心理科学进展 ›› 2024, Vol. 32 ›› Issue (11): 1872-1881.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2024.01872
收稿日期:
2024-02-01
出版日期:
2024-11-15
发布日期:
2024-09-05
通讯作者:
刘昌, E-mail: liuchang@njnu.edu.cn基金资助:
Received:
2024-02-01
Online:
2024-11-15
Published:
2024-09-05
摘要:
审美愉悦的加工流畅性理论认为刺激加工的容易程度会引发积极情绪, 从而促进对刺激的积极评价。在近20年的概念发展和实证研究中, 该模型逐渐面临理论和经验的双重挑战。结合预测加工框架和认知动机模型有望对早期流畅性理论进行补充和完善, 形成多模型整合的审美判断流畅性解释框架。它强调了四个因素在影响审美判断方面的作用:对刺激的期望、对流畅性本身的期望、与特定信念相关的定向目标以及与流畅性和确定性相关的非定向目标。四个因素共同决定了加工流畅性如何介入审美判断, 以及会对审美判断产生哪些具体影响。该框架为更好地解释审美判断过程中矛盾而复杂的流畅性效应提供了理论支持, 并为该领域未来的实证研究指明了方向。
中图分类号:
高程, 刘昌. (2024). 加工流畅性、期望和认知目标如何塑造审美判断?——基于多模型整合的视角. 心理科学进展 , 32(11), 1872-1881.
GAO Cheng, LIU Chang. (2024). How do processing fluency, expectation, and epistemic goals influence aesthetic judgment? A perspective of multi-model integration. Advances in Psychological Science, 32(11), 1872-1881.
[1] | 柴方圆, 喻丰, 彭凯平. (2016). 审美愉悦与加工流畅性. 心理学探新, 36(2), 101-106. |
[2] | 何先友, 陈雅珏, 杨丹妮, 何德娴. (2019). 流畅性对审美鉴赏的影响——从加工流畅性模型到审美愉悦与兴趣模型. 华南师范大学学报(社会科学版), (3), 71-76. |
[3] | 唐国尧, 徐祥运. (2022). 审美知觉研究的新视域: 预测加工理论探析. 科学技术哲学研究, 39(5), 27-32. |
[4] |
Albrecht, S., & Carbon, C. C. (2014). The fluency amplification model: Fluent stimuli show more intense but not evidently more positive evaluations. Acta Psychologica, 148, 195-203.
doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.02.002 pmid: 24603044 |
[5] | Armstrong, T., & Detweiler-Bedell, B. (2008). Beauty as an emotion: The exhilarating prospect of mastering a challenging world. Review of General Psychology, 12(4), 305-329. |
[6] |
Brielmann, A. A., & Dayan, P. (2022). A computational model of aesthetic value. Psychological Review, 129(6), 1319-1337.
doi: 10.1037/rev0000337 pmid: 35786988 |
[7] | Brouillet, D., & Friston, K. (2023). Relative fluency (unfelt vs felt) in active inference. Consciousness and Cognition, 115, 103579-103579. |
[8] |
Bullot, N. J., & Reber, R. (2013). The artful mind meets art history: Toward a psycho-historical framework for the science of art appreciation. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(2), 123-137.
doi: 10.1017/S0140525X12000489 pmid: 23507091 |
[9] |
Carr, E. W., Brady, T. F., & Winkielman, P. (2017). Are you smiling, or have I seen you before? Familiarity makes faces look happier. Psychological Science, 28(8), 1087-1102.
doi: 10.1177/0956797617702003 pmid: 28594281 |
[10] | Carr, E. W., Huber, D. E., Pecher, D., Zeelenberg, R., Halberstadt, J., & Winkielman, P. (2017). The ugliness-in- averageness effect: Tempering the warm glow of familiarity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 112(6), 787-812. |
[11] |
Carr, E. W., Rotteveel, M., & Winkielman, P. (2016). Easy moves: Perceptual fluency facilitates approach-related action. Emotion, 16(4), 540-552.
doi: 10.1037/emo0000146 pmid: 26751628 |
[12] |
Erle, T. M., & Topolinski, S. (2018). Disillusionment: How expectations shape the enjoyment of early perceptual processes. Experimental Psychology, 65(6), 332-344.
doi: 10.1027/1618-3169/a000419 pmid: 30638171 |
[13] | Forster, M. (2022). Processing fluency. In M. Nadal & O. Vartanian (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of empirical aesthetics (pp. 430-446). Oxford University Press. |
[14] | Frascaroli, J., Leder, H., Brattico, E., & Van de Cruys, S. (2024). Aesthetics and predictive processing: Grounds and prospects of a fruitful encounter. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 379(1895), 20220410. |
[15] | Friston, K. (2005). A theory of cortical responses. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 360(1456), 815-836. |
[16] |
Friston, K. (2010). The free-energy principle: A unified brain theory? Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11(2), 127-138.
doi: 10.1038/nrn2787 pmid: 20068583 |
[17] |
Friston, K. J., Lin, M., Frith, C. D., Pezzulo, G., Hobson, J. A., & Ondobaka, S. (2017). Active inference, curiosity and insight. Neural Computation, 29(10), 2633-2683.
doi: 10.1162/neco_a_00999 pmid: 28777724 |
[18] | Furnham, A., & Bunyan, M. (1988). Personality and art preferences. European Journal of Personality, 2(1), 67-74. |
[19] | Gershman, S. J. (2019). What does the free energy principle tell us about the brain? Neurons, Behavior, Data Analysis, and Theory, 2(3), 1-12. |
[20] | Graf, L. K. M., & Landwehr, J. R. (2015). A dual-process perspective on fluency-based aesthetics: The Pleasure- Interest Model of Aesthetic Liking. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 19(4), 395-410. |
[21] |
Graf, L. K. M., & Landwehr, J. R. (2017). Aesthetic pleasure versus aesthetic interest: The two routes to aesthetic liking. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 15.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00015 pmid: 28194119 |
[22] | Halberstadt, J., & Winkielman, P. (2014). Easy on the eyes, or hard to categorize: Classification difficulty decreases the appeal of facial blends. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 50, 175-183. |
[23] | Hansen, J., & Topolinski, S. (2011). An exploratory mindset reduces preference for prototypes and increases preference for novel exemplars. Cognition & Emotion, 25(4), 709-716. |
[24] | Hansen, J., & Wänke, M. (2013). Fluency in context:Discrepancy makes processing experiences informative. In C. Unkelbach & R. Greifender (Eds.), The experience of thinking: How the fluency of mental processes influences cognition and behaviour (pp. 70-84). Psychology Press. |
[25] | Heilbron, M., Armeni, K., Schoffelen, J.-M., Hagoort, P., & de Lange, F. P. (2022). A hierarchy of linguistic predictions during natural language comprehension. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 119(32), e2201968119. |
[26] | Herzog, S. M., & Hertwig, R. (2013). The ecological validity of fluency. In C. Unkelbach & R. Greifender (Eds.), The experience of thinking: How the fluency of mental processes influences cognition and behaviour (pp. 190-219). Psychology Press. |
[27] |
Hesp, C., Smith, R., Parr, T., Allen, M., Friston, K. J., & Ramstead, M. J. D. (2021). Deeply felt affect: The emergence of valence in deep active inference. Neural Computation, 33(2), 398-446.
doi: 10.1162/neco_a_01341 pmid: 33253028 |
[28] | Jacoby, L. L., Kelley, C., M., & Dywan, J. (1989). Memory attributions. In H. L. Roediger & F. I. M. Craik (Eds), Varieties of memory and consciousness: Essays in honour of Endel Tulving (pp. 391-422). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. |
[29] | Jakesch, M., & Carbon, C.-C. (2012). The mere exposure effect in the domain of haptics. PLoS ONE, 7(2), e31215. |
[30] | Jakesch, M., Leder, H., & Forster, M. (2013). Image ambiguity and fluency. PLoS ONE, 8(9), e74084. |
[31] | Jiang, Y., Marcowski, P., Ryazanov, A., & Winkielman, P. (2023). People conform to social norms when gambling with lives or money. Scientific Reports, 13(1), 853. |
[32] | Kaminska, O. K., Magnuski, M., Olszanowski, M., Gola, M., Brzezicka, A., & Winkielman, P. (2020). Ambiguous at the second sight: Mixed facial expressions trigger late electrophysiological responses linked to lower social impressions. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 20(2), 441-454. |
[33] | Kaube, H., Eiserbeck, A., & Rahman, R. A. (2023). Separating art from the artist: The effect of negative affective knowledge on ERPs and aesthetic experience. PLOS ONE, 18(1), e0281082. |
[34] | Kruglanski, A. W. (2023). Uncertain: How to turn your biggest fear into your greatest power. Penguin. |
[35] |
Kruglanski, A. W., Jasko, K., & Friston, K. (2020). All thinking is “wishful” thinking. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 24(6), 413-424.
doi: S1364-6613(20)30079-6 pmid: 32284177 |
[36] | Kruglanski, A. W., Jasko, K., Milyavsky, M., Chernikova, M., Webber, D., Pierro, A., & di Santo, D. (2018). Cognitive consistency theory in social psychology: A paradigm reconsidered. Psychological Inquiry, 29(2), 45-59. |
[37] | Kukkonen, K. (2024). Designs on consciousness: Literature and predictive processing. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 379(1895), 20220423. |
[38] | Leder, H., Belke, B., Oeberst, A., & Augustin, D. (2004). A model of aesthetic appreciation and aesthetic judgments. British Journal of Psychology, 95(4), 489-508. |
[39] | Leder, H., & Nadal, M. (2014). Ten years of a model of aesthetic appreciation and aesthetic judgments: The aesthetic episode-developments and challenges in empirical aesthetics. British Journal of Psychology, 105(4), 443-464. |
[40] | Mantegna, F., Hintz, F., Ostarek, M., Alday, P. M., & Huettig, F. (2019). Distinguishing integration and prediction accounts of ERP N400 modulations in language processing through experimental design. Neuropsychologia, 134, 107199. |
[41] | Mayer, S., & Landwehr, J. R. (2018). Quantifying visual aesthetics based on processing fluency theory: Four algorithmic measures for antecedents of aesthetic preferences. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 12(4), 399-431. |
[42] |
Menninghaus, W., Wagner, V., Wassiliwizky, E., Schindler, I., Hanich, J., Jacobsen, T., & Koelsch, S. (2019). What are aesthetic emotions? Psychological Review, 126(2), 171-195.
doi: 10.1037/rev0000135 pmid: 30802122 |
[43] |
Montoya, R. M., Horton, R. S., Vevea, J. L., Citkowicz, M., & Lauber, E. A. (2017). A re-examination of the mere exposure effect: The influence of repeated exposure on recognition, familiarity, and liking. Psychological Bulletin, 143(5), 459-498.
doi: 10.1037/bul0000085 pmid: 28263645 |
[44] | Muth, C., & Carbon, C.-C. (2024). Predicting instabilities: An embodied perspective on unstable experiences with art and design. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 379(1895), 20220416. |
[45] | Nadal, M., & Chatterjee, A. (2018). Neuroaesthetics and art’s diversity and universality. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 10(3), e1487. |
[46] | Nieuwland, M. S., Barr, D. J., Bartolozzi, F., Busch-Moreno, S., Darley, E., Donaldson, D. I., … Wolfsthurn, S. V. G. Z. (2020). Dissociable effects of prediction and integration during language comprehension: Evidence from a large- scale study using brain potentials. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 375(1791), 20180522. |
[47] |
Nook, E. C., & Zaki, J. (2015). Social norms shift behavioral and neural responses to foods. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 27(7), 1412-1426.
doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00795 pmid: 25671502 |
[48] | Owen, H. E., Halberstadt, J., Carr, E. W., & Winkielman, P. (2016). Johnny Depp, reconsidered: How category-relative processing fluency determines the appeal of gender ambiguity. PLOS ONE, 11(2), e0146328. |
[49] | Pocheptsova, A., Labroo, A. A., & Dhar, R. (2010). Making products feel special: When metacognitive difficulty enhances evaluation. Journal of Marketing Research, 47(6), 1059-1069. |
[50] |
Reber, R., Schwarz, N., & Winkielman, P. (2004). Processing fluency and aesthetic pleasure: Is beauty in the perceiver’s processing experience? Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8(4), 364-382.
doi: 10.1207/s15327957pspr0804_3 pmid: 15582859 |
[51] | Rosenbaum, J. E., & Johnson, B. K. (2016). Who’s afraid of spoilers? Need for cognition, need for affect, and narrative selection and enjoyment. Psychology of Popular Media Culture, 5(3), 273-289. |
[52] | Ryali, C. K., Goffin, S., Winkielman, P., & Yu, A. J. (2020). From likely to likable: The role of statistical typicality in human social assessment of faces. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 117(47), 29371-29380. |
[53] | Schwarz, N., Jalbert, M., Noah, T., & Zhang, L. (2021). Metacognitive experiences as information: Processing fluency in consumer judgment and decision making. Consumer Psychology Review, 4(1), 4-25. |
[54] |
Siev, J. J., & Teeny, J. D. (2024). Personal misconduct elicits harsher professional consequences for artists (vs. scientists): A moral-decoupling process. Psychological Science, 35(1), 82-92.
doi: 10.1177/09567976231214739 pmid: 38150630 |
[55] | Silvia, P. J. (2012). Human emotions and aesthetic experience:An overview of empirical aesthetics. In A. Shimamura & S. E. Palmer (Eds.), Aesthetic science: Connecting minds, brains, and experience (pp. 250-275). Oxford University Press. |
[56] | Unkelbach, C., & Greifender, R. (2013). The experience of thinking: How the fluency of mental processes influences cognition and behaviour. Taylor and Francis. |
[57] | Van de Cruys, S., Bervoets, J., & Moors, A. (2022). Preferences need inferences. In M.Nadal & M.Skov Eds, The Routledge international handbook of neuroaesthetics (pp. 475-506). Routledge. |
[58] | Van de Cruys, S., Frascaroli, J., & Friston, K. (2024). Order and change in art: Towards an active inference account of aesthetic experience. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 379(1895), 20220411. |
[59] |
Van de Cruys, S., & Wagemans, J. (2011). Putting reward in art: A tentative prediction error account of visual art. I-Perception, 2(9), 1035-1062.
doi: 10.1068/i0466aap pmid: 23145260 |
[60] | Vogel, T., Ingendahl, M., & Winkielman, P. (2021). The architecture of prototype preferences: Typicality, fluency, and valence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 150(1), 187-194. |
[61] | Wänke, M., & Hansen, J. (2015). Relative processing fluency. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 24(3), 195-199. |
[62] | Whittlesea, B. W. A. (1993). Illusions of familiarity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19(6), 1235-1253. |
[63] |
Whittlesea, B. W. A., & Williams, L. D. (1998). Why do strangers feel familiar, but friends don’t? A discrepancy- attribution account of feelings of familiarity. Acta Psychologica, 98(2-3), 141-165.
pmid: 9621828 |
[64] | Wiersema, D. V., van der Schalk, J., & van Kleef, G. A. (2012). Who’s afraid of red, yellow, and blue? Need for cognitive closure predicts aesthetic preferences. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 6(2), 168-174. |
[65] | Winkielman, P., Huber, D. E., Kavanagh, L., & Schwarz, N. (2012). Fluency of consistency:When thoughts fit nicely and flow smoothly. In B. Gawronski & F. Strack (Eds.), Cognitive consistency: A fundamental principle in social cognition (pp. 89-111). The Guilford Press. |
[66] |
Winkielman, P., Olszanowski, M., & Gola, M. (2015). Faces in-between: Evaluations reflect the interplay of facial features and task-dependent fluency. Emotion, 15(2), 232-242.
doi: 10.1037/emo0000036 pmid: 25642724 |
[67] | Winkielman, P., Schwarz, N., Fazendeiro, T. A., & Reber, R. (2003). The hedonic marking of processing fluency:Implications for evaluative judgment. In I. Munch & K. C. Klauer (Eds.), The psychology of evaluation: Affective processes in cognition and emotion (pp. 189-217). Lawrence Erlbaum. |
[68] |
Wolf, J. (2020). Predictive coding: Neuroscience and art. Progress in Brain Research, 253, 139-167.
doi: S0079-6123(20)30093-5 pmid: 32771121 |
[69] | Yoo, J., Jasko, K., & Winkielman, P. (2024). Fluency, prediction and motivation: How processing dynamics, expectations and epistemic goals shape aesthetic judgements. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 379(1895), 20230326. |
[70] | Yoo, J., & Winkielman, P. (2023). Constructing aesthetic preferences: The interplay of individual and social influences. The Association for Consumer Research Conference, Seattle, WA, October, 28-30. |
[1] | 叶方, 邱惠林, 蒋柯, 李长瑾. 基于预测加工理论的角色交换解释模型[J]. 心理科学进展, 2023, 31(8): 1504-1516. |
[2] | 李雪玉, 高申春. 预测加工与认知边界:基于现象学的视角[J]. 心理科学进展, 2023, 31(7): 1186-1194. |
[3] | 王钟涵, 王晓田. 中国人群自杀风险的性别比:相关的理论、风险因素、应对策略及社会期望下的压力应对[J]. 心理科学进展, 2023, 31(11): 2155-2170. |
[4] | 曹思琪, 汤晨晨, 伍海燕, 刘勋. 价值计算决定何时与如何努力[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(4): 877-887. |
[5] | 尹奎, 张凯丽, 赵景, 巩振兴. 员工授权期望的效应及其理论机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(2): 353-364. |
[6] | 陈芳艳;李锋盈;李伟健. 知觉线索对元记忆监控的影响[J]. 心理科学进展, 2016, 24(4): 494-500. |
[7] | 谢晓非;胡天翊;林靖;路西. 期望差异:危机中的风险沟通障碍[J]. 心理科学进展, 2013, 21(5): 761-774. |
[8] | 沐守宽;周伟. 缺失数据处理的期望-极大化算法与马尔可夫蒙特卡洛方法[J]. 心理科学进展, 2011, 19(7): 1083-1090. |
[9] | 胥遥山;李永娟. 酒精影响个体社会行为的机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2011, 19(4): 565-572. |
[10] | 孙红月;苏寅;周坤;李纾. 从风险决策中的多次博弈到单次博弈:量变还是质变?[J]. 心理科学进展, 2011, 19(10): 1417-1425. |
[11] | 何金莲;郭少聃;张利燕. 繁殖感的个体差异及其影响因素[J]. 心理科学进展, 2009, 17(5): 996-1001. |
[12] | 孙启武;江光荣. 心理咨询中的期望:概念辨析及理论视角[J]. 心理科学进展, 2008, 16(6): 913-918. |
[13] | 周国梅,荆其诚. 心理学家Daniel Kahneman获2002年诺贝尔经济学奖[J]. 心理科学进展, 2003, 11(1): 1-5. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||