心理科学进展 ›› 2021, Vol. 29 ›› Issue (9): 1617-1627.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2021.01617
收稿日期:
2020-12-07
发布日期:
2021-07-22
通讯作者:
李广政
E-mail:psyliz@sina.com
基金资助:
Received:
2020-12-07
Published:
2021-07-22
Contact:
LI Guangzheng
E-mail:psyliz@sina.com
摘要:
手势是在交流或认知过程中产生的、不直接作用于物体的手部运动, 具有具体性和抽象性。其分类主要从手势的来源、手势的内容、手势的意图及手势和言语的匹配性等角度进行划分。不同类型手势在出现时间及发展趋势上存在差异。手势在儿童词汇学习、言语表达、数学问题解决、空间学习及记忆等方面起促进作用, 但对言语理解的影响未得出一致结论。未来可关注不同类型手势与儿童认知发展的关系及对比不同来源手势在各学习领域的优势情况。
中图分类号:
王辉, 李广政. (2021). 儿童手势及其与学习的关系. 心理科学进展 , 29(9), 1617-1627.
WANG Hui, LI Guangzheng. (2021). Children’s gestures and the relationship with learning. Advances in Psychological Science, 29(9), 1617-1627.
[1] | 罗丹. (2020). 前言语阶段婴儿手势对语言发展的预测. 学前教育研究, (9), 39-47. |
[2] | 托马塞洛. (2012). 人类沟通的起源 (蔡雅菁, 译译). 北京: 商务印书馆. |
[3] | 伍珍, 郭睿. (2017). 婴儿指示性手势与其语言学习的关系. 心理科学进展, 25(10), 1705-1712. |
[4] | 于文华, 鲁忠义. (2020). 手势认知功能研究的新视角: “空间化”手势假设. 心理科学进展, 28(3), 426-433. |
[5] | 张恒超. (2019). 交流手势认知理论. 心理科学进展, 27(3), 499-507. |
[6] |
Acredolo L. P., & Goodwyn S. W. (1988). Symbolic gesturing in normal infants. Child Development, 59(2), 450-466.
pmid: 2452052 |
[7] |
Alibali M. W., & Goldin-Meadow S. (1993). Gesture-speech mismatch and mechanisms of learning: What the hands reveal about a child’ s state of mind. Cognitive Psychology, 25(4), 468-523.
pmid: 8243044 |
[8] |
Alibali M. W., & Nathan M. J. (2012). Embodiment in mathematics teaching and learning: Evidence from learners’ and teachers’ gestures. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 21(2), 247-286.
doi: 10.1080/10508406.2011.611446 URL |
[9] | Alibali M. W., Nathan M. J., Church R. B., Wolfgram M. S., Kim S., & Knuth E. J. (2013). Teachers’ gestures and speech in mathematics lessons: Forging common ground by resolving trouble spots. Mathematics Education, 45(3), 425-440. |
[10] | Asch S. E., & Nerlove H. (1960). The development of double function terms in children. In B. Kaplan & S. Wapner (Eds.), Perspectives in psychological theory (pp.47-60). New York: International Universities Press. |
[11] |
Aussems S. (2020). How seeing iconic gestures facilitates action event memory and verb learning in 3-year-old children. Language Acquisition, 27(1), 68-70.
doi: 10.1080/10489223.2019.1624759 URL |
[12] |
Austin E. E., & Sweller N. (2014). Presentation and production: The role of gesture in spatial communication. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 122, 92-103.
doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2013.12.008 URL |
[13] | Bates E., Camaioni L., & Volterra V. (1975). The acquisition of performatives prior to speech. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly of Behavior and Development, 21(3), 205-226. |
[14] |
Beaudoin-Ryan L., & Goldin-Meadow S. (2014). Teaching moral reasoning through gesture. Developmental Science, 17(6), 984-990.
doi: 10.1111/desc.12180 pmid: 24754707 |
[15] |
Billow R. M. (1981). Observing spontaneous metaphor in children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 31(3), 430-445.
doi: 10.1016/0022-0965(81)90028-X URL |
[16] |
Booth A. E., McGregor K. K., & Rohlfing K. J. (2008). Socio-pragmatics and attention: Contributions to gesturally guided word learning in toddlers. Language Learning and Development, 4(3), 179-202.
doi: 10.1080/15475440802143091 URL |
[17] |
Broaders S. C., Cook S. W., Mitchell Z., & Goldin-Meadow S. (2007). Making children gesture brings out implicit knowledge and leads to learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 136(4), 539-550.
doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.136.4.539 URL |
[18] |
Bruner J. (1981). The social context of language acquisition. Language and Communication, 1(2/3), 155-178.
doi: 10.1016/0271-5309(81)90010-0 URL |
[19] |
Cameron H., & Xu X. (2011). Representational Gesture, pointing gesture, and memory recall of preschool children. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 35(2), 155-171.
doi: 10.1007/s10919-010-0101-2 URL |
[20] |
Church R. B., & Goldin-Meadow S. (1986). The mismatch between gesture and speech as an index of transitional knowledge. Cognition, 23(1), 43-71.
pmid: 3742990 |
[21] | Church R. B., Garber P., & Rogalski K. (2007). The role of gesture in memory and social communication. Gesture, 7(2), 137-158. |
[22] | Clark A. (2008). Supersizing the mind: Embodiment, action, and cognitive extension. New York: Oxford University Press. |
[23] |
Cochet H., & Vauclair J. (2010). Pointing gestures produced by toddlers from 15 to 30 months: Different functions, hand shapes and laterality patterns. Infant Behavior and Development, 33(4), 431-441.
doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2010.04.009 URL |
[24] |
Cook S. W., Duffy R. G., & Fenn K. M. (2013). Consolidation and transfer of learning after observing hand gesture. Child Development, 84(6), 1863-1871.
doi: 10.1111/cdev.2013.84.issue-6 URL |
[25] |
Dargue N., Sweller N., & Jones M. P. (2019). When our hands help us understand: A meta-analysis into the effects of gesture on comprehension. Psychological Bulletin, 145(8), 765-784.
doi: 10.1037/bul0000202 URL |
[26] |
Ehrlich S. B., Levine S. C., & Goldin-Meadow S. (2006). The importance of gesture in children’s spatial reasoning. Developmental Psychology, 42(6), 1259.
pmid: 17087558 |
[27] | Gentner D. (1982). Why nouns are learned before verbs: Linguistic relativity versus natural partitioning. In S. A. Kuczaj (Ed.), Language development: Language, cognition, and culture (Vol. 2, pp. 301-334). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. |
[28] |
Gentner D. (1988). Metaphor as structure mapping: The relational shift. Child Development, 59(1), 47-59.
doi: 10.2307/1130388 URL |
[29] | Goldin-Meadow S. (2016). Using our hands to change our minds. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 8(1-2), e1368. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1368 |
[30] |
Goldin-Meadow S. (2018). Taking a hands-on approach to learning. Policy Insights from the Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 5(2), 163-170.
doi: 10.1177/2372732218785393 URL |
[31] |
Goldin-Meadow S., & Alibali M. W. (2013). Gesture’s role in speaking, learning, and creating language. Annual Review of Psychology, 64(1), 257-283.
doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143802 URL |
[32] |
Goldin-Meadow S., Goodrich W., Sauer E., & Iverson J. (2007). Young children use their hands to tell their mothers what to say. Developmental Science, 10(6), 778-785.
pmid: 17973795 |
[33] |
Goldin-Meadow S., Levine S. C., Zinchenko E., Yip T. K., Hemani N., & Factor L. (2012). Doing gesture promotes learning a mental transformation task better than seeing gesture. Developmental Science, 15(6), 876-884.
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2012.01185.x pmid: 23106741 |
[34] |
Goldin-Meadow S., & Mylander C. (1983). Gestural communication in deaf children: Non-effect of parental input on language development. Science, 221(4608), 372-374.
pmid: 6867713 |
[35] |
Goldstein M. H., Schwade J., Briesch J., & Syal S. (2010). Learning while babbling: Prelinguistic object-directed vocalizations indicate a readiness to learn. Infancy, 15(4), 362-391.
doi: 10.1111/j.1532-7078.2009.00020.x pmid: 32693523 |
[36] |
Guidetti M., & Nicoladis E. (2008). Introduction to special issue: Gestures and communicative development. First Language, 28(2), 107-115.
doi: 10.1177/0142723708088914 URL |
[37] |
Hostetter A. B. (2011). When do gestures communicate? A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 137(2), 297-315.
doi: 10.1037/a0022128 pmid: 21355631 |
[38] |
Hostetter A. B., & Alibali M. W. (2008). Visible embodiment: Gestures as simulated action. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15(3), 495-514.
doi: 10.3758/PBR.15.3.495 URL |
[39] |
Igualada A., Esteve-Gibert N., & Prieto P. (2017). Beat gestures improve word recall in 3-to 5-year-old children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 156, 99-112.
doi: S0022-0965(16)30275-2 pmid: 28061372 |
[40] | Ito K. (2014). Children’s pragmatic use of prosodic prominence. In D. McNeill (Ed.), Pragmatic Development in Frst Language Acquisition (pp.199-218). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. |
[41] |
Iverson J. M., Capirci O., & Caselli M. C. (1994). From communication to language in two modalities. Cognitive Development, 9(1), 23-43.
doi: 10.1016/0885-2014(94)90018-3 URL |
[42] |
Iverson J. M., & Goldin-Meadow S. (2005). Gesture paves the way for language development. Psychological Science, 16(5), 367-371.
pmid: 15869695 |
[43] |
Johnson E. S., & Meade A. C. (1987). Developmental patterns of spatial ability: An early sex difference. Child Development, 58(3), 725-740.
pmid: 3608645 |
[44] |
Kirk E., & Lewis C. (2017). Gesture facilitates children’s creative thinking. Psychological Science, 28(2), 225-232.
doi: 10.1177/0956797616679183 URL |
[45] |
Lebarton E. S., Goldin-Meadow S., & Raudenbush S. (2015). Experimentally induced increases in early gesture lead to increases in spoken vocabulary. Journal of Cognition and Development, 16(2), 199-220.
doi: 10.1080/15248372.2013.858041 URL |
[46] | Lempers J. D. (1976). Production of pointing, comprehension of pointing and understanding of looking behavior in young children (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Minnesota. |
[47] |
Leonard T., & Cummins F. (2011). The temporal relation between beat gestures and speech. Language and Cognitive Processes, 26(10), 1457-1471.
doi: 10.1080/01690965.2010.500218 URL |
[48] |
Leung E. H., & Rheingold H. L. (1981). Development of pointing as a social gesture. Developmental Psychology, 17(2), 215-220.
doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.17.2.215 URL |
[49] | Lhommet M., & Marsella S. (2014). Metaphoric gestures: Towards grounded mental spaces. Berlin: Springer International Publishing. |
[50] |
Llanes-Coromina J., Vilà-Giménez I., Kushch O., Borràs-Comes J., & Prieto P. (2018). Beat gestures help preschoolers recall and comprehend discourse information. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 172, 168-188.
doi: S0022-0965(17)30046-2 pmid: 29626756 |
[51] |
Macoun A., & Sweller N. (2016). Listening and watching: The effects of observing gesture on preschoolers’ narrative comprehension. Cognitive Development, 40, 68-81.
doi: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2016.08.005 URL |
[52] |
Mathew M., Yuen I., & Demuth K. (2018). Talking to the beat: Six-year-olds’ use of stroke-defined non-referential gestures. First Language, 38(2), 111-128.
doi: 10.1177/0142723717734949 URL |
[53] |
Matthews D., Behne T., Lieven E., & Tomasello M. (2012). Origins of the human pointing gesture: A training study. Developmental Science, 15(6), 817-829.
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2012.01181.x URL |
[54] | Mayer R. E. (2009). Multimedia learning (2nd ed.). Cambridge: University Press. |
[55] |
McGregor K. K., Rohlfing K. J., Bean A., & Marschner E. (2009). Gesture as a support for word learning: The case of under. Journal of Child Language, 36(4), 807-828.
doi: 10.1017/S0305000908009173 pmid: 18947455 |
[56] | McGuinness D., & Morley C. (1991). Sex differences in the development of visuo-spatial ability in preschool children. Journal of Mental Imagery, 15(3-4), 143-150. |
[57] |
McNeil N. M., Alibali M. W., & Evans J. L. (2000). The role of gesture in children’s comprehension of spoken language: Now they need it, now they don’t. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 24(2), 131-150.
doi: 10.1023/A:1006657929803 URL |
[58] |
McNeill D. (1985). So you think gestures are nonverbal? Psychological Review, 92(3), 350-371.
doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.92.3.350 URL |
[59] | McNeill D. (1992). Hand and mind: What gestures reveal about thought. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago press. |
[60] |
Miller H. E., Andrews C. A., & Simmering V. R. (2020). Speech and gesture production provide unique insights into young children’s spatial reasoning. Child Development, 91(6), 1934-1952.
doi: 10.1111/cdev.v91.6 URL |
[61] |
Mumford K. H., & Kita S. (2013). Children use gesture to interpret novel verb meanings. Child Development, 85(3), 1181-1189.
doi: 10.1111/cdev.12188 URL |
[62] | Murphy C., & Messer D. (1977). Mothers, infants and pointing:A study of gesture. In H. R. Schaffer (Ed.), Studies in mother-infant interaction (pp.325-354). London: Academic Press. |
[63] |
Novack M. A., Congdon E., Hemani-Lopez N., & Goldin- Meadow S..(2014). From action to abstraction: Using the hands to learn math. Psychological Science, 25(4), 903-910.
doi: 10.1177/0956797613518351 URL |
[64] |
Novack M. A., & Goldin-Meadow S. (2015). Learning from gesture: How our hands change our minds. Educational Psychology Review, 27(3), 405-412.
doi: 10.1007/s10648-015-9325-3 URL |
[65] |
Novack M. A., & Goldin-Meadow S. (2017). Gesture as representational action: A paper about function. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 24(3), 652-665.
doi: 10.3758/s13423-016-1145-z URL |
[66] |
Olson J., & Masur E. F. (2015). Mothers’ labeling responses to infants’ gestures predict vocabulary outcomes. Journal of Child Language, 42(6), 1289-1311.
doi: 10.1017/S0305000914000828 URL |
[67] |
Özçalışkan Ş. (2007). Metaphors we move by: Children’s developing understanding of metaphorical motion in typologically distinct languages. Metaphor and Symbol, 22(2), 147-168.
doi: 10.1080/10926480701235429 URL |
[68] |
Özçalişkan Ş., Gentner D., & Goldin-Meadow S. (2014). Do iconic gestures pave the way for children’s early verbs? Applied Psycholinguistics, 35(6), 1143-1162.
doi: 10.1017/S0142716412000720 URL |
[69] | Özçalışkan Ş., Goldin-Meadow S., Stam G., & Ishino M. (2011). Is there an iconic gesture spurt at 26 months? In G. Stam & M. Ishino (Eds.), Integrating gestures: The interdisciplinary nature of gesture (pp.163-174). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. |
[70] |
Rauscher F. H., Krauss R. M., & Chen Y. (1996). Gesture, speech, and lexical access: The role of lexical movements in speech production. Psychological Science, 7(4), 226-231.
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1996.tb00364.x URL |
[71] |
Rowe M. L., & Goldin-Meadow S. (2009). Early gesture selectively predicts later language learning. Developmental Science, 12(1), 182-187.
doi: 10.1111/desc.2009.12.issue-1 URL |
[72] |
Sekine K., Sowden H., & Kita S. (2015). The development of the ability to semantically integrate information in speech and iconic gesture in comprehension. Cognitive Science, 39(8), 1855-1880.
doi: 10.1111/cogs.12221 URL |
[73] | Shattuck-Hufnagel S., Ren A., Mathew M., Yuen I., & Demuth K. (2016). Non-referential gestures in adult and child speech:Are they prosodic? In J. Barnes, A. Brugos, S. Shattuck-Hufnagel, & N. Veilleaux (Eds.), Proceedings of the 8th International Conference on Speech Prosody (pp.836-839). Boston: International Speech Communication Association. |
[74] |
So W. C., Sim Chen-Hui C., & Low Wei-Shan J.(2012). Mnemonic effect of iconic gesture and beat gesture in adults and children: Is meaning in gesture important for memory recall? Language and Cognitive Processes, 27(5), 665-681.
doi: 10.1080/01690965.2011.573220 URL |
[75] |
Stanfield C., Williamson R., & Özçalışkan Ş. (2014). How early do children understand gesture-speech combinations with iconic gestures? Journal of Child Language, 41(2), 462-471.
doi: 10.1017/S0305000913000019 pmid: 23534839 |
[76] |
Stevanoni E., & Salmon K. (2005). Giving memory a hand: Instructing children to gesture enhances their event recall. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 29(4), 217-233.
doi: 10.1007/s10919-005-7721-y URL |
[77] |
Sweller N., Shinooka-Phelan A., & Austin E. (2020). The effects of observing and producing gestures on japanese word learning. Acta Psychologica, 207, 103079. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2020.103079
doi: S0001-6918(19)30405-6 URL pmid: 32422417 |
[78] |
Tellier M. (2008). The effect of gestures on second language memorisation by young children. Gesture, 8(2), 219-235.
doi: 10.1075/gest URL |
[79] |
Tolar T. D., Lederberg A. R., Gokhale S., & Tomasello M. (2008). The development of the ability to recognize the meaning of iconic signs. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 13(2), 225-240.
doi: 10.1093/deafed/enm045 URL |
[80] |
Vilà-Giménez I., Igualada A., & Prieto P. (2019). Observing storytellers who use rhythmic beat gestures improves children's narrative discourse performance. Developmental Psychology, 55(2), 250-262.
doi: 10.1037/dev0000604 pmid: 30489138 |
[81] | Vilà-Giménez I., & Prieto P. (2020). Encouraging kids to beat: Children’s beat gesture production boosts their narrative performance. Developmental Science, 23(6), e12967. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12967 |
[82] |
Wilson M. (2002). Six views of embodied cognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9(4), 625-636.
doi: 10.3758/BF03196322 URL |
[83] |
Winner E., Rosenstiel A. K., & Gardner H. (1976). The development of metaphoric understanding. Developmental Psychology, 12(4), 289-297.
doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.12.4.289 URL |
[1] | 赖燕群, 杨琪, 黄宝珍, 赛李阳. 记忆的顿悟优势效应[J]. 心理科学进展, 2019, 27(12): 2034-2042. |
[2] | 冉光明;陈旭;张兴;马原啸. 社会性预期优势效应的神经机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2016, 24(5): 684-691. |
[3] | 张琪;尹天子;冉光明. 动态面孔表情优势效应的心理机制及神经基础[J]. 心理科学进展, 2015, 23(9): 1514-1522. |
[4] | 孙继民;姚温青;胡学平. 声音情绪的跨文化识别[J]. 心理科学进展, 2014, 22(5): 802-809. |
[5] | 韦程耀;赵冬梅. 面部表情的跨文化表达与识别研究述评[J]. 心理科学进展, 2012, 20(10): 1614-1622. |
[6] | 张秋颖;陈建文;于全磊;辛鹏. 情绪识别中的群内优势效应[J]. 心理科学进展, 2011, 19(2): 209-216. |
[7] | 陈永胜;牟丽霞. 西方社区感研究的现状与趋势[J]. 心理科学进展, 2007, 15(1): 169-173. |
[8] | 曾盼盼;俞国良. 数学学习不良的研究及趋势[J]. 心理科学进展, 2002, 10(1): 48-56. |
[9] | 叶浩生. 对西方心理学发展的几点看法[J]. 心理科学进展, 1997, 5(4): 2-653. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||