心理科学进展 ›› 2022, Vol. 30 ›› Issue (11): 2414-2423.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2022.02414
收稿日期:
2022-02-06
出版日期:
2022-11-15
发布日期:
2022-09-07
通讯作者:
黄建平
E-mail:jphuang@suda.edu.cn
基金资助:
HUANG Jianping1(), XU Jingxian1, WAN Xiaoang2
Received:
2022-02-06
Online:
2022-11-15
Published:
2022-09-07
Contact:
HUANG Jianping
E-mail:jphuang@suda.edu.cn
摘要:
消费者能够基于长期形成的联结产生预期并引导产品搜索。然而, 在实际搜索过程中, 消费者可能会遭遇符合预期的经验或违反预期的经验。本研究主要探讨基于不同产品搜索经验的联想学习影响消费行为的机制, 主要涉及(1)消费者是否会基于产品搜索经验的联想学习产生“违反预期”的预期, 并且影响随后搜索中的注意加工; (2)违反预期和符合预期的产品搜索经验对任务无关信息的联想学习所产生的影响, 同时关注先前短期的联想学习能否建立预期从而引导随后的产品注意搜索过程; (3)基于产品搜索经验的联想学习对产品偏好的影响, 并将行为偏好和大脑奖赏进行联系。本研究有助于阐明消费情境中产品搜索对消费者行为和大脑的影响机制, 为营销应用中通过改变消费体验、促进产品购买提供实证依据。
中图分类号:
黄建平, 许婧娴, 宛小昂. (2022). 联想学习对消费行为的影响:基于产品搜索经验的视角. 心理科学进展 , 30(11), 2414-2423.
HUANG Jianping, XU Jingxian, WAN Xiaoang. (2022). Influence of associative learning on consumer behavior: From the perspective of product search experience. Advances in Psychological Science, 30(11), 2414-2423.
[1] | 陈彩燕, 王成功, 魏知超, 徐钟庚. (2014). 期望、体验和即时评价对消费者学习的影响. 中国健康心理学杂志, 22(12), 1801-1804. |
[2] | 吴新辉, 袁登华. (2009). 消费者品牌联想的建立与测量. 心理科学进展, 17(2), 451-459. |
[3] | 徐菁, 蒋多. (2009). 期望、体验和回忆:当消费者不能从体验中学习. 心理学报, 41(8), 745-752. |
[4] | 臧学莲, 张笑笑, 贾丽娜, 李根强, 李红. (2017). 选择性注意机制在情景线索效应中的作用. 心理科学进展, 25(9), 1503-1511. |
[5] |
Berns, G. S., Capra, C. M., Moore, S., & Noussair, C. (2010). Neural mechanisms of the influence of popularity on adolescent ratings of music. NeuroImage, 49(3), 2687-2696. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.070
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.070 URL pmid: 19879365 |
[6] |
Berns, G. S., & Moore, S. E. (2012). A neural predictor of cultural popularity. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 22(1), 154-160. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcps.2011.05.001
doi: 10.1016/j.jcps.2011.05.001 URL |
[7] |
Best, M., Lawrence, N. S., Logan, G. D., McLaren, I. P. L., & Verbruggen, F. (2016). Should I stop or should I go? The role of associations and expectancies. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 42(1), 115-137. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000116
doi: 10.1037/xhp0000116 URL |
[8] | Brakus, J. J., Schmitt, B. H., & Zarantonello, L. (2009). Brand experience: What is it? How is it measured? Does it affect loyalty?. Journal of Marketing, 73(3), 52-68. https://doi.org/10.1509/jmkg.73.3.052 |
[9] |
Chang, Y., & Ko, Y. J. (2018). The effects of association strength on attention and product evaluation. European Journal of Marketing, 52(5), 1257-1279. https://doi.org/10.1108/EJM-05-2016-0261
doi: 10.1108/EJM-05-2016-0261 URL |
[10] |
Chen, Z., Veling, H., Dijksterhuis, A., & Holland, R. W. (2016). How does not responding to appetitive stimuli cause devaluation: Evaluative conditioning or response inhibition? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145(12), 1687-1701. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000236
doi: 10.1037/xge0000236 URL |
[11] |
Chun, M. M., & Jiang, Y. (1998). Contextual cueing: Implicit learning and memory of visual context guides spatial attention. Cognitive Psychology, 36(1), 28-71. https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1998.0681
URL pmid: 9679076 |
[12] | Chun M. & Wolfe J. (2001). Visual Attention. In E. Bruce Goldstein (Eds.),Blackwell Handbook of Perception (pp. 272-310). MA: Blackwell Publishers. |
[13] |
Coppin, G., Delplanque, S., Bernard, C., Cekic, S., Porcherot, C., Cayeux, I., & Sander, D. (2014). Choice both affects and reflects preferences. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(7), 1415-1427. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2013.863953
doi: 10.1080/17470218.2013.863953 URL |
[14] |
Criaud, M., & Boulinguez, P. (2013). Have we been asking the right questions when assessing response inhibition in go/no-go tasks with fMRI? A meta-analysis and critical review. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 37(1), 11-23. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.11.003
doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.11.003 URL |
[15] |
Dampuré, J., Ros, C., Rouet, J.-F., & Vibert, N. (2012). How word familiarity facilitates visual search for verbal material. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 26(2), 271-288. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1821.
doi: 10.1002/acp.1821 URL |
[16] |
D’Astolfo, L., & Rief, W. (2017). Learning about expectation violation from prediction error paradigms-A meta-analysis on brain processes following a prediction error. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1253. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01253
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01253 URL |
[17] |
Davis, A. M., Gaur, V., & Kim, D. (2020). Consumer learning from own experience and social information: An experimental study. Management Science. 67(5), 2657-3320. https://doi.org/10.1287/mnsc.2020.3691
doi: 10.1287/mnsc.2020.3582 URL |
[18] | Dawson, S., Bloch, P. H., & Ridgway, N. (1990). Shopping motives, emotional states, and retail outcomes. Journal of Retailing, 66(4), 408-427. |
[19] |
de Houwer, J., & Beckers, T. (2002). Higher-order retrospective revaluation in human causal learning. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section B, 55(2b), 137-151. https://doi.org/10.1080/02724990143000216
doi: 10.1080/02724990143000216 URL |
[20] |
de Oca, B. M., & Black, A. A. (2013). Bullets versus burgers: Is it threat or relevance that captures attention? American Journal of Psychology, 126(3), 287-300. https://doi.org/10.5406/amerjpsyc.126.3.0287
URL pmid: 24027943 |
[21] |
Deppe, M., Schwindt, W., Kugel, H., Plassmann, H., & Kenning, P. (2005). Nonlinear responses within the medial prefrontal cortex reveal when specific implicit information influences economic decision making. Journal of Neuroimaging, 15(2), 171-182. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1552-6569.2005.tb00303.x
URL pmid: 15746230 |
[22] |
de Vito, D., Al-Aidroos, N., & Fenske, M. J. (2017). Neural evidence that inhibition is linked to the affective devaluation of distractors that match the contents of working memory. Neuropsychologia, 99, 259-269. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.03.022
doi: S0028-3932(17)30102-1 URL pmid: 28341131 |
[23] | Do Carmo Blanco, N., & Allen, J. J. B. (2018). Guidance of spatial attention during associative learning: Contributions of predictability and intention to learn. Psychophysiology, 55(8), e13077. https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.13077. |
[24] |
Fenske, M. J., & Raymond, J. E. (2006). Affective influences of selective attention. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15(6), 312-316. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2006.00459.x
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2006.00459.x URL |
[25] |
Florack, A., Egger, M., & Hübner, R. (2020). When products compete for consumers attention: How selective attention affects preferences. Journal of Business Research, 111, 117-127. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2019.05.009
doi: 10.1016/j.jbusres.2019.05.009 URL |
[26] |
Garrison, J., Erdeniz, B., & Done, J. (2013). Prediction error in reinforcement learning: A meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 37(7), 1297-1310. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.03.023.
doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.03.023 URL |
[27] |
Gollwitzer, M., Thorwart, A., & Meissner, K. (2018). Psychological responses to violations of expectations. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 2357. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02357
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02357 URL |
[28] |
Gozli, D. G., Moskowitz, J. B., & Pratt, J. (2014). Visual attention to features by associative learning. Cognition, 133(2), 488-501.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.07.014 URL pmid: 25173722 |
[29] |
Guevara Pinto, J. D., & Papesh, M. H. (2019). Incidental memory following rapid object processing: The role of attention allocation strategies. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 45(9), 1174-1190. https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000664
doi: 10.1037/xhp0000664 URL |
[30] |
Gwinn, R., Leber, A. B., & Krajbich, I. (2019). The spillover effects of attentional learning on value-based choice. Cognition, 182, 294-306. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2018.10.012
doi: S0010-0277(18)30270-1 URL pmid: 30391643 |
[31] |
Higgins, M. J., & Hayes, J. E. (2019). Learned color taste associations in a repeated brief exposure paradigm. Food Quality and Preference, 71, 354-365. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.foodqual.2018.08.013
doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2018.08.013 URL |
[32] |
Hoch, S. J., & Deighton, J. (1989). Managing what consumers learn from experience. Journal of Marketing, 53(2), 1-20. https://doi.org/10.1177/002224298905300201
doi: 10.1177/002224298905300301 URL |
[33] |
Hoegg, J., & Alba, J. W. (2007). Taste perception: More than meets the tongue. Journal of Consumer Research, 33(4), 490-498.https://doi.org/10.1086/510222
doi: 10.1086/510222 URL |
[34] |
Hout, M. C., & Goldinger, S. D. (2010). Learning in repeated visual search. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72(5), 1267-1282.https://doi.org/10.3758/APP.72.5.1267
doi: 10.3758/APP.72.5.1267 URL |
[35] |
Hout, M. C., & Goldinger, S. D. (2012). Incidental learning speeds visual search by lowering response thresholds, not by improving efficiency: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38(1), 90-112.https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023894
doi: 10.1037/a0023894 URL |
[36] |
Huang, J., Peng, Y., & Wan, X. (2021a). The color-flavor incongruency effect in visual search for food labels: An eye-tracking study. Food Quality and Preference, 88, 104078. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.foodqual.2020.104078
doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2020.104078 URL |
[37] |
Huang, J., & Wan, X. (2019). The color-flavor incongruency effect in product evaluation and brand perception. Journal of Consumer Behavior, 18(6), 484-495. https://doi.org/10.1002/cb.1787
doi: 10.1002/cb.1787 URL |
[38] |
Huang, J., Wang, F., Sui, J., & Wan, X. (2019). Functional and structural basis of the color-flavor incongruency effect in visual search. Neuropsychologia, 127, 66-74. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2019.02.013
doi: S0028-3932(18)30691-2 URL pmid: 30797830 |
[39] |
Huang, J., Zhao, P., & Wan, X. (2021b). From brain variations to individual differences in the color-flavor incongruency effect: A combined VR and resting-state fMRI study. Journal of Business Research, 123, 604-612.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2020.10.031
doi: 10.1016/j.jbusres.2020.10.031 URL |
[40] |
Izuma, K., Matsumoto, M., Murayama, K., Samejima, K., Sadato, N., & Matsumoto, K. (2010). Neural correlates of cognitive dissonance and choice-induced preference change. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(51), 22014-22019. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1011879108
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1011879108 URL |
[41] |
Janiszewski, C., Kuo, A., & Tavassoli, N. T. (2013). The influence of selective attention and inattention to products on subsequent choice. Journal of Consumer Research, 39(6), 1258-1274. https://doi.org/10.1086/668234
doi: 10.1086/668234 URL |
[42] |
Kadel, H., Feldmann-Westefeld, T., & Schuh, A. (2016). Associative learning undermines top-down control of visual attention. Journal of Vision, 16(12), 1032-1032. https://doi.org/10.1167/16.12.1032
doi: 10.1167/16.12.1032 URL |
[43] |
Karmarkar, U. R., & Plassmann, H. (2019). Consumer Neuroscience: Past, Present, and Future. Organizational Research Methods, 22(1), 174-195. https://doi.org/10.1177/1094428117730598
doi: 10.1177/1094428117730598 URL |
[44] |
Kiss, M., Goolsby, B. A., Raymond, J. E., Shapiro, K. L., Silvert, L., Nobre, A. C., ... Eimer, M. (2007). Efficient attentional selection predicts distractor devaluation: Event-related potential evidence for a direct link between attention and emotion. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(8), 1316-1322. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2007.19.8.1316
doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.8.1316 URL pmid: 17651005 |
[45] |
Koster, R., Duzel, E., & Dolan, R. J. (2015). Action and valence modulate choice and choice-induced preference change. PLoS One, 10(3), e0119682.https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0119682
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0119682 URL |
[46] |
Lamy, D., Yashar, A., & Ruderman, L. (2010). A dual-stage account of inter-trial priming effects. Vision Research, 50(14), 1396-1401.
doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2010.01.008 pmid: 20079758 |
[47] |
Le Pelley, M. E., Mitchell, C. J., Beesley, T., George, D. N., & Wills, A. J. (2016). Attention and associative learning in humans: An integrative review. Psychological Bulletin, 142(10), 1111-1140. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000064
doi: 10.1037/bul0000064 URL pmid: 27504933 |
[48] |
Lee, L., Frederick, S., & Ariely, D. (2006). Try it, you’ll like it: The influence of expectation, consumption, and revelation on preferences for beer. Psychological Science, 17(12), 1054-1058. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01829.x
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01829.x URL |
[49] |
Levy, I., Lazzaro, S. C., Rutledge, R. B., & Glimcher, P. W. (2011). Choice from non-choice: Predicting consumer preferences from blood oxygenation level-dependent signals obtained during passive viewing. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(1), 118-125. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3214-10.2011
doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3214-10.2011 URL pmid: 21209196 |
[50] |
Lin, S., Zhang, J., & Hauser, J. R. (2015). Learning from experience, simply. Marketing Science, 34(1), 1-19. https://doi.org/10.1287/mksc.2014.0868
doi: 10.1287/mksc.2014.0868 URL |
[51] |
Lindström, R., Paavilainen, P., Kujala, T., & Tervaniemi, M. (2012). Processing of audiovisual associations in the human brain: Dependency on expectations and rule complexity. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 159. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00159
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00159 URL pmid: 22654778 |
[52] | Luck, S. J. (2005). Ten simple rules for designing and interpreting ERP experiments. Event-related potentials: A methods handbook, 4. |
[53] |
Ma, Q., Wang, K., Wang, X., Wang, C., & Wang, L. (2010). The influence of negative emotion on brand extension as reflected by the change of N2: A preliminary study. Neuroscience Letters, 485(3), 237-240. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2010.09.020
doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2010.09.020 URL pmid: 20849918 |
[54] |
Maljkovic, V., & Nakayama, K. (1994). Priming of pop-out: I. Role of features. Memory & Cognition, 22(6), 657-672. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03209251
doi: 10.3758/BF03209251 URL |
[55] | Mandler, G. (1982). The structure of value:Accounting for taste. In M. S. Clark & S. T. Fiske (Eds.),Affect and cognition: The 17th annual Carnegie symposium (pp. 3-36). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. |
[56] |
Martiny-Huenger, T., Gollwitzer, P. M., & Oettingen, G. (2014). Distractor devaluation in a flanker task: Object- specific effects without distractor recognition memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40(2), 613-625.https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034130
doi: 10.1037/a0034130 URL |
[57] |
Mathwick, C., & Rigdon, E. (2004). Play, flow, and the online search experience. Journal of Consumer Research, 31(2), 324-332.https://doi.org/10.1086/422111
doi: 10.1086/422111 URL |
[58] |
McClure, S. M., Li, J., Tomlin, D., Cypert, K. S., Montague, L. M., & Montague, P. R. (2004). Neural correlates of behavioral preference for culturally familiar drinks. Neuron, 44(2), 379-387. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2004.09.019
URL pmid: 15473974 |
[59] | Nummenmaa, L., Hietanen, J. K., Calvo, M. G., & Hyönä, J. (2011). Food catches the eye but not for everyone: A BMI-contingent attentional bias in rapid detection of nutriments. PLoS One, 6(5), e19215. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0019215 |
[60] |
Piqueras-Fiszman, B., & Spence, C. (2011). Crossmodal correspondences in product packaging. Assessing color- flavor correspondences for potato chips (crisps). Appetite, 57(3), 753-757. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2011.07.012
doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2011.07.012 URL pmid: 21824502 |
[61] |
Plassmann, H., & Weber, B. (2015). Individual differences in marketing placebo effects: Evidence from brain imaging and behavioral experiments. Journal of Marketing Research, 52(4), 493-510. https://doi.org/10.1509/jmr.13.0613
doi: 10.1509/jmr.13.0613 URL |
[62] |
Quandt, J., Holland, R. W., Chen, Z., & Veling, H. (2019). The role of attention in explaining the no-go devaluation effect: Effects on appetitive food items. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 45(8), 1119-1133. https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000659
doi: 10.1037/xhp0000659 URL |
[63] |
Rief, W., & Glombiewski, J. A. (2016). Expectation-focused psychological interventions (EFPI). Verhaltenstherapie, 26, 47-54. https://doi.org/10.1159/000442374
doi: 10.1159/000442374 URL |
[64] |
Rief W., Glombiewski J. A., Gollwitzer M., Schubö A., Schwarting R., & Thorwart A. (2015). Expectancies as core features of mental disorders. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 28(5), 378-385. https://doi.org/10.1097/YCO.0000000000000184
doi: 10.1097/YCO.0000000000000184 URL pmid: 26164612 |
[65] |
Sambrook, T. D., & Goslin, J. (2015). A neural reward prediction error revealed by a meta-analysis of ERPs using great grand averages. Psychological Bulletin, 141(1), 213-235. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000006
doi: 10.1037/bul0000006 URL pmid: 25495239 |
[66] |
Sawada, R., Sato, W., Minemoto, K., & Fushiki, T. (2019). Hunger promotes the detection of high-fat food. Appetite, 142, 104377. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2019.104377
doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2019.104377 URL |
[67] |
Sawada, R., Sato, W., Toichi, M., & Fushiki, T. (2017). Fat content modulates rapid detection of food: A visual search study using fast food and Japanese diet. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1033. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01033
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01033 URL pmid: 28690568 |
[68] | Schaefer, A., Buratto, L. G., Goto, N., & Brotherhood, E. V. (2016). The feedback-related negativity and the P300 brain potential are sensitive to price expectation violations in a virtual shopping task. PloS One, 11(9), e0163150. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0163150 |
[69] |
Shanks, D. R. (2007). Associationism and cognition: Human contingency learning at 25. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60(3), 291-309.https://doi.org/10.1080/17470210601000581
doi: 10.1080/17470210601000581 URL |
[70] |
Sharot, T., Fleming, S. M., Yu, X., Koster, R., & Dolan, R. J. (2012). Is choice-induced preference change long lasting?. Psychological Science, 23(10), 1123-1129. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612438733
doi: 10.1177/0956797612438733 URL pmid: 22933456 |
[71] |
Shen, H., & Sengupta, J. (2014). The crossmodal effect of attention on preferences: Facilitation versus impairment. Journal of Consumer Research, 40(5), 885-903. https://doi.org/10.1086/673261
doi: 10.1086/673261 URL |
[72] | Spence, C. (2015). Multisensory flavor perception. Cell, 161(1), 24-35.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cell.2015.03.007 |
[73] | Stilwell, B. T., & Vecera, S. P. (2019). Learned distractor rejection during strong target guidance. Journal of Vision, 19(10), 213a-213a. https://doi.org/10.1167/19.10.213a |
[74] |
Sui, J., He, X., & Humphreys, G. W. (2012). Perceptual effects of social salience: Evidence from self-prioritization effects on perceptual matching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38(5), 1105-1117. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029792
doi: 10.1037/a0029792 URL |
[75] |
Tusche, A., Bode, S., & Haynes, J.-D. (2010). Neural responses to unattended products predict later consumer choices. Journal of Neuroscience, 30(23), 8024-8031. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0064-10.2010
doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0064-10.2010 URL pmid: 20534850 |
[76] | van Osselaer, S. M. (2008). Associative learning and consumer decisions. Handbook of Consumer Psychology, 1, 699-729. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203809570 |
[77] |
Velasco, C., Wan, X., Knoeferle, K., Zhou, X., Salgado- Montejo, A., & Spence, C. (2015). Searching for flavor labels in food products: The influence of color-flavor congruence and association strength. Frontiers in Psychology, 6: 301. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00301
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00301 URL pmid: 25870568 |
[78] | Wilson, D. A., & Stevenson, R. J. (2006). Learning to smell: Olfactory perception from neurobiology to behavior. Baltimore, MD: The Johns Hopkins University Press. |
[79] |
Wolfe, J. M. (1994). Guided search 2.0 a revised model of visual search. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1(2), 202-238. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03200774.
doi: 10.3758/BF03200774 URL |
[1] | 王勇丽, 葛胜男, Lancy Lantin Huang, 万勤, 卢海丹. 言语想象的神经机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2023, 31(4): 608-621. |
[2] | 孔祥祯, 张凤翔, 蒲艺. 空间导航的脑网络基础和调控机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2023, 31(3): 330-337. |
[3] | 张明霞, 李雨欣, 李瑾, 刘勋. 内外动机对青少年记忆的影响及其神经机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2023, 31(1): 1-9. |
[4] | 王松雪, 程思, 蒋挺, 刘勋, 张明霞. 外在奖赏对陈述性记忆的影响[J]. 心理科学进展, 2023, 31(1): 78-86. |
[5] | 邹迪, 李红, 王福顺. 唤醒定义探析及其认知神经生理基础[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(9): 2020-2033. |
[6] | 林浇敏, 李爱梅, 周雅然, 何军红, 周蕾. 眼动操纵技术在决策研究中的应用前景:改变决策行为[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(8): 1794-1803. |
[7] | 邓尧, 王梦梦, 饶恒毅. 风险决策研究中的仿真气球冒险任务[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(6): 1377-1392. |
[8] | 李亮, 李红. 人们为什么会羞怯:认知机制及神经基础[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(5): 1038-1049. |
[9] | 李俊娇, 陈伟, 石佩, 董媛媛, 郑希付. 预期错误在恐惧记忆更新中的作用与机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(4): 834-850. |
[10] | 武晓菲, 肖风, 罗劲. 创造性认知重评在情绪调节中的迁移效应及其神经基础[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(3): 477-485. |
[11] | 章丽娜, 宣宾. 语言产生中词频效应老化的神经基础与时间进程[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(2): 333-342. |
[12] | 李何慧, 黄慧雅, 董琳, 罗跃嘉, 陶伍海. 发展性阅读障碍与小脑异常:小脑的功能和两者的因果关系[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(2): 343-353. |
[13] | 胡佳宝, 雷扬, 定险峰, 程晓荣, 范炤. 大众与个人审美品位的认知与神经机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(2): 354-364. |
[14] | 叶丽群, 谭欣, 姚堃, 丁玉珑. 正常老化对视觉早期注意的影响——来自ERP的证据[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(12): 2746-2763. |
[15] | 陈群林, 丁珂. 发散思维的序列位置效应:创新想法动态产生机制的新视角[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(11): 2507-2517. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||