心理科学进展 ›› 2018, Vol. 26 ›› Issue (6): 1012-1018.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2018.01012
收稿日期:
2017-10-12
出版日期:
2018-06-10
发布日期:
2018-04-28
通讯作者:
叶一舵
E-mail:yeyiduo@163.com
基金资助:
DENG Jue1, YE Yiduo1(), CHEN Yanfang2
Received:
2017-10-12
Online:
2018-06-10
Published:
2018-04-28
Contact:
YE Yiduo
E-mail:yeyiduo@163.com
摘要:
音乐句法的习得来源于对音乐图式统计规则的长期抽象, 有赖于日常音乐曝光和统计学习能力, 其在构建音乐心理词典的过程中发挥着重要作用并影响着音乐图式性期待的形成。音乐句法可在无意识曝光中被内隐习得, 而外显的专业训练可能有利于音乐统计学习能力的提升。对ERAN、N5、mCPS等音乐句法相关脑电成分进行研究有助于深入了解音乐句法内隐习得的加工机制。在完整音乐结构背景下, 考察各音乐要素习得之间的交互作用, 将使音乐学习实验更具生态效度, 促进研究成果运用于音乐教学实践。
中图分类号:
邓珏, 叶一舵, 陈言放. (2018). 音乐句法的内隐习得及其对图式性期待的影响 *. 心理科学进展 , 26(6), 1012-1018.
DENG Jue, YE Yiduo, CHEN Yanfang. (2018). Implicit acquisition of musical syntax and its influence on schema expectation. Advances in Psychological Science, 26(6), 1012-1018.
[1] | 蒋存梅 . (2016). 音乐心理学. 上海: 华东师范大学出版社. |
[2] |
李菲菲, 宋晨璐, 刘宝根 . ( 2017). 轻中度智力障碍儿童的视觉统计学习. 中国特殊教育, (8), 44-49.
doi: 10.3969/j.issn.1007-3728.2017.08.015 URL |
[3] | 罗小平, 黄虹 . (2008). 音乐心理学 (第2版). 上海: 上海音乐学院出版社. |
[4] | 马谐, 杨玉芳, 张秋月 . ( 2016). 音乐句法的加工. 科学通报, 61( 10), 1099-1111. |
[5] | 马谐, 杨玉芳 . ( 2017). 音乐句法嵌套结构加工的认知神经机制. 中国心理学会音乐心理学专业委员会(筹)成立大会暨首届学术年会, 上海. |
[6] | 欧阳玥, 戴志强 . ( 2010). 音乐节拍认知的研究评述. 心理科学进展, 18( 11), 1692-1699. |
[7] | 张晶晶, 杨玉芳 . ( 2017). 音乐句法加工的影响因素. 心理科学进展, 25( 11), 1823-1830. |
[8] | 张前, 王次炤 . (1992). 音乐美学基础. 北京: 人民音乐出版社. |
[9] |
郑茂平 . ( 2006). 音乐知觉期待的研究述评. 心理科学进展, 14( 6), 866-872.
doi: 10.3969/j.issn.1671-3710.2006.06.011 URL |
[10] | 郑茂平 . ( 2010). 音乐审美期待的心理实质——迈尔期待理论及相关延展研究的心理学解释. 中央音乐学院学报, (3), 126-136. |
[11] |
Batterink L. J., Reber P. J., Neville H. J., & Paller K. A . ( 2015). Implicit and explicit contributions to statistical learning. Journal of Memory & Language, 83, 62-78.
doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2015.04.004 URL pmid: 4448134 |
[12] |
Besson, M., & Faïta, F. (1995). An event-related potential (ERP) study of musical expectancy: Comparison of musicians with nonmusicians. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception & Performance, 21( 6), 1278-1296.
doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.21.6.1278 URL |
[13] |
Bharucha, J. J., & Stoeckig, K. (1987). Priming of chords: Spreading activation or overlapping frequency spectra? Perception & Psychophysics, 41( 6), 519-524.
doi: 10.3758/BF03210486 URL pmid: 3615148 |
[14] |
Bly B. M., Carrión R. E., & Rasch B . ( 2009). Domain- specific learning of grammatical structure in musical and phonological sequences. Memory & Cognition, 37( 1), 10-20.
doi: 10.3758/MC.37.1.10 URL pmid: 19103971 |
[15] |
Brochard R., Abecasis D., Potter D., Ragot R., & Drake C . ( 2003). The "Ticktock" of our internal clock: Direct brain evidence of subjective accents in isochronous sequences. Psychological Science, 14( 4), 362-366.
doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.24441 URL |
[16] |
Dienes, Z., & Longuet-Higgins, C. (2004). Can musical transformations be implicitly learned?. Cognitive Science, 28( 4), 531-558.
doi: 10.1207/s15516709cog2804_2 URL |
[17] |
Egermann H., Pearce M. T., Wiggins G. A., & McAdams S . ( 2013). Probabilistic models of expectation violation predict psychophysiological emotional responses to live concert music. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 13( 3), 533-553.
doi: 10.3758/s13415-013-0161-y URL pmid: 23605956 |
[18] |
Ettlinger M., Margulis E. H.,& Wong, P. C. M. (2011). Implicit memory in music and language. Frontiers in Psychology, 2( 6), 211.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00211 URL pmid: 21927608 |
[19] |
Frost R., Armstrong B. C., Siegelman N., & Christiansen M. H . ( 2015). Domain generality vs. modality specificity: The paradox of statistical learning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19( 3), 117-125.
doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2014.12.010 URL pmid: 25631249 |
[20] |
Grahn, J. A., & Schuit, D. (2012). Individual differences in rhythmic ability: Behavioral and neuroimaging investigations. Psychomusicology, 22( 2), 105-121.
doi: 10.1037/a0031188 URL |
[21] |
Guo, S., & Koelsch, S. (2016). Effects of veridical expectations on syntax processing in music: Event-related potential evidence. Scientific Reports, 6, 19064.
doi: 10.1038/srep19064 URL pmid: 4726113 |
[22] |
Halpern A. R., Zioga I., Shankleman M., Lindsen J., Pearce M. T., & Bhattacharya J . ( 2017). That note sounds wrong! Age-related effects in processing of musical expectation. Brain & Cognition, 113, 1-9.
doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2016.12.006 URL pmid: 28064077 |
[23] |
Hannon, E. E., & Trainor, L. J . ( 2007). Music acquisition: Effects of enculturation and formal training on development. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11( 11), 466-472.
doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.08.008 URL pmid: 17981074 |
[24] | Hodges D. A. (2006). Handbook of music psychology. San Antonio: IMR Press. (Original work published 1996) |
[25] | [ 霍杰斯 , D. A. (2006). 音乐心理学手册 (刘沛, 任恺译). 长沙: 湖南文艺出版社.] |
[26] |
Honing H., Ladinig O., Háden G. P., & Winkler I . ( 2009). Is beat induction innate or learned?. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1169, 93-96.
doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04761.x URL |
[27] |
Ignacio, D. A . ( 2016). The cognitive effects of musical expectations on affective priming. Psychomusicology: Music, Mind, & Brain, 26( 4), 358-365.
doi: 10.1037/pmu0000160 URL |
[28] |
James C. E., Cereghetti D. M., Tribes E. R., & Oechslin M. S . ( 2015). Electrophysiological evidence for a specific neural correlate of musical violation expectation in primary-school children. NeuroImage, 104, 386-397.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.09.047 URL pmid: 25278251 |
[29] |
Jentschke S., Koelsch S., Sallat S., & Friederici A. D . ( 2008). Children with specific language impairment also show impairment of music-syntactic processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20( 11), 1940-1951.
doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20135 URL pmid: 18416683 |
[30] |
Jentschke S., Friederici A. D., & Koelsch S . ( 2014). Neural correlates of music-syntactic processing in two-year old children. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 9, 200-208.
doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2014.04.005 URL pmid: 24907450 |
[31] |
Jonaitis, E. M., & Saffran, J. R . ( 2009). Learning harmony: The role of serial statistics. Cognitive Science, 33( 5), 951-968.
doi: 10.1111/j.1551-6709.2009.01036.x URL pmid: 21585492 |
[32] |
Jongsma M. L. A., Desain P., & Honing H . ( 2004). Rhythmic context influences the auditory evoked potentials of musicians and nonmusicians. Biological Psychology, 66( 2), 129-152.
doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2003.10.002 URL pmid: 15041136 |
[33] |
Knösche T. R., Neuhaus C., Haueisen J., Alter K., Maess B., Witte O. W., & Friederici A. D . ( 2005). Perception of phrase structure in music. Human Brain Mapping, 24( 4), 259-273.
doi: 10.1002/hbm.20088 URL pmid: 15678484 |
[34] |
Koelsch S., Gunter T., Friederici A. D., & Schröger E . ( 2000). Brain indices of music processing: "nonmusicians" are musical. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12( 3), 520-541.
doi: 10.1162/089892900562183 URL |
[35] |
Koelsch S., Gunter T. C., Wittfoth M., & Sammler D . ( 2005). Interaction between syntax processing in language and in music: An ERP study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17( 10), 1565-1577.
doi: 10.1162/089892905774597290 URL pmid: 16269097 |
[36] |
Koelsch, S., & Sammler, D. (2008). Cognitive components of regularity processing in the auditory domain. PLoS One, 3( 7), e2650.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0002650 URL pmid: 2440538 |
[37] |
Kuhn, G., & Dienes, Z. (2006). Differences in the types of musical regularity learnt in incidental- and intentional- learning conditions. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59( 10), 1725-1744.
doi: 10.1080/17470210500438361 URL pmid: 16945857 |
[38] |
Leman, M. (2000). An auditory model of the role of short-term memory in probe-tone ratings. Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 17( 4), 481-509.
doi: 10.2307/40285830 URL |
[39] |
Loui, P. (2012). Learning and liking of melody and harmony: Further studies in artificial grammar learning. Topics in Cognitive Science, 4( 4), 554-567.
doi: 10.1111/j.1756-8765.2012.01208.x URL pmid: 3465524 |
[40] |
Loui, P., & Wessel, D. (2008). Learning and liking an artificial musical system: Effects of set size and repeated exposure. Musicae Scientiae the Journal of the European Society for the Cognitive Sciences of Music, 12( 2), 207-230.
doi: 10.1177/102986490801200202 URL pmid: 20151034 |
[41] |
Loui P., Wessel D. L.,& Hudson Kam, C. L. (2010). Humans rapidly learn grammatical structure in a new musical scale. Music Perception, 27( 5), 377-388.
doi: 10.1525/mp.2010.27.5.377 URL pmid: 2927013 |
[42] |
Loui P., Wu E. H., Wessel D. L., & Knight R. T . ( 2009). A generalized mechanism for perception of pitch patterns. Journal of Neuroscience, 29( 2), 454-459.
doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4503-08.2009 URL pmid: 19144845 |
[43] |
Nan Y., Knösche T. R., & Friederici A. D . ( 2009). Non-musicians' perception of phrase boundaries in music: A cross-cultural ERP study. Biological Psychology, 82( 1), 70-81.
doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2009.06.002 URL pmid: 19540302 |
[44] | Patel A. D. (2012). Music, language, and the brain. New York: Oxford University Press. (Original work published 2008) |
[45] | [ 帕特尔 , A. D. (2012). 音乐、语言与脑 (杨玉芳, 蔡丹超等译). 上海: 华东师范大学出版社.] |
[46] |
Pearce, M. T., & Christensen, J. F . ( 2012). Conference report: The neurosciences and music-IV—learning and memory. Psychomusicology, 22( 1), 70-73.
doi: 10.1037/a0027235 URL |
[47] |
Peretz, I. (1996). Can we lose memory for music? A case of music agnosia in a nonmusician. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 8( 6), 481-496.
doi: 10.1162/jocn.1996.8.6.481 URL pmid: 23961980 |
[48] | Rohrmeier, M. A., & Cross, I. (2010). Narmour’s principles affect implicit learning of melody. In S. M. Demorest (Ed.), Proceedings of the 11th international conference on music perception and cognition (ICMPC 2010)(pp. 1-5). Seattle, WA: University of Washington School of Music. |
[49] |
Rohrmeier, M., & Rebuschat, P. (2012). Implicit learning and acquisition of music. Topics in Cognitive Science, 4( 4), 525-553.
doi: 10.1111/j.1756-8765.2012.01223.x URL pmid: 23060126 |
[50] |
Rohrmeier M., Rebuschat P., & Cross I . ( 2011). Incidental and online learning of melodic structure. Consciousness & Cognition, 20( 2), 214-222.
doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2010.07.004 URL pmid: 20832338 |
[51] |
Schubert E.,& Pearce, M.( 2015). A new look at musical expectancy: The veridical versus the general in the mental organization of music. In International symposium on computer music multidisciplinary research (pp. 358-370). Springer International Publishing.
doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-46282-0_23 URL |
[52] |
Schultz B. G., Stevens C. J., Keller P. E., & Tillmann B . ( 2013). The implicit learning of metrical and nonmetrical temporal patterns. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66( 2), 360-380.
doi: 10.1080/17470218.2012.712146 URL pmid: 22943558 |
[53] |
Seger C. A., Spiering B. J., Sares A. G., Quraini S. I., Alpeter C., David J., & Thaut M. H . ( 2013). Corticostriatal contributions to musical expectancy perception. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 25( 7), 1062-1077.
doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00371 URL pmid: 23410032 |
[54] |
Szpunar K. K., Schellenberg E. G., & Pliner P . ( 2004). Liking and memory for musical stimuli as a function of exposure. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 30( 2), 370-381.
doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.30.2.370 URL pmid: 14979811 |
[55] |
Thompson W. F., Balkwill L. L., & Vernescu R . ( 2000). Expectancies generated by recent exposure to melodic sequences. Memory & Cognition, 28( 4), 547-555.
doi: 10.3758/BF03201245 URL pmid: 10946538 |
[56] |
Tillmann B., Stevens C., & Keller P. E . ( 2011). Learning of timing patterns and the development of temporal expectations. Psychological Research, 75( 3), 243-258.
doi: 10.1007/s00426-010-0302-7 URL pmid: 20683612 |
[57] |
Tillmann B., Gosselin N., Bigand E., & Peretz I . ( 2012). Priming paradigm reveals harmonic structure processing in congenital amusia. Cortex, 48( 8), 1073-1078.
doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.01.001 URL pmid: 22326325 |
[58] | Trainor, L. J., & Corrigall, K. A . ( 2010). Music acquisition and effects of musical experience. In J. M. Riess, R. Fay, & A. Popper (Eds.), Music perception (Vol. 36, pp. 89-127). New York, NY: Springer. |
[59] |
Zajonc, R. B . ( 2001). Mere exposure: A gateway to the subliminal. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 10( 6), 224-228.
doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.00154 URL |
[60] |
Zhang J. J., Jiang C. M., Zhou L. S., & Yang Y. F . ( 2016). Perception of hierarchical boundaries in music and its modulation by expertise. Neuropsychologia, 91, 490-498.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.09.013 URL pmid: 27659874 |
[1] | 徐贵平, 范若琳, 金花. 统计学习的认知神经机制及其与语言的关系[J]. 心理科学进展, 2020, 28(9): 1525-1538. |
[2] | 张晶晶, 梁啸岳, 陈伊笛, 陈庆荣. 音乐句法加工的认知机制与音乐结构的影响模式[J]. 心理科学进展, 2020, 28(6): 883-892. |
[3] | 张晶晶, 杨玉芳. 音乐句法加工的影响因素[J]. 心理科学进展, 2017, 25(11): 1823-1830. |
[4] | 宋新燕;孟祥芝. 婴儿语音感知发展及其机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2012, 20(6): 843-852. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||