心理科学进展 ›› 2022, Vol. 30 ›› Issue (8): 1894-1904.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2022.01894
• 研究前沿 • 上一篇
收稿日期:
2021-07-19
出版日期:
2022-08-15
发布日期:
2022-06-23
通讯作者:
胡小勇
E-mail:huxiaoyong@swu.edu.cn
基金资助:
DU Tangyan, HU Xiaoyong(), YANG Jing, LI Lanyu, WANG Tiantian
Received:
2021-07-19
Online:
2022-08-15
Published:
2022-06-23
Contact:
HU Xiaoyong
E-mail:huxiaoyong@swu.edu.cn
摘要:
大量证据显示低社会经济地位者常做出“短视行为”, 如在教育上投资较少、有较多的债务和较少的储蓄等, 而这些行为又进一步恶化其不利处境。威胁感知引发心理转变, 使得低社会经济地位者将注意力聚焦在与稀缺相关的事情上, 并具体地、情境化地对事件进行表征, 同时倾向于采取快生命史策略, 进而导致短视决策行为发生。未来研究除了更深入地探讨低社会经济地位者行为决策陷阱的多层次多水平路径机制外, 应更加广泛而深入地开发出符合我国国情的有效干预方案, 使其为实现共同富裕提供科学的心理学路径。
中图分类号:
杜棠艳, 胡小勇, 杨静, 李兰玉, 王甜甜. (2022). 低社会经济地位与跨期决策:威胁视角下的心理转变机制. 心理科学进展 , 30(8), 1894-1904.
DU Tangyan, HU Xiaoyong, YANG Jing, LI Lanyu, WANG Tiantian. (2022). Low Socioeconomic Status and intertemporal choice: The mechanism of “psychological-shift” from the perspective of threat. Advances in Psychological Science, 30(8), 1894-1904.
[1] | 管健, 周一骑. (2016). 生命史的快策略与慢策略: 理解心理与行为差异的新路径. 西北师大学报(社会科学版), (6), 115-121. |
[2] | 李实. (2021). 走向共同富裕, 要做好五件事. 半月谈, (1), 12-14. |
[3] | 王燕, 林镇超, 侯博文, 孙时进. (2017). 生命史权衡的内在机制: 动机控制策略的中介作用. 心理学报, 49(6), 783-793. |
[4] | 王一鸣. (2021). 扩大中等收入群体促进共同富裕. 中国政协, (17), 46-47. |
[5] | 习近平. (2021). 扎实推动共同富裕. 求是, (20), 4-8. |
[6] | 徐小阳, 李洁, 金丽馥. (2020). 普惠金融对农村教育贫困的纾解效应. 中国农村经济, (9), 41-64. |
[7] | 张栋浩, 尹志超. (2018). 金融普惠、风险应对与农村家庭贫困脆弱性. 中国农村经济, (4), 54-73. |
[8] | 郑晓冬, 上官霜月, 陈典, 方向明. (2020). 有条件现金转移支付与农村长期减贫:国际经验与中国实践. 中国农村经济, (9), 124-144. |
[9] |
Amasino D., Sullivan N., Kranton R., & Huettel S. (2019). Amount and time exert independent influences on intertemporal choice. Nature Human Behaviour, 3(4), 383-392.
doi: 10.1038/s41562-019-0537-2 pmid: 30971787 |
[10] |
Belsky J. (2019). Early-life adversity accelerates child and adolescent development. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 28(3), 241-246.
doi: 10.1177/0963721419837670 |
[11] |
Berns G., Laibson D., & Loewenstein G. (2007). Intertemporal choice - Toward an integrative framework. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(11), 482-488.
doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.08.011 URL |
[12] | Bertrand M., Mullainathan S., & Shafir E. (2006). Behavioral economics and marketing in aid of decision making among the poor. Journal of Public Policy & Marketing, 25(1), 8-23. |
[13] |
Bischoff C., & Hansen J. (2016). Influencing support of charitable objectives in the near and distant future: Delay discounting and the moderating influence of construal level. Social Influence, 11(4), 217-229.
doi: 10.1080/15534510.2016.1232204 URL |
[14] |
Blanden J., & Gregg P. (2004). Family income and educational attainment: A review of approaches and evidence for Britain. Oxford Review of Economic Policy, 20(2), 245-263.
doi: 10.1093/oxrep/grh014 URL |
[15] |
Boardman J., & Robert S. (2000). Neighborhood socioeconomic status and perceptions of self-efficacy. Sociological Perspectives, 43(1), 117-136.
doi: 10.2307/1389785 URL |
[16] | Boyce W., & Ellis B. (2005). Biological sensitivity to context: I. An evolutionary-developmental theory of the origins and functions of stress reactivity. Development and Psychopathology, 17(2), 271-301. |
[17] |
Brown J. R., Farrell A. M., & Weisbenner S. J. (2015). Decision-making approaches and the propensity to default: Evidence and implications. Journal of Financial Economics, 121(3), 477-495.
doi: 10.1016/j.jfineco.2016.05.010 URL |
[18] |
Bulley A., & Schacter D. L. (2020). Deliberating trade-offs with the future. Nature Human Behaviour, 4(3), 238-247.
doi: 10.1038/s41562-020-0834-9 pmid: 32184495 |
[19] |
Caner A., & Okten C. (2013). Higher education in turkey: Subsidizing the rich or the poor? Economics of Education Review, 35, 75-92.
doi: 10.1016/j.econedurev.2013.03.007 URL |
[20] |
Cannon C., Goldsmith K., Roux C., & Kirmani A. (2019). A self‐regulatory model of resource scarcity. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 29(1), 104-127.
doi: 10.1002/jcpy.1035 URL |
[21] |
Carvalho L. S., Meier S., & Wang S. W. (2016). Poverty and economic decision-making: Evidence from changes in financial resources at payday. The American Economic Review, 106(2), 260-284.
doi: 10.1257/aer.20140481 URL |
[22] |
Chiou W.-B., Wu W.-H., & Chang M.-H. (2013). Think abstractly, smoke less: A brief construal-level intervention can promote self-control, leading to reduced cigarette consumption among current smokers. Addiction, 108(5), 985-992.
doi: 10.1111/add.12100 URL |
[23] | Coleman J. S., Campbell E. Q., Hobson C. J., McPartland J., Mood A. M., Weinfeld F. D., & York R. L. (1966). Equality of Educational Opportunity. Washington, DC: US Office of Education. |
[24] |
Deck C., & Jahedi S. (2015). The effect of cognitive load on economic decision making: A survey and new experiments. European Economic Review, 78, 97-119.
doi: 10.1016/j.euroecorev.2015.05.004 URL |
[25] |
Epper T., Fehr E., Fehr-Duda H., Kreiner C. T., Lassen D. D., Leth-Petersen S., & Rasmussen G. N. (2020). Time discounting and wealth inequality. American Economic Review, 110(4), 1177-1205.
doi: 10.1257/aer.20181096 URL |
[26] |
Farah M., & Hook C. J. (2017). Trust and the poverty trap. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114(21), 5327-5329.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1704798114 URL |
[27] |
Figueredo A. J., Vásquez G., Brumbach B. H., & Schneider S. M. R. (2004). The heritability of life history strategy: The k-factor, covitality, and personality. Social Biology, 51(3-4), 121-143.
pmid: 17019827 |
[28] |
Frankenhuis W., & Nettle D. (2020). The strengths of people in poverty. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 29(1), 16-21.
doi: 10.1177/0963721419881154 |
[29] | Frankenhuis W., Nettle D., & Dall S. R. X. (2019). A case for environmental statistics of early-life effects. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 374(1770), 20180110. |
[30] |
Frederick S., Loewenstein G., & O'donoghue T. (2002). Time discounting and time preference: A critical review. Journal of Economic Literature, 40(2), 351-401.
doi: 10.1257/jel.40.2.351 URL |
[31] |
Fritsche I., & Jugert P. (2017). The consequences of economic threat for motivated social cognition and action. Current Opinion in Psychology, 18, 31-36.
doi: S2352-250X(17)30050-7 pmid: 29221509 |
[32] |
Fujita K., & Han H. A. (2009). Moving beyond deliberative control of impulses: The effect of construal levels on evaluative associations in self-control conflicts. Psychological Science, 20(7), 799-804.
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02372.x URL |
[33] |
Fujita K., Trope Y., Liberman N., & Levin-Sagi M. (2006). Construal levels and self-control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90(3), 351-367.
doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.90.3.351 URL |
[34] |
Goldsmith K., Griskevicius V., & Hamilton R. (2020). Scarcity and consumer decision making: Is scarcity a mindset, a threat, a reference point, or a journey? Journal of the Association for Consumer Research, 5(4), 358-364.
doi: 10.1086/710531 URL |
[35] | Golsteyn B. H. H., Grönqvist H., & Lindahl L. (2014). Adolescent time preferences predict lifetime outcomes. The Economic Journal (London), 124(580), F739-F761. |
[36] |
Griskevicius V., Ackerman J., Cantú S. M., Delton A. W., Robertson T. E., Simpson J., Thompson M. E., & Tybur J. M. (2013). When the economy falters, do people spend or save? Responses to resource scarcity depend on childhood environments. Psychological Science, 24(2), 197-205.
doi: 10.1177/0956797612451471 pmid: 23302295 |
[37] | Griskevicius V., Delton A. W., Robertson T. E., & Tybur J. M. (2011). Environmental contingency in life history strategies: the influence of mortality and socioeconomic status on reproductive timing. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 100(2), 241-254. |
[38] |
Hamilton R., Thompson D., Bone S., Chaplin L., Griskevicius V., Goldsmith K., … Zhu M. (2019). The effects of scarcity on consumer decision journeys. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 47(3), 532-550.
doi: 10.1007/s11747-018-0604-7 URL |
[39] |
Handa S., Seidenfeld D., & Tembo G. (2020). The impact of a large-scale poverty-targeted cash transfer program on intertemporal choice. Economic Development and Cultural Change, 69(1), 485-512.
doi: 10.1086/702997 URL |
[40] |
Haushofer J., & Fehr E. (2014). On the psychology of poverty. Science, 344(6186), 862-867.
doi: 10.1126/science.1232491 pmid: 24855262 |
[41] | Haushofer J., & Fehr E. (2019). Negative income shocks increase discount rates. University of Zurich Working Paper. |
[42] |
Heckman S. J., & Montalto C. P. (2018). Consumer risk preferences and higher education enrollment decisions. Journal of Consumer Affairs, 52(1), 166-196.
doi: 10.1111/joca.12139 URL |
[43] |
Hout M. (2012). Social and economic returns to college education in the united states. Annual Review of Sociology, 38(1), 379-400.
doi: 10.1146/annurev.soc.012809.102503 URL |
[44] |
Jahnel T., Ferguson S. G., Shiffman S., Thrul J., & Schüz B. (2018). Momentary smoking context as a mediator of the relationship between SES and smoking. Addictive Behaviors, 83, 136-141.
doi: S0306-4603(17)30467-7 pmid: 29273313 |
[45] |
Jimura K., Chushak M., Westbrook A., & Braver T. (2018). Intertemporal decision-making involves prefrontal control mechanisms associated with working memory. Cerebral Cortex, 28(4), 1105-1116.
doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhx015 URL |
[46] |
Kim K. T., Wilmarth M. J., & Henager R. (2017). Poverty levels and debt indicators among low-income households before and after the great recession. Journal of Financial Counseling and Planning, 28(2), 196-212.
doi: 10.1891/1052-3073.28.2.196 URL |
[47] |
Kraay A., & McKenzie D. (2014). Do poverty traps exist? Assessing the evidence. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 28(3), 127-148.
doi: 10.1257/jep.28.3.127 URL |
[48] |
Lea S., & Webley P. (2006). Money as tool, money as drug: The biological psychology of a strong incentive. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 29(2), 161-209.
doi: 10.1017/S0140525X06009046 URL |
[49] | Lewis O. (1966/2010). The culture of poverty. In G. Gmelch & W. Zenner (eds.), Urban life: Readings in the anthropology of the city (fifth edition) (pp. 175-184). Long Grove, IL: Waveland Press. |
[50] |
Luck S., & Vogel E. (1997). The capacity of visual working memory for features and conjunctions. Nature, 390(6657), 279-281.
doi: 10.1038/36846 URL |
[51] |
Malkoc S. A., Zauberman G., & Bettman J. R. (2010). Unstuck from the concrete: Carryover effects of abstract mindsets in intertemporal preferences. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 113(2), 112-126.
doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2010.07.003 URL |
[52] |
Mani A., Mullainathan S., Shafir E., & Zhao J. (2013). Poverty impedes cognitive function. Science, 341(6149), 976-980.
doi: 10.1126/science.1238041 URL |
[53] |
Mehryar A. H. (1984). The role of psychology in national development: Wishful thinking and reality. International Journal of Psychology, 19(1-4), 159-167.
doi: 10.1080/00207598408247523 URL |
[54] |
Meier S., & Sprenger C. (2010). Present-biased preferences and credit card borrowing. American Economic Journal: Applied Economics, 2(1), 193-210.
doi: 10.1257/app.2.1.193 URL |
[55] |
Mittal C., & Griskevicius V. (2016). Silver spoons and platinum plans: How childhood environment affects adult health care decisions. Journal of Consumer Research, 43(4), 636-656.
doi: 10.1093/jcr/ucw046 URL |
[56] |
Mittal C., Griskevicius V., & Haws K. L. (2020). From cradle to grave: How childhood and current environments impact consumers’ subjective life expectancy and decision-making. Journal of Consumer Research, 47(3), 350-372.
doi: 10.1093/jcr/ucaa003 URL |
[57] |
Moav O., & Neeman Z. (2012). Saving rates and poverty: The role of conspicuous consumption and human capital. The Economic Journal, 122(563), 933-956.
doi: 10.1111/j.1468-0297.2012.02516.x URL |
[58] |
Moeini-Jazani M., Albalooshi S., & Müller Seljeseth I. (2019). Self-affirmation reduces delay discounting of the financially deprived. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 1729.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01729 pmid: 31417458 |
[59] | Mullainathan S., & Shafir E. (2013). Scarcity: Why having too little means so much. New York: Times Books. |
[60] |
Nettle D. (2010). Dying young and living fast: Variation in life history across English neighborhoods. Behavioral Ecology, 21(2), 387-395.
doi: 10.1093/beheco/arp202 URL |
[61] |
Norris A. H., Rao N., Huber-Krum S., Garver S., Chemey E., & Turner A. N. (2019). Scarcity mindset in reproductive health decision making: a qualitative study from rural Malawi. Culture, Health & Sexuality, 21(12), 1333-1348.
doi: 10.1080/13691058.2018.1562092 URL |
[62] | Ong Q., Theseira W., & Ng I. Y. (2019). Reducing debt improves psychological functioning and changes decision- making in the poor. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 116(15), 7244-7249. |
[63] |
Pepper G., & Nettle D. (2017). The behavioural constellation of deprivation: Causes and consequences. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 40, e314.
doi: 10.1017/S0140525X1600234X URL |
[64] |
Reimers S., Maylor E., Stewart N., & Chater N. (2009). Associations between a one-shot delay discounting measure and age, income, education and real-world impulsive behavior. Personality and Individual Differences, 47(8), 973-978.
doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2009.07.026 URL |
[65] |
Rickard I. J., Frankenhuis W. E., & Nettle D. (2014). Why are childhood family factors associated with timing of maturation? A role for internal prediction. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 9(1), 3-15.
doi: 10.1177/1745691613513467 pmid: 26173236 |
[66] |
Scholten M., & Read D. (2010). The psychology of intertemporal tradeoffs. Psychological Review, 117(3), 925-944.
doi: 10.1037/a0019619 pmid: 20658858 |
[67] |
Shah A. K., Mullainathan S., & Shafir E. (2012). Some consequences of having too little. Science, 338(6107), 682-685.
doi: 10.1126/science.1222426 URL |
[68] |
Shah A. K., Zhao J., Mullainathan S., & Shafir E. (2018). Money in the mental lives of the poor. Social Cognition, 36(1), 4-19.
doi: 10.1521/soco.2018.36.1.4 URL |
[69] | Sheehy-Skeffington J. (2018). Decision-Making up against the wall:A framework for understanding the behavioral dimension of low socio-economic status. In A. K. Üskül & S. Oishi (Eds.), Socio-economic environment and human psychology: Social, ecological, and cultural perspectives (pp. 105-138). New York: Oxford University Press. |
[70] | Sheehy-Skeffington J. (2019). Inequality from the bottom up: Toward a “psychological shift” model of decision-making under socioeconomic threat. (pp.213-231). Springer International Publishing. |
[71] |
Sheehy-Skeffington J. (2020). The effects of low socioeconomic status on decision-making processes. Current Opinion in Psychology, 33, 183-188.
doi: S2352-250X(19)30123-X pmid: 31494518 |
[72] | Sheehy-Skeffington J., & Haushofer J. (2014). The behavioural economics of poverty. In Barriers and opportunities at the base of the pyramid: The role of the private sector in inclusive development (pp. 96-112). Istanbul, Turkey: UNDP Istanbul International Center for Private Sector in Development. |
[73] |
Small M. L., Harding D. J., & Lamont M. (2010). Introduction: Reconsidering culture and poverty. The ANNALS of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 629(1), 6-27.
doi: 10.1177/0002716210362077 URL |
[74] |
Taylor A. (2010). Troubling childhood innocence: Reframing the debate over the media sexualisation of children. Australasian Journal of Early Childhood, 35(1), 48-57.
doi: 10.1177/183693911003500108 URL |
[75] | Tomm B. M., & Zhao J. (2016). Scarcity captures attention and induces neglect:Eye-tracking and behavioral evidence. In A. Papafragou, D. Grodner, D. Mirman & J. C. Trueswell (Eds.), Proceedings of the 38th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 1199-1204). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society. |
[76] |
Trope Y., & Liberman N. (2003). Temporal construal. Psychological Review, 110(3), 403-421.
doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.110.3.403 URL |
[77] |
Trope Y., & Liberman N. (2010). Construal-level theory of psychological distance. Psychological Review, 117(2), 440-463.
doi: 10.1037/a0018963 URL |
[78] |
Vohs K. D. (2013). The poor's poor mental power. Science, 341(6149), 969-970.
doi: 10.1126/science.1244172 URL |
[79] |
Wasylyshyn N., Hemenway Falk B., Garcia J. O., Cascio C. N., O’Donnell M. B., Bingham C. R.,... Falk E. B. (2018). Global brain dynamics during social exclusion predict subsequent behavioral conformity. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 13(2), 182-191.
doi: 10.1093/scan/nsy007 pmid: 29529310 |
[80] | Ximenes V. M., Cidade E. C., & Silva G. R. G. (2019). Psychology research in the poverty context: What for and for whom? (pp.3-16). Springer International Publishing. |
[81] |
Zaleskiewicz T., Bernady A., & Traczyk J. (2020). Entrepreneurial risk taking is related to mental imagery: A fresh look at the old issue of entrepreneurship and risk. Applied Psychology, 69(4), 1438-1469.
doi: 10.1111/apps.12226 |
[82] | Zhao J., & Tomm B. M. (2018). Psychological responses to scarcity. In Oxford research encyclopedia of psychology (pp. 1-21). New York: Oxford University Press. |
[1] | 毕翠华, 齐怀远. 时间感知在跨期决策中的作用——时间决策模型的新探索[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(5): 1106-1118. |
[2] | 张姝玥, 黄骏青, 赵峰, 徐科朋. 社会排斥影响跨期决策的心理机制探讨[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(3): 486-498. |
[3] | 刘萍, 张荣伟, 李丹. 自我超越价值观对持久幸福感的作用及机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(3): 660-669. |
[4] | 董天天, 王婷, 张和云, 贺雯. 元刻板印象的效价与效应相回馈吗?[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(3): 693-702. |
[5] | 孙海龙, 安薪如, 李爱梅, 赖慧燕, 李泽虹. 动机冲突影响混合跨期决策:趋近-回避动机理论视角[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(12): 2628-2638. |
[6] | 马冰, 杨蓉, 杜旌, 马贵梅. 居危思变?工作不安全感对创新行为的差异化影响?[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(11): 2381-2394. |
[7] | 胡小勇, 杜棠艳, 李兰玉, 王甜甜. 低社会经济地位影响自我调节的神经机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(10): 2278-2290. |
[8] | 李爱梅, 车敬上, 刘楠, 孙海龙, 周玮. 海量信息如何影响跨期决策?基于注意资源的理论视角[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(9): 1521-1533. |
[9] | 程诚, 郭培杨, 杨丽, 王梦雅. 基于三元精神病态模型的精神病态认知-情感加工框架[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(9): 1628-1646. |
[10] | 孙国晓, 张力为. 竞赛压力、注意控制与运动表现关系的理论演进[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(6): 1122-1130. |
[11] | 尹俊婷, 王冠, 罗俊龙. 威胁对创造力的影响:认知与情绪双加工路径[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(5): 815-826. |
[12] | 孙洪杰, 刘菲菲, 冯文婷, 崔丙群. 个体如何以消费行为应对自我威胁?基于导向-路径整合模型的分析[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(5): 921-935. |
[13] | 王祯. 儿童的性别刻板印象威胁及其干预[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(2): 276-285. |
[14] | 杨玲, 刘文鑫, 张炀, 张建勋, 牛禄霖. 物质成瘾领域延迟折扣研究中的外部效度问题[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(1): 140-149. |
[15] | 王煜, 李梦菊. 建设性还是破坏性?羞愧的双路径模型[J]. 心理科学进展, 2020, 28(8): 1325-1336. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||