[1] |
Allport, A. , & Wylie, G.( 2000) . Task switching, stimulus-response bindings, and negative priming. In S. Monsell & J. Driver (Eds.), Control of cognitive processes: Attention and performance XVIII (pp. 35-70). Cambridge: MIT Press.
|
[2] |
Allport, D. A., Styles, E. A., & Hsieh, S. ( 1994). Shifting intentional set: exploring the dynamic control of tasks. Attention & Performance, Vol. XV(#5), 421-452.
|
[3] |
Botvinick, M. M., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S., & Cohen, J. D. ( 2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review, 108( 3), 624-652.
doi: 10.1037//0033-295X.108.3.624
URL
pmid: 11488380
|
[4] |
Clayson, P.E., & Larson, M.J . ( 2012). Cognitive performance and electrophysiological indices of cognitive control: A validation study of conflict adaptation. Psychophysiology, 49( 5), 627-637.
|
[5] |
Egner, T.( 2008). Multiple conflict-driven control mechanisms in the human brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12( 10), 374-380.
doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.07.001
URL
|
[6] |
Egner, T., & Hirsch, J.( 2005). The neural correlates and functional integration of cognitive control in a stroop task. Neuroimage, 24( 2), 539-547.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.09.007
URL
pmid: 15627596
|
[7] |
Evans, M. A., Shedden, J. M., Hevenor, S. J., & Hahn, M. C. ( 2000). The effect of variability of unattended information on global and local processing: evidence for lateralization at early stages of processing. Neuropsychologia, 38( 3), 225-239.
|
[8] |
Grundy, J.G., & Shedden, J.M . ( 2014a). A role for recency of response conflict in producing the bivalency effect. Psychological Research, 78( 5), 679-691.
|
[9] |
Grundy, J.G., & Shedden, J.M . ( 2014b). Support for a history-dependent predictive model of dACC activity in producing the bivalency effect: An event-related potential study. Neuropsychologia, 57, 166-178.
|
[10] |
Grundy, J. G., Benarroch, M. F. F., Woodward, T. S., Metzak, P. D., Whitman, J. C., & Shedden, J. M. ( 2013). The Bivalency effect in task switching: Event-related potentials. Human Brain Mapping, 34( 5), 999-1012.
doi: 10.1002/hbm.21488
URL
pmid: 22162123
|
[11] |
Kerns, J. G., Cohen, J. D., MacDonald III, A. W., Cho, R. Y., Stenger, V. A., & Carter, C. S. ( 2004). Anterior cingulate conflict monitoring and adjustments in control. Science, 303( 5560), 1023-1026.
doi: 10.1126/science.1089910
URL
|
[12] |
Larson, M. J., Kaufman, D. A., & Perlstein, W. M. ( 2009). Neural time course of conflict adaptation effects on the Stroop task. Neuropsychologia, 47( 3), 663-670.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.11.013
URL
pmid: 19071142
|
[13] |
Meier, B., Rey-Mermet, A., Woodward, T. S., Müri, R., & Gutbrod, K. ( 2013). Episodic context binding in task switching: Evidence from amnesia. Neuropsychologia, 51( 5), 886-892.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.01.025
URL
pmid: 23395937
|
[14] |
Meier, B., Woodward, T. S., Rey-Mermet, A., & Graf, P. ( 2009). The bivalency effect in task switching: general and enduring. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63( 3), 201-210.
doi: 10.1002/hbm.21488
URL
pmid: 19739903
|
[15] |
Metzak, P. D., Meier, B., Graf, P., & Woodward, T. S. ( 2013). More than a surprise: The bivalency effect in task switching. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25( 7), 833-842.
doi: 10.1080/20445911.2013.832196
URL
|
[16] |
Poulsen, C., Luu, P., Davey, C., & Tucker, D. M. ( 2005). Dynamics of task sets: evidence from dense-array event- related potentials. Cognitive Brain Research, 24( 1), 133-154.
doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.01.008
URL
pmid: 15922166
|
[17] |
Rey-Mermet, A., & Meier, B.( 2012a). The bivalency effect: Evidence for flexible adjustment of cognitive control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38( 1), 213-221.
doi: 10.1037/a0026024
URL
|
[18] |
Rey-Mermet, A., & Meier, B.( 2012b). The bivalency effect: adjustment if cognitive control without response set priming. Psychological Research, 76( 1), 50-59.
|
[19] |
Rey-Mermet, A., & Meier, B.( 2013). An orienting response is not enough: Bivalency not infrequency causes the bivalency effect. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 9( 3), 146-155.
|
[20] |
Rey-Mermet, A., & Meier, B.( 2014). More conflict does not trigger more adjustment of cognitive control for subsequent events: A study of the bivalency effect. Actapsychologica, 145, 111-117.
|
[21] |
Rey-Mermet, A., & Meier, B.( 2016). Post-conflict slowing after incongruent stimuli: from general to conflict-specific. Psychological Research, 81( 3), 611-628.
|
[22] |
Rey-Mermet, A., Koenig, T., & Meier, B. ( 2013). The bivalency effect represents an interference-triggered adjustment of cognitive control: An ERP study. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 13( 3), 575-583.
|
[23] |
Sheth, S. A., Mian, M. K., Patel, S. R., Asaad, W. F., Williams, Z. M., Dougherty, D. D., … Eskandar, E. N. ( 2012). Human dorsal anterior cingulate cortex neurons mediate ongoing behavioral adaptation. Nature, 488( 7410), 218-221.
doi: 10.1038/nature11239
URL
|
[24] |
Spapé, M. M., Band, G. P. H., & Hommel, B. ( 2011). Compatibility-sequence effects in the Simon task reflect episodic retrieval but not conflict adaptation: Evidence from LRP and N2. Biological Psychology, 88( 1), 116-123.
|
[25] |
Woodward, T. S., Meier, B., Tipper, C., & Graf, P. ( 2003). Bivalency is costly: Bivalent stimuli elicit cautious responding. Experimental Psychology, 50( 4), 233-238.
|
[26] |
Woodward, T. S., Metzak, P. D., Meier, B., & Holroyd, C. B. ( 2008). Anterior cingulate cortex signals the requirement to break inertia when switching tasks: A study of the bivalency effect. NeuroImage, 40( 3), 1311-1318.
|
[27] |
Woodward, T. S., Ruff, C. C., & Ngan, E. T. C.( 2006b). Short- and long-term changes in anterior cingulate activation during resolution of task-set competition. Brain Research, 1068( 1), 161-169.
|
[28] |
Wylie, G. R., Javitt, D. C., & Foxe, J. J. ( 2003). Task switching: a high-density electrical mapping study. Neuroimage, 20( 4), 2322-2342.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.08.010
URL
pmid: 14683733
|