心理科学进展 ›› 2018, Vol. 26 ›› Issue (12): 2260-2271.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2018.02260
收稿日期:
2018-01-29
出版日期:
2018-12-15
发布日期:
2018-10-30
通讯作者:
廖江群
E-mail:liaojq@tsinghua.edu.cn
基金资助:
Received:
2018-01-29
Online:
2018-12-15
Published:
2018-10-30
Contact:
LIAO Jiangqun
E-mail:liaojq@tsinghua.edu.cn
摘要:
最近的研究发现某些具身效应不能被重复, 这给心理学界带来了空前的可重复性危机。基于对具身认知“概念化”、“替代”和“构成”主题的分析, 通过区分具身效应的生成层次、强弱条件、建构方向和任务特性四个方面, 以便在实验中更准确地“定位”具身效应。未来研究可以从具身效应层次的分离与综合, 强弱条件的界分, 调节或中介机制探讨以及实验范式革新等方面加以推进。
中图分类号:
刘传军, 廖江群. (2018). 具身效应何处寻:解决可重复性危机的分析性途径. 心理科学进展 , 26(12), 2260-2271.
LIU Chuanjun, LIAO Jiangqun. (2018). An analytical approach to understanding and solving the replication crisis of the embodiment effect. Advances in Psychological Science, 26(12), 2260-2271.
1 |
陈巍 . ( 2014). 可重复性: 盘旋在具身认知实验室上方的“幽灵”. 心理技术与应用, ( 1), 23-25.
doi: 10.3969/j.issn.2095-5588.2014.01.006 URL |
2 |
陈巍, 郭本禹 . ( 2014). 具身-生成的认知科学: 走出“战国时代”. 心理学探新, 34( 2), 111-116.
doi: 10.3969/j.issn.1003-5184.2014.02.003 URL |
3 | 陈潇, 江琦, 侯敏, 朱梦音 . ( 2014). 具身道德: 道德心理学研究的新取向. 心理发展与教育, 30( 6), 664-672. |
4 | 方溦, 葛列众, 甘甜 . ( 2016). 道德具身认知的理论研究. 心理与行为研究, 14( 6), 765-772. |
5 |
韩冬, 叶浩生 . ( 2014). 重中之“重”——具身视角下重的体验与表征. 心理科学进展, 22( 6), 918-925.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2014.00918 URL |
6 | 何华容, 丁道群 . ( 2014). 良心的重量: 基于具身视角的内疚情绪与重量感关系的实验研究. 第十七届全国心理学学术会议, 中国北京. |
7 | 黄茜羽, 李宏翰 . ( 2013). 冷暖体验对道德判断的影响. 心理学与创新能力提升——第十六届全国心理学学术会议, 中国南京. |
8 |
胡传鹏, 王非, 过继成思, 宋梦迪, 隋洁, 彭凯平 . ( 2016). 心理学研究中的可重复性问题: 从危机到契机. 心理科学进展, 24( 9), 1504-1518.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2016.01504 URL |
9 |
黎晓丹, 杜建政, 叶浩生 . ( 2016). 中国礼文化的具身隐喻效应: 蜷缩的身体使人更卑微. 心理学报, 48( 6), 746-756.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2016.00746 URL |
10 |
刘亚, 王振宏, 孔风 . ( 2011). 情绪具身观: 情绪研究的新视角. 心理科学进展, 19( 1), 50-59.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2011.00050 URL |
11 |
刘钊, 丁凤琴 . ( 2016). 大学生道德概念的重量与洁净隐喻. 中国健康心理学杂志, 24( 4), 533-536.
doi: 10.13342/j.cnki.cjhp.2016.04.014 URL |
12 | 栾子烟 . ( 2013). 道德温度:身体冷暖与青少年道德决策(硕士学位论文). 上海:华东师范大学. |
13 | 牛怡然, 鲁忠义 . ( 2013). 道德概念的亮度隐喻表征及其对亮度知觉判断的影响 . 心理学与创新能力提升——第十六届全国心理学学术会议, 中国南京. |
14 | 彭凯平, 喻丰 . ( 2012). 道德的心理物理学现象、机制与意义. 中国社会科学, ( 12), 28-45. |
15 | 苏彦捷, 孙芳芳 . ( 2014). 道德具身性的元分析研究. 华东师范大学学报(教育科学版), 32( 2), 88-96. |
16 |
伍秋萍, 冯聪, 陈斌斌 . ( 2011). 具身框架下的社会认知研究述评. 心理科学进展, 19( 3), 336-345.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2011.00336 URL |
17 | 薛灿灿, 叶浩生 . ( 2011). 具身社会认知: 认知心理学的生态学转向. 心理科学, 34( 5), 1230-1235. |
18 | 阎书昌 . ( 2011). 身体洁净与道德. 心理科学进展, 19( 8), 1242-1248. |
19 | 杨继平, 郭秀梅 . ( 2016). 具身视角下道德概念的隐喻研究. 心理学探新, 36( 5), 387-391. |
20 |
杨继平, 郭秀梅, 王兴超 . ( 2017). 道德概念的隐喻表征——从红白颜色、左右位置和正斜字体的维度. 心理学报, 49( 7), 875-885.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2017.00875 URL |
21 | 叶浩生 . ( 2010). 具身认知: 认知心理学的新取向. 心理科学进展, 18( 5), 705-710. |
22 |
叶浩生 . ( 2013). 认知与身体: 理论心理学的视角. 心理学报, 45( 4), 481-488.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2013.00481 URL |
23 |
殷融, 曲方炳, 叶浩生 . ( 2012). 具身概念表征的研究及理论述评. 心理科学进展, 20( 9), 1372-1381.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2012.01372 URL |
24 |
殷融, 苏得权, 叶浩生 . ( 2013). 具身认知视角下的概念隐喻理论. 心理科学进展, 21( 2), 220-234.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2013.00220 URL |
25 | 翟贤亮, 葛鲁嘉 . ( 2017). 从离身到具身: 具身管理学的可能. 心理科学, 40( 1), 238-243. |
26 |
Ackerman J. M., Nocera C. C., & Bargh J. A . ( 2010). Incidental haptic sensations influence social judgments and decisions. Science, 328( 5986), 1712-1715.
doi: 10.1126/science.1189993 URL pmid: 20576894 |
27 |
Andy, C(2008 ). Supersizing the mind: Embodiment, action, and cognitive extension. New York: Oxford University Press.
doi: 10.1016/S0920-5861(02)00217-1 URL |
28 |
Bai Y., Maruskin L. A., Chen S., Gordon A. M., Stellar J. E., McNeil G. D., .. Keltner D . ( 2017). Awe, the diminished self, and collective engagement: Universals and cultural variations in the small self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 113( 2), 185-209.
doi: 10.1037/pspa0000087 URL pmid: 28481617 |
29 |
Banerjee P., Chatterjee P., & Sinha J . ( 2012). Is it light or dark? Recalling moral behavior changes perception of brightness. Psychological Science, 23( 4), 407-409.
doi: 10.1177/0956797611432497 URL pmid: 22395128 |
30 |
Bargh J. A., Chen M., & Burrows L . ( 1996). Automaticity of social behavior: Direct effects of trait construct and stereotype-activation on action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71( 2), 230-244.
doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.71.2.230 URL pmid: 8765481 |
31 | Carney D. R., Cuddy A. J., & Yap A. J . ( 2010). Power posing: Brief nonverbal displays affect neuroendocrine levels and risk tolerance. Psychological Science, 21( 10), 1363-1368. |
32 |
Chapman H. A., Kim D. A., Susskind J. M., & Anderson A. K . ( 2009). In bad taste: Evidence for the oral origins of moral disgust. Science, 323( 5918), 1222-1226.
doi: 10.1126/science.1165565 URL pmid: 19251631 |
33 |
Chartrand T. L., Huber J., Shiv B., & Tanner R. J . ( 2008). Nonconscious goals and consumer choice. Journal of Consumer Research, 35( 2), 189-201.
doi: 10.1086/588685 URL |
34 |
Day, M.V., &Bobocel, D.R . ( 2013). The weight of a guilty conscience: Subjective body weight as an embodiment of guilt. PLoS One, 8( 7), e69546.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0069546 URL pmid: 3729967 |
35 |
Dijksterhuis, Ap, &Nordgren, Loran F . ( 2006). A theory of unconscious thought. Perspectives on Psychological Science: A Journal of the Association for Psychological Science, 1( 2), 95-109.
doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6916.2006.00007.x URL pmid: 26151465 |
36 |
Doyen S., Klein O., Pichon C. L., & Cleeremans A . ( 2012). Behavioral priming: It's all in the mind, but whose mind? Plos One, 7( 1), e29081.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0029081 URL pmid: 22279526 |
37 |
Eisenberger N. I., Lieberman M. D., & Williams K. D . ( 2003). Does rejection hurt? An fMRI study of social exclusion. Science, 302( 5643), 290-292.
doi: 10.1126/science.1089134 URL pmid: 14551436 |
38 |
Eskine K. J., Kacinik N. A., & Prinz J. J . ( 2011). A bad taste in the mouth: Gustatory disgust influences moral judgment. Psychological Science, 22( 3), 295-299.
doi: 10.1177/0956797611398497 URL pmid: 21307274 |
39 |
Eskine K. J., Kacinik N. A., & Webster G. D . ( 2012). The bitter truth about morality: Virtue, not vice, makes a bland beverage taste nice. PLoS One, 7( 7), e41159.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0041159 URL pmid: 3399822 |
40 |
Ferrin, A. ( 2017). Good moral judgment and decision-making without deliberation. Southern Journal of Philosophy, 55( 1), 68-95.
doi: 10.1111/sjp.12210 URL |
41 |
Fischer J., Fischer P., Englich B., Aydin N., & Frey D . ( 2011). Empower my decisions: The effects of power gestures on confirmatory information processing. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47( 6), 1146-1154.
doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2011.06.008 URL |
42 | Garrison K. E., Tang D., & Schmeichel B. J . ( 2016). Embodying power: A preregistered replication and extension of the power pose effect. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 7( 7), 623-630. |
43 |
Gilead M., Gal O., Polak M., & Cholow Y . ( 2015). The role of nature and nurture in conceptual metaphors the case of gustatory priming. Social Psychology, 46( 3), 167-173.
doi: 10.1027/1864-9335/a000238 URL |
44 | Giner-Sorolla, R., &Chapman, H.A . ( 2017). Beyond Purity. Psychological Science, 28( 1), 80-91. |
45 |
Goldinger S. D., Papesh M. H., Barnhart A. S., Hansen W. A., & Hout M. C . ( 2016). The poverty of embodied cognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23( 4), 959-978.
doi: 10.3758/s13423-015-0860-1 URL pmid: 27282990 |
46 |
Greene, J.D. ( 2007). Why are VMPFC patients more utilitarian? A dual-process theory of moral judgment explains. Trends In Cognitive Sciences, 11( 8), 322-323.
doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.06.004 URL pmid: 17625951 |
47 |
Greene J. D., Morelli S. A., Lowenberg K., Nystrom L. E., & Cohen J. D . ( 2008). Cognitive load selectively interferes with utilitarian moral judgment. Cognition, 107( 3), 1144-1154.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.11.004 URL pmid: 2429958 |
48 |
Gronau Q. F., Van Erp S., Heck D. W., Cesario J., Jonas K. J., & Wagenmakers E.-J . ( 2017). A Bayesian model-averaged meta-analysis of the power pose effect with informed and default priors: The case of felt power. Comprehensive Results in Social Psychology, 2( 1), 123-138.
doi: 10.1080/23743603.2017.1326760 URL |
49 |
Helzer, E.G., &Pizarro, D.A . ( 2011). Dirty liberals! reminders of physical cleanliness influence moral and political attitudes. Psychological Science, 22( 4), 517-522.
doi: 10.1177/0956797611402514 URL pmid: 21421934 |
50 |
Homan P., Ely B. A., Yuan M., Brosch T., Ng J., Trope Y., & Schiller D . ( 2017). Aversive smell associations shape social judgment. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 144, 86-95.
doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2017.07.004 URL |
51 |
Horberg E. J., Oveis C., Keltner D., & Cohen A. B . ( 2009). Disgust and the moralization of purity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97( 6), 963-976.
doi: 10.1037/a0017423 URL pmid: 19968413 |
52 |
IJzerman, H., &Koole, S.L . ( 2011). From perceptual rags to metaphoric riches-bodily, social, and cultural constraints on sociocognitive metaphors: Comment on Landau, Meier, and Keefer (2010). Psychological Bulletin, 137( 2), 355-361.
doi: 10.1037/a0022373 URL pmid: 21355634 |
53 |
Inbar Y., Pizarro D. A., & Bloom P . ( 2009). Conservatives are more easily disgusted than liberals. Cognition & Emotion, 23( 4), 714-725.
doi: 10.1080/02699930802110007 URL |
54 |
Inbar Y., Pizarro D. A., Knobe J., & Bloom P . ( 2009). Disgust sensitivity predicts intuitive disapproval of gays. Emotion, 9( 3), 435-439.
doi: 10.1037/a0015960 URL pmid: 19485621 |
55 |
Johnson D. J., Cheung F., & Donnellan M. B . ( 2014). Does cleanliness influence moral judgments? A direct replication of Schnall, Benton, and Harvey (2008). Social Psychology, 45( 3), 209-215.
doi: 10.1027/1864-9335/a000186 URL |
56 |
Kang, E., &Lakshmanan, A . ( 2017). Role of executive attention in consumer learning with background music. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 27( 1), 35-48.
doi: 10.1016/j.jcps.2016.03.003 URL |
57 |
Kang Y., Williams L. E., Clark M. S., Gray J. R., & Bargh J. A . ( 2011). Physical temperature effects on trust behavior: The role of insula. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 6( 4), 507-515.
doi: 10.1093/scan/nsq077 URL pmid: 3150863 |
58 |
Landau M. J., Meier B. P., & Keefer L. A . ( 2010). A metaphor-enriched social cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 136( 6), 1045-1067.
doi: 10.1037/a0020970 URL pmid: 20822208 |
59 |
Landy, J.F., &Goodwin, G.P . ( 2015). Does incidental disgust amplify moral judgment? a meta-analytic review of experimental evidence. Perspectives on Psychological Science A Journal of The Association for Psychological Science, 10( 4), 518-536.
doi: 10.1177/1745691615583128 URL pmid: 26177951 |
60 | Lee, S. W.S., &Schwarz, N . ( 2011). Wiping the slate clean: Psychological consequences of physical cleansing. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20( 5), 307-311. |
61 |
Lee, S. W.S., &Schwarz, N . ( 2012). Bidirectionality, mediation, and moderation of metaphorical effects: The embodiment of social suspicion and fishy smells. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 103( 5), 737-749.
doi: 10.1037/a0029708 URL pmid: 22905770 |
62 |
Liljenquist K., Zhong C. B., & Galinsky A. D . ( 2010). The smell of virtue: Clean scents promote reciprocity and charity. Psychological Science, 21( 3), 381-383.
doi: 10.1177/0956797610361426 URL pmid: 2042407420 |
63 |
Meier B. P., Moeller S. K., Riemer-Peltz M., & Robinson M. D . ( 2012). Sweet taste preferences and experiences predict prosocial inferences, personalities, and behaviors. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102( 1), 163-174.
doi: 10.1037/a0025253 URL pmid: 21875232 |
64 | Meier B. P., Simone S., Norbert S., & Bargh J. A . ( 2012). Embodiment in social psychology. Topics in Cognitive Science, 4( 4), 705-716. |
65 |
Montoro P. R., Contreras M. J., Elosúa M. R., & Marmolejoramos F . ( 2015). Cross-modal metaphorical mapping of spoken emotion words onto vertical space. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1205.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01205 URL pmid: 4531208 |
66 |
Moore, C., &Barresi, J . ( 2017). The role of second-person information in the development of social understanding. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1667.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01667 URL pmid: 5622194 |
67 |
Mukherjee S., Kramer T., & Kulow K . ( 2017). The effect of spicy gustatory sensations on variety-seeking. Psychology & Marketing, 34( 8), 786-794.
doi: 10.1002/mar.21022 URL |
68 |
Natanzon, M., &Ferguson, M.J . ( 2012). Goal pursuit is grounded: The link between forward movement and achievement. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48( 1), 379-382.
doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2011.06.021 URL |
69 | Niedenthal, P.M. ( 2007). Embodying emotion. Science, 316( 5827), 1002-1005. |
70 |
Nosek B. A., Hawkins C. B., & Frazier R. S . ( 2011). Implicit social cognition: From measures to mechanisms. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15( 4), 152-159.
doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.01.005 URL pmid: 3073696 |
71 |
Oosterwijk S., Rotteveel M., Fischer A. H., & Hess U . ( 2009). Embodied emotion concepts: How generating words about pride and disappointment influences posture. European Journal of Social Psychology, 39( 3), 457-466.
doi: 10.1002/ejsp.584 URL |
72 |
Preston, J.L., &Ritter, R.S . ( 2012). Cleanliness and godliness: Mutual association between two kinds of personal purity. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48( 6), 1365-1368.
doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2012.05.015 URL |
73 |
Price, T.F., &Harmon-Jones, E . ( 2015). Embodied emotion: The influence of manipulated facial and bodily states on emotive responses. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews-Cognitive Science, 6( 6), 461-473.
doi: 10.1002/wcs.1370 URL pmid: 26401657 |
74 |
Ranehill E., Dreber A., Johannesson M., Leiberg S., Sul S., & Weber R. A . ( 2015). Assessing the robustness of power posing: No effect on hormones and risk tolerance in a large sample of men and women. Psychological Science, 26( 5), 653-656.
doi: 10.1177/0956797614553946 URL pmid: 25810452 |
75 |
Ritter, R.S., &Preston, J.L . ( 2011). Gross gods and icky atheism: Disgust responses to rejected religious beliefs. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47( 6), 1225-1230.
doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2011.05.006 URL |
76 | Rotella, K.N., &Richeson, J.A . ( 2013). Body of guilt: Using embodied cognition to mitigate backlash to reminders of personal & ingroup wrongdoing. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49( 4), 643-650. |
77 |
Rozin P., Haidt J., & Fincher K . ( 2009). Psychology. From oral to moral. Science, 323( 5918), 1179-1180.
doi: 10.1126/science.1170492 URL pmid: 19251619 |
78 |
Rozin P., Lowery L., Imada S., & Haidt J . ( 1999). The CAD triad hypothesis: A mapping between three moral emotions (contempt, anger, disgust) and three moral codes (community, autonomy, divinity). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76( 4), 574-586.
doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.76.4.574 URL pmid: 10234846 |
79 |
Schaefer M., Denke C., Heinze H. J., & Rotte M . ( 2014). Rough primes and rough conversations: Evidence for a modality-specific basis to mental metaphors. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9( 11), 1653-1659.
doi: 10.1093/scan/nst163 URL pmid: 4221208 |
80 |
Schaefer M., Rotte M., Heinze H. J., & Denke C . ( 2015). Dirty deeds and dirty bodies: Embodiment of the Macbeth effect is mapped topographically onto the somatosensory cortex. Scientific Reports, 5, 18051.
doi: 10.1038/srep18051 URL pmid: 26686599 |
81 | Schilbach L., Timmermans B., Reddy V., Costall A., Bente G., Schlicht T., & Vogeley K . ( 2013). Toward a second-person neuroscience. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36( 4), 393-462. |
82 |
Schnall, S. ( 2017). Disgust as embodied loss aversion. European Review of Social Psychology, 28( 1), 50-94.
doi: 10.1080/10463283.2016.1259844 URL |
83 |
Schnall S., Benton J., & Harvey S . ( 2008). With a clean conscience: Cleanliness reduces the severity of moral judgments. Psychological Science, 19( 12), 1219-1222.
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02227.x URL pmid: 19121126 |
84 | Schnall S., Haidt J., Clore G. L., & Jordan A. H . ( 2008). Disgust as embodied moral judgment. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 34( 8), 1096-1109. |
85 |
Seidel, A., &Prinz, J . ( 2013). Sound morality: Irritating and icky noises amplify judgments in divergent moral domains. Cognition, 127( 1), 1-5.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2012.11.004 URL pmid: 23318349 |
86 | ShapiroL. ( 2011). Embodied Cognition. London and New York: Rutledge. |
87 |
Sheikh S., Botindari L., & White E . ( 2013). Embodied metaphors and emotions in the moralization of restrained eating practices. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49( 3), 509-513.
doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2012.12.016 URL |
88 |
Sherman, G.D., &Clore, G.L . ( 2009). The color of sin: White and black are perceptual symbols of moral purity and pollution. Psychological Science, 20( 8), 1019-1025.
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02403.x URL pmid: 2832478 |
89 |
Skarlicki D. P., Hoegg J., Aquino K., & Nadisic T . ( 2013). Does injustice affect your sense of taste and smell? The mediating role of moral disgust. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49( 5), 852-859.
doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2013.03.011 URL |
90 |
Song H. J., Vonasch A. J., Meier B. P., & Bargh J. A . ( 2012). Brighten up: Smiles facilitate perceptual judgment of facial lightness. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48( 1), 450-452.
doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2011.10.003 URL |
91 |
Valdesolo P., Ouyang J., & DeSteno D . ( 2010). The rhythm of joint action: Synchrony promotes cooperative ability. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46( 4), 693-695.
doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2010.03.004 URL |
92 |
Wei W. Q., Lu J. G., Galinsky A. D., Wu H., Gosling S. D., Rentfrow P. J., .. Wang L . ( 2017). Regional ambient temperature is associated with human personality. Nature Human Behaviour, 1( 12), 890-895.
doi: 10.1038/s41562-017-0240-0 URL |
93 |
Williams, L.E., &Bargh, J.A . ( 2008). Experiencing physical warmth promotes interpersonal warmth. Science, 322( 5901), 606-607.
doi: 10.1126/science.1162548 URL pmid: 2737341 |
94 |
Wilson, M. ( 2002). Six views of embodied cognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9( 4), 625-636.
doi: 10.3758/BF03196322 URL pmid: 12613670 |
95 |
Wiswede D., MÜnte T. F., Krämer U. M., & RÜsseler J . ( 2009). Embodied emotion modulates neural signature of performance monitoring. Plos One, 4( 6), e5754.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0005754 URL pmid: 2685014 |
96 |
Xu A. J., Zwick R., & Schwarz N . ( 2012). Washing away your (good or bad) luck: Physical cleansing affects risk-taking behavior. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141( 1), 26-30.
doi: 10.1037/a0023997 URL pmid: 21707206 |
97 | Yan Z. X., Ding D. Q., & Yan L. S . ( 2011). To wash your body, or purify your soul: Physical cleansing would strengthen the sense of high moral character. Psychology, 2( 9), 992-997. |
98 |
Yang Q., Wu X. C., Zhou X. Y., Mead N. L., Vohs K. D., & Baumeister R. F . ( 2013). Diverging effects of clean versus dirty money on attitudes, values, and interpersonal behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104( 3), 473-489.
doi: 10.1037/a0030596 URL pmid: 23127377 |
99 |
Zapatafonseca L., Froese T., Schilbach L., Vogeley K., & Timmermans B . ( 2018). Sensitivity to social contingency in adults with high-functioning autism during computer- mediated embodied interaction. Behavioral Science, 8, 22.
doi: 10.3390/bs8020022 URL pmid: 29419758 |
100 |
Zestcott C. A., Stone J., & Landau M. J . ( 2017). The role of conscious attention in how weight serves as an embodiment of importance. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 43( 12), 1712-1723.
doi: 10.1177/0146167217727505 URL |
101 |
Zhang, M., &Li, X.P . ( 2012). From physical weight to psychological significance: The contribution of semantic activations. Journal of Consumer Research, 38( 6), 1063-1075.
doi: 10.1086/661768 URL |
102 | Zhong, C.B., &Liljenquist, K . ( 2006). Washing away your sins: Threatened morality and physical cleansing. Science, 313( 5792), 1451-1452. |
103 |
Ziv N., Hoftman M., & Geyer M . ( 2012). Music and moral judgment: The effect of background music on the evaluation of ads promoting unethical behavior. Psychology of Music, 40( 6), 738-760.
doi: 10.1177/0305735611406579 URL |
[1] | 黄顺森, 陈豪杰, 来枭雄, 代欣然, 王耘. 多元宇宙样分析:简介及应用[J]. 心理科学进展, 2023, 31(2): 196-208. |
[2] | 杨雪, 朱旭. 情感触摸对心理健康的影响及应用[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(12): 2789-2798. |
[3] | 金雨薇, 孙潇, 宋耀武. 具身记忆及其内在机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(11): 2497-2506. |
[4] | 白亚停, 何文广. 二语具身认知:自动激活还是母语中介?[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(11): 1970-1978. |
[5] | 于文华, 鲁忠义. 手势认知功能研究的新视角:“空间化”手势假设[J]. 心理科学进展, 2020, 28(3): 426-433. |
[6] | 熊素红, 孙洪杰, 陆佶, 姚琦, 张全成. 具身认知视角的饮食消费行为——基于心理模拟“具身”方式[J]. 心理科学进展, 2020, 28(3): 486-496. |
[7] | 李莹, 张灿, 王悦. 道德情绪在道德隐喻映射中的作用及其神经机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2019, 27(7): 1224-1231. |
[8] | 殷继兴, 胡传鹏. 神经科学偏见效应:可重复性及其心理机制的探索[J]. 心理科学进展, 2019, 27(12): 1988-1995. |
[9] | 陈琳, 田晓明, 段锦云. 建议采纳的认知机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2019, 27(1): 149-159. |
[10] | 魏华, 段海岑, 周宗奎. 具身认知视角下的消费者行为[J]. 心理科学进展, 2018, 26(7): 1294-1306. |
[11] | 李恒. “前”、“后”轴上的内隐时空映射及其影响因素 *[J]. 心理科学进展, 2018, 26(6): 975-983. |
[12] | 路红, 张心园, 韦文琦, 刘毅. 空间物理秩序对心理和行为的影响[J]. 心理科学进展, 2018, 26(3): 560-570. |
[13] | 胡传鹏;王非;过继成思;宋梦迪; 隋洁;彭凯平. 心理学研究中的可重复性问题:从危机到契机[J]. 心理科学进展, 2016, 24(9): 1504-1518. |
[14] | 孙炳丽;田雨;孙海洋;王文忠. 从心理咨询与治疗的角度看自我[J]. 心理科学进展, 2016, 24(1): 83-90. |
[15] | 叶红燕;张凤华. 从具身视角看道德判断[J]. 心理科学进展, 2015, 23(8): 1480-1488. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||