07-15
Published:
2020-05-21
Contact:
ZUO Bin
E-mail:zuobin@mail.ccnu.edu.cn
摘要:
面孔认知研究表明, 人们识别和再认自己所属群体(如种族、性别、年龄)面孔的成绩显著好于识别其他群体面孔的成绩。近年来围绕面孔识别的这种自我群体偏向进行了实验研究, 研究者提出了知觉经验说和社会认知论两种理论来解释其认知加工机制, 而类别化-个性化模型和双路径模型则是将两种理论分别加以整合提出的新解释。研究者还对面孔识别自我群体偏向的神经机制进行了研究, 探讨了评价者、评价对象和评价任务等因素对自我群体偏向的影响。提升面孔识别自我群体偏向研究的生态效度, 构建整合性理论模型以及加强跨文化和本土研究是未来研究的重要方向。
中图分类号:
温芳芳, 佐斌, 马书瀚, 谢志杰. (2020). 面孔识别的自我群体偏向. 心理科学进展 , 28(7), 1164-1171.
WEN Fangfang, ZUO Bin, MA Shuhan, XIE Zhijie. (2020). Own-group bias in face recognition. Advances in Psychological Science, 28(7), 1164-1171.
[1] | 吕勇, 刘亚平, 罗跃嘉. (2011). 记忆面孔, 男女有别: 关于面孔再认性别差异的行为与ERP研究. 科学通报, 56(14), 1112-1123. |
[2] | 唐卫海, 张晓沛, 唐厅, 李楠, 刘希平. (2017). 同龄效应的类别化-个体化模型. 心理科学, (3), 540-546. |
[3] | 佐斌, 温芳芳, 宋静静, 代涛涛. (2019). 社会分类的特性、维度及心理效应. 心理科学进展, 27(1), 141-148. |
[4] |
Ackerman, J. M., Shapiro, J. R., Neuberg, S. L., Kenrick, D. T., Becker, D. V., Griskevicius, V., ... Schaller, M. (2006). They all look the same to me (unless they’ re angry): From out-group homogeneity to out-group heterogeneity. Psychological Science, 17(10), 836-840.
URL pmid: 17100781 |
[5] | Bernstein, M. J., Sacco, D., Young, S. G., & Hugenberg, K. (2014). The impact of race and inclusionary status on memory for ingroup and outgroup faces. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 36(3), 191-198. |
[6] |
Blandón-Gitlin, I., Pezdek, K., Saldivar, S., & Steelman, E. (2014). Oxytocin eliminates the own-race bias in face recognition memory. Brain Research, 1580, 180-187.
doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2013.07.015 URL pmid: 23872107 |
[7] | Bower, G. H., & Karlin, M. B. (1974). Depth of processing pictures of faces and recognition memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 103(4), 751-757. |
[8] |
Cronin, S. L., Craig, B. M., & Lipp, O. V. (2019). Emotional expressions reduce the own-age bias. Emotion, 19(7), 1206-1213.
URL pmid: 30265077 |
[9] | Freeman, J. B., Ma, Y., Barth, M., Young, S. G., Han, S., & Ambady, N. (2013). The neural basis of contextual influences on face categorization. Cerebral Cortex February, 25(2), 415-422. |
[10] | Hayward, W. G., Crookes, k., & Rhodes, G. (2013). The other-race effect: Holistic coding differences and beyond. Visual Cognition, 21(9-10), 1224-1247. |
[11] | Hehman, E., Mania, E. W., & Gaertner, S. L. (2010). Where the division lies: common ingroup identity moderates the cross-race facial-recognition effect. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46(2), 445-448. |
[12] | Herlitz, A., & Lovén, J. (2013). Sex differences and the own-gender bias in face recognition: A meta-analytic review. Visual Cognition, 21(9-10), 1306-1336. |
[13] |
Hills, P. J., Pake, J. M., & Dempsey, J. R. (2018). Exploring the contribution of motivation and experience in the postpubescent own-gender bias in face recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 44(9), 1426-1446.
URL pmid: 29733672 |
[14] | Hugenberg, K., Wilson, J. P., See, P. E., & Young, S. G. (2013). Towards a synthetic model of own group biases in face memory. Visual Cognition, 21(9-10), 1392-1417. |
[15] |
Hugenberg, K., Young, S. G., Bernstein, M. J., & Sacco, D. F. (2010). The categorization-individuation model: An integrative account of the other-race recognition deficit. Psychological Review, 117(4), 1168-1187.
doi: 10.1037/a0020463 URL pmid: 20822290 |
[16] |
Kawakami, K., Williams, A., Sidhu, D., Choma, B. L., Rodriguez-Bailón, R., Cañadas, E., & Hugenberg, K. (2014). An eye for the I: Preferential attention to the eyes of ingroup members. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 107(1), 1-20.
URL pmid: 24956311 |
[17] | Kuiper, N. A., & Rogers, T. B. (1979). Encoding of personal information: Self-other differences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37(4), 499-514. |
[18] |
Lewin, C., & Herlitz, A. (2002). Sex differences in face recognition-women's faces make the difference. Brain & Cognition, 50(1), 121-128.
URL pmid: 12372357 |
[19] |
Lovén, J., Herlitz, A., & Rehnman, J. (2011). Women's own-gender bias in face recognition memory. Experimental Psychology, 58(4), 333-340.
doi: 10.1027/1618-3169/a000100 URL pmid: 21310695 |
[20] | Man, T. W., & Hills, P. J. (2017). Eye-tracking the own-gender bias in face recognition: Other-gender faces are viewed differently to own-gender faces. Visual Cognition, 24(9-10), 447-458. |
[21] | Martschuk, N., & Sporer, S. l. (2018). Memory for faces in old age: A meta-analysis. Psychology and Aging, 33(6), 904-923. |
[22] | McKone, E., Aitkin, A., & Edwards, M. (2005). Categorical and coordinate relations in faces, or fechner's law and face space instead? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31(6), 1181-1198. |
[23] |
Mckone, E., Hall, A., Pidcock, M., Palermo, R., Wilkinson, R. B., Rivolta, D., et al. (2011). Face ethnicity and measurement reliability affect face recognition performance in developmental prosopagnosia: Evidence from the Cambridge face memory test - Australian. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 28(2), 109-146.
URL pmid: 22122116 |
[24] |
Michel, C., Corneille, O., & Rossion, B. (2007). Race categorization modulates holistic face encoding. Cognitive Science, 31(5), 911-924.
URL pmid: 21635322 |
[25] |
Michel, C., Corneille, O., & Rossion, B. (2010). Race categorization modulates holistic face encoding. Cognitive Science, 31(5), 911-924.
URL pmid: 21635322 |
[26] | Motta-Mena, N., Picci, G., & Scherf, S. (2016). Dominance elicits the own-gender bias in males. Journal of Vision, 16(12), 497. |
[27] | Motta-Mena, Picci, & Scherf. (2016). Dominance elicits the own-gender bias in males. Journal of Vision, 16(12), 494. |
[28] | Mueller, J. H., Bailis, K. L., & Goldstein, A. G. (1979). Depth of processing and anxiety in facial recognition. British Journal of Psychology, 70(4), 511-515. |
[29] | Ofan, R. H., Rubin, N., & Amodio, D. M. (2011). Seeing race: N170 responses to race and their relation to automatic racial attitudes and controlled processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(10), 3151-3161. |
[30] |
Pauker, K., Weisbuch, M., Ambady, N., Sommers, S. R., Adams, R. B., & Ivcevic, Z. (2009). Not so black and white: Memory for ambiguous group members. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 96(4), 795-810.
URL pmid: 19309203 |
[31] |
Picci, G., Gotts, S. J., & Scherf, K. S. (2016). A theoretical rut: Revisiting and critically evaluating the generalized under/over-connectivity hypothesis of autism. Developmental Science, 19(4), 524-549.
URL pmid: 27412228 |
[32] |
Proietti, V., Cassia, V. M., & Mondloch, C. J. (2015). The own-age face recognition bias is task dependent. British Journal of Psychology, 106(3), 446-467.
URL pmid: 25491773 |
[33] | Rhodes, G., Simmons, L. W., & Peters, M. (2005). Attractiveness and sexual behavior: Does attractiveness enhance mating success? Evolution and Human Behavior, 26(2), 186-201. |
[34] |
Rhodes, M. G., & Anastasi, J. S. (2012). The own-age bias in face recognition: A meta-analytic and theoretical review. Psychological Bulletin, 138(1), 146-74.
doi: 10.1037/a0025750 URL pmid: 22061689 |
[35] | Rogers, T. B., Rogers, P. J., & Kuiper, N. A. (1979). Evidence for the self as a cognitive prototype: The "false alarms effect." Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 5(1), 53-56. |
[36] | Rule, N. O., Ambady, N., Adams, R. B., & Macrae, C. N. (2007). Us and them: Memory advantages in perceptually ambiguous groups. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 4, 687-692. |
[37] |
Rule, N. O., Garrett, J. V., & Ambady, N. (2010). Places and faces: Geographic environment influences the ingroup memory advantage. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98(3), 343-355.
doi: 10.1037/a0018589 URL pmid: 20175617 |
[38] | Short, L. A., Semplonius, T., Proietti, V., & Mondloch, C. J. (2014). Differential attentional allocation and subsequent recognition for young verus older adult faces. Visual Cognition, 22(9-10), 1272-1295. |
[39] | Sporer, S. L. (1991). Deep-Deeper-Deepest? Encoding strategies and the recognition of human faces. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 17(2), 323-333. |
[40] | Sporer, S. L. (2001). Recognizing faces of other ethnic groups: An integration of theories. Psychology Public Policy & Law, 7(1), 36-97. |
[41] | Sporer, S. L., & Horry, R. (2011). Recognizing faces from ethnic in-groups and out‐groups: Importance of outer face features and effects of retention interval. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 25(3), 424-431. |
[42] | Tanaka, J. W., Heptonstall, B., & Hagen, S. (2013). Perceptual expertise and the plasticity of other-race face recognition. Visual Cognition, 21(9-10), 1183-1201. |
[43] |
Tham, D. S. Y., Bremner, J. G., & Hay, D. (2015). In infancy the timing of emergence of the other-race effect is dependent on face gender. Infant Behavior and Development, 40, 131-138.
doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2015.05.006 URL pmid: 26143499 |
[44] |
Valentine, T. (1991). A unified account of the effects of distinctiveness, inversion, and race in face recognition. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A: Human Experimental Psychology, 43(2), 161-204.
URL pmid: 1866456 |
[45] |
van, Bavel, J., J., & Cunningham, W. A. (2012). A social identity approach to person memory: Group membership, collective identification, and social role shape attention and memory. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38(12), 1566-1578.
doi: 10.1177/0146167212455829 URL pmid: 22914991 |
[46] |
Wang, B. (2013). Gender difference in recognition memory for neutral and emotional faces. Memory, 21(8), 991-1003.
doi: 10.1080/09658211.2013.771273 URL pmid: 23432017 |
[47] |
Wan, L. L., Crookes, K., Reynolds, K. J., Irons, J. L., & McKone, E. (2015). A cultural setting where the other-race effect on face recognition has no social-motivational component and derives entirely from lifetime perceptual experience. Cognition, 144, 91-115.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2015.07.011 URL pmid: 26257000 |
[48] | Wiese, H., Komes, J., & Schweinberger, S. R. (2013). Ageing faces in ageing minds: A review on the own-age bias in face recognition. Visual Cognition, 21(9-10), 1337-1363. |
[49] |
Wiese, H., & Schweinberger, S. R. (2018). Inequality between biases in face memory: Event-related potentials reveal dissociable neural correlates of own-race and own-gender biases. Cortex, 101, 119-135.
doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2018.01.016 URL pmid: 29475077 |
[50] |
Wilson, J. P., See, P. E., Bernstein, M. J., Hugenberg, K., & Chartier, C. (2014). Differences in anticipated interaction drive own group biases in face memory. PloS One, 9(3), e90668.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0090668 URL pmid: 24599294 |
[51] |
Wolff, N., Kemter, K., Schweinberger, S. R., & Wiese, H. (2014). What drives social in-group biases in face recognition memory? ERP evidence from the own-gender bias. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9(5), 580-590.
URL pmid: 23474824 |
[52] |
Yan, L., Zhe, W., Huang, J., Sun, Y. H. P., Judges, R. A., Xiao, N. G., ... Lee, K. (2017). Own-group face recognition bias: The effects of location and reputation. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1734.
URL pmid: 29066989 |
[53] | Young, S. G., Bernstein, M. J., & Hugenberg, K. (2010). When do own-group biases in face recognition occur? Encoding versus post-encoding. Social Cognition, 28(2), 240-250. |
[54] | Young, S. G., & Hugenberg, K. (2012). Individuation motivation and face experience can operate jointly to produce the own-race bias. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 3(1), 80-87. |
[1] | 那宇亭, 赵宇雯, 关丽丽. 自我面孔识别的神经机制:基于fMRI研究的ALE元分析[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(10): 1783-1795. |
[2] | 周爱保, 谢珮, 崔嘉薇. 视听整合任务下精神分裂症患者自我面孔对声音识别的影响[J]. 心理科学进展, 2019, 27(suppl.): 3-3. |
[3] | 胡冰洁, 严璘璘, 夏文玲. 群体地位对面孔识别的影响[J]. 心理科学进展, 2019, 27(suppl.): 38-38. |
[4] | 王亚飞, 罗健亮, 杨糠, 黄飞云, 王玲. 多面孔识别过程中的视觉搜索策略[J]. 心理科学进展, 2019, 27(suppl.): 112-112. |
[5] | 于明阳, 李富洪, 曹碧华, . 愉快面孔识别优势及其认知神经机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2018, 26(2): 254-261. |
[6] | 钱浩悦;朱敏;高湘萍. 老年人面孔构形加工特点[J]. 心理科学进展, 2017, 25(2): 230-236. |
[7] | 张凯莉, 张琴, 周静, 王沛. 面孔识别过程中多重社会亚范畴的交互作用[J]. 心理科学进展, 2017, 25(11): 1955-1963. |
[8] | 李琎; 范伟; 张文洁; 钟毅平. 旋转自我面孔识别及其认知机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2016, 24(Suppl.): 50-. |
[9] | 冯新然; 周国梅. 学习时间和面孔运动模式对动态面孔本族效应的影响[J]. 心理科学进展, 2016, 24(Suppl.): 79-. |
[10] | 王原野; 罗欢. 面孔启动效应中的行为振荡[J]. 心理科学进展, 2016, 24(Suppl.): 96-. |
[11] | 马建苓;陈旭;王婧. 自我面孔识别的特征、影响因素及ERP研究述评[J]. 心理科学进展, 2012, 20(2): 240-247. |
[12] | 关丽丽;齐铭铭;张庆林;杨娟. 自我面孔识别的脑机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2011, 19(9): 1313-1318. |
[13] | 汪亚珉;黄雅梅. 面孔识别中的构形加工与特征加工[J]. 心理科学进展, 2011, 19(8): 1126-1137. |
[14] | 王玉娜;陈楚侨. 自我缺损与精神分裂症[J]. 心理科学进展, 2010, 18(12): 1882-1891. |
[15] | 李明芳;张烨;张庆林. 面孔识别中脑电成分N170的研究概述[J]. 心理科学进展, 2010, 18(12): 1942-1948. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||