Loading...
ISSN 0439-755X
CN 11-1911/B

Archive

    25 August 2023, Volume 55 Issue 8 Previous Issue    Next Issue

    Reports of Empirical Studies
    For Selected: Toggle Thumbnails
    Reports of Empirical Studies
    Time course of the integration of the morpho-semantics and the meaning of two-character Chinese compound words
    CAI Wenqi, ZHANG Xiangyang, WANG Xiaojuan, YANG Jianfeng
    2023, 55 (8):  1207-1219.  doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2023.01207
    Abstract ( 334 )   HTML ( 40 )  
    PDF (615KB) ( 292 )  

    Previous studies have shown that morpho-semantic information can be automatically activated and influence word meaning access. However, the time course underlying the morpho-semantic activation and subsequent integration is unclear. In particular, an important issue is to clarify how morpho-semantic information involves in word semantic integration processing. The current event-related potential (ERP) study examined the time course of morpho-semantic information of the first and the second character that participated in whole-word semantic integration. We selected three types of two-character words: transparent compound words (e.g., 炽热, hot) having similar meanings with their two characters (both 炽 and 热 mean hot), opaque words (e.g., 风流, dissolute) having a different meaning to either the first (风, wind) or the second character (流, flow) and monomorphemic words (e.g., 伶俐, clever) having two characters that are not two independent morphemes. During the first character processing, the result found a morphological effect in the early (300~400 ms) and the late (460~700 ms) time window, showing more negative amplitude for two types of compound words than monomorphemic words. Whereas during the second character processing, the result found a significant semantic transparency effect at the early stage (260~420 ms), showing more negative-going waveform for the opaque words than transparent ones, and an inversed morphological effect at the late stage (480~700 ms) showing that two types of compound words evoked more positive amplitude than monomorphemic words. The results suggested that the morpheme was an independent unit represented in the mental lexicon and automatically activated at an early processing stage. Its meaning can facilitate the access of the related word meaning or inhibit the processing of the unrelated word meaning.

    Figures and Tables | References | Related Articles | Metrics
    Effect of inhibition of return on audiovisual cross-modal correspondence
    ZU Guangyao, LI Shuqi, ZHANG Tianyang, WANG Aijun, ZHANG Ming
    2023, 55 (8):  1220-1233.  doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2023.01220
    Abstract ( 121 )   HTML ( 25 )  
    PDF (155KB) ( 110 )  
    References | Related Articles | Metrics
    Value-directed attentional refreshing and its mechanism
    LI Haifeng, LIN Shiqing, WAN Bowen
    2023, 55 (8):  1234-1242.  doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2023.01234
    Abstract ( 146 )   HTML ( 19 )  
    PDF (201KB) ( 87 )  

    Attentional refreshing is the process of promoting and prolonging the activation of information in working memory (WM) by returning it to the focus of attention. This process can prevent the information in WM from fading over time or being disrupted by distractors. Previous studies have demonstrated that attentional refreshing can be guided by retro-cues or influenced by various experiences, such as reward-related or self-related stimuli. Recent studies have also explored the value effect in WM and found that people tend to prioritize more valuable information in WM, indicating that value may play a role in guiding attentional refreshing during retention. In a groundbreaking study by Atkinson et al. (2022), attentional refreshing was shown to partially explain the value effect in WM. However, the study was unable to determine why high-value information was prioritized for refreshing. It has been suggested that the value effect in WM may be due to a biased attentional refreshing procedure where individuals tend to focus more frequently or for longer periods on the more valuable item during retention, as compared to the other items.

    To investigate the value-directed attentional refreshing and its underlying mechanism, this study conducted three experiments. All experiments were designed with a within-subject design, with the independent variable being the value of the item (high or low). The sample size for each experiment was determined using G*power based on prior research. Experiment 1 examined the value-directed attentional refreshing under the condition of simultaneous presentation of information. Twenty-four valid participants took part in the experiment, 7 of them were male, with an average age of 18.92 ± 1.14 years.

    The procedure for Experiment 1 was shown in Figure 1. Participants were reminded before the start of the experiment to try their best to obtain the highest score during the recall phase. At the beginning of the experiment, a 500 ms fixation point was presented on the screen, followed by an articulatory suppression task, which required participants to continuously repeat “Fujian Normal University” until the dot detection task was completed. After a 500 ms interval, the memory material was presented, and participants were asked to remember all the letters within 4000 ms. Each letter was associated with a number representing its value. Then, a fixation point lasting 3000 to 3700 ms was presented on the screen, followed by a 300 ms box around the fixation point, reminding participants that a dot detection task was about to be performed. In the dot detection task, participants had 1500 ms to judge whether the two small dots displayed on the screen were vertical or horizontal. If they were vertical, they pressed the “9” key; if they were horizontal, they pressed the “0” key. The keys were balanced among participants, and the probability of the two small dots being horizontal or vertical was 50%. The small dots were randomly presented in one of the positions where the letters were previously presented. The dot detection position was balanced among the values. After the dot detection task, participants had 12 s to input the letters into six boxes displayed on the screen, without inputting the numbers. After completing the input, participants received feedback on the accuracy of the dot detection task and the recall score for the current trial. Each participant completed three blocks of trials, with 48 trials in each block and a total of 144 trials. Before the formal experiment, participants were required to complete five practice trials of the same program to ensure that they were familiar with the experimental procedure.

    A paired-sample t-test was conducted on the data from Experiment 1 and revealed that: (1) Participants' memory performance on high-value items (83.50% ± 7.47%) was significantly better than that on low-value items (54.35% ± 15.07%), t(23) = 8.55, p < 0.001, 95% CI = [0.22, 0.36], Cohen’s d = 1.75, indicating that value-guided memory exists in WM processes. (2) Participants' reaction time on the dot probe task at the location corresponding to high-value items (770 ms ± 102 ms) was significantly faster than that at the location corresponding to low-value items (789 ms ± 117 ms), t(23) = −2.41, p = 0.012, 95% CI = [−34.50, −2.60], Cohen’s d = −0.49, indicating that attentional refreshing during the WM maintenance is guided by value when items were presented simultaneously.

    Under the condition that items were presented simultaneously, Experiment 1 confirmed the existence of a value-directed attentional refreshing process. However, because the memory items in Experiment 1 were presented simultaneously, there are alternative explanations for this result. Firstly, during the encoding phase, participants may have paid more attention to the location of high-value items, which could lead to a reflective attentional bias. Therefore, the attentional advantage during the maintenance phase may have been caused by location, rather than value. Secondly, since memory items were presented simultaneously, participants may have adopted a strategy of encoding memory items in sequence, starting from high-value items in order to achieve the highest score during encoding. Similarly, during the free recall phase, participants may have prioritized recalling high-value items, leading to higher scores for those items than for low-value items. In order to eliminate these two possibilities, Experiment 2 presented the items one by one, with each item presented for the same amount of time (1000 ms) and randomly appearing in one of six positions. In addition, in order to test whether smaller values can still generate value-directed attentional refreshing, the corresponding pair of numbers for high and low values in Experiment 2 were set to “5” and “1”, respectively.

    Experiment 2 recruited 23 participants, 7 of whom were male, with an average age of 19.83 ± 1.80 years. The results showed that: (1) participants' memory performance for high-value items (79.41% ± 13.91%) was significantly better than that for low-value items (48.71% ± 11.59%), t(22) = 8.07, p < 0.001, 95% CI = [0.23, 0.39], Cohen's d = 1.68, indicating value could guide WM; (2) participants' reaction time at the location of high-value items (735 ms ± 110 ms) was significantly faster than that at the location of low-value items (751 ms ± 130 ms), t(22) = −1.79, p = 0.044, 95% CI = [−34.96, −2.58], Cohen's d = −0.37, indicating value could guide attentional refreshing during the WM maintenance when items were presented in succession.

    Experiment 3 employed the blank screen paradigm in combination with eye-tracking technology to investigate the attentional refreshing advantage of high-value information. Based on the biased attentional refreshing procedures (Atkinson et al., 2018, 2021; Hitch et al., 2018; Hu et al., 2016; Sandry et al., 2014), value-directed attentional refreshing can be achieved in two ways: by frequently refreshing the position of high-value information or by allocating more time to refreshing the position of high-value information. During the WM maintenance, attentional refreshing reactivates the location marker corresponding to the item, thereby causing eye movements to shift towards the corresponding spatial location on the blank screen. Therefore, the number of fixations on the regions of interest corresponding to different value information during the maintenance can measure the frequency of attentional refreshing, and the fixation duration at each fixation point can measure the duration of attentional refreshing.

    The procedure for Experiment 3 was shown in Figure 2. Participants were instructed to try their best to obtain the highest score during the recall phase. At the beginning of the experiment, a fixation point was presented on the screen for 500 ms, followed by an articulatory suppression task that required participants to repeat “Fujian Normal University” until the recall test. Then, four numbers corresponding to the value scores of the graphs at each position were displayed on the screen for 1000 ms. After the numbers disappeared for 500 ms, a screen containing 4 grey graphs was displayed for 1500 ms, during which participants were required to remember these graphs. Then, a blank screen lasting 3000 ms was presented, followed by a prompt displayed on the screen for 1000 ms. The prompt was used to instruct participants to recall the graph at that position. Finally, five graphs were presented on the screen. Participants should choose a graph from the 5 graphs that matched the graph corresponding to the cued position in 4000 ms. Each participant completed two blocks of trials, with 30 trials in each block and a total of 60 trials. Before the formal experiment, participants completed 5 practice trials to ensure they were familiar with the experimental procedure. The corresponding pair of numbers for high and low values in Experiment 3 were set to “6” and “1”, respectively.

    Experiment 3 recruited 24 participants, 10 of whom were male, with an average age of 21.00 ± 2.38 years. The results showed that: (1) participants’ memory performance for high-value (85.34% ± 12.52%) items was significantly higher than that for low-value (73.33% ± 17.23%) items, t(23) = 3.82, p < 0.001, 95% CI = [0.06, 0.19], Cohen’s d = 0.78, indicating that value could guide WM; (2) the number of fixations on the location corresponding to the high-value (0.32 ± 0.10 times/trial) items during the blank screen period was significantly higher than that on the location corresponding to the low-value (0.27 ± 0.08 times/trial) items, t(23) = 2.63, p = 0.007, 95% CI = [0.01, 0.08], Cohen’s d = 0.54; (3) there is no significant difference in the duration of single fixations on the location corresponding to high-value (313 ms ± 102 ms) and low-value (332 ms ± 125 ms) items during the blank screen period, t(23) = −1.55, p = 0.135, 95% CI = [−44.04, 6.33], Cohen’s d = −0.32. Since the null hypothesis statistical test p-value was greater than 0.05, it failed to support the null hypothesis. Therefore, a Bayesian test was conducted on the duration of single fixations to examine whether it supports the null hypothesis. The result showed that BF10 = 0.61, which did not support the null hypothesis. The above results indicated that value-directed attentional refreshing might be achieved by increasing the refresh rate of high-value information.

    The above experiments directly confirmed the value-directed attentional refreshing that high-value information received priority for attentional refreshing in WM retention when compared to low-value information. More importantly, the results indicated that value-directed attentional refreshing might be achieved by increasing the refresh rate of high-value information rather than deploying more time on it. This study contributes to the research on attentional refreshing and provides new insights into how people prioritize information in their daily lives. Moreover, it sheds light on the mechanism of value-directed attentional refreshing and helps develop the time-based resource-sharing model to a certain extent. These findings can aid researchers in developing computational models that simulate people’s attentional refreshing process.

    Figures and Tables | References | Related Articles | Metrics
    Dynamic changes on brain function during early stage of Tai Chi training: A motor imagery-based fMRI study
    LI Lin, ZHANG Xiaoyou, XU Yakui, ZONG Boyi, ZHAO Wenrui, ZHAO Ge, YAO Meng, ZHAN Zhuxuan, YIN Dazhi, FAN Mingxia
    2023, 55 (8):  1243-1254.  doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2023.01243
    Abstract ( 232 )   HTML ( 22 )  
    PDF (2576KB) ( 173 )  

    Accumulating evidence suggests that regular participation in physical exercise, especially structured exercise with complex movement patterns like Tai Chi, is linked to change in brain function as measured by spontaneous and task-evoked neural activities. Understanding the changes in neurological activities during the early stages of motor skill learning would facilitate more effective teaching and coaching, but research on brain function during the early stages of Tai Chi training has not been conducted. Based on this, we conducted a longitudinal study in which functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) was used to collect data on brain function and behavioral measures during the early stage of Tai Chi training at multiple time points.

    Nineteen college students with no prior Tai Chi experience were recruited for this study and were placed in a 14-week Tai Chi training program Of note, Tai Chi training session was recorded in order to evaluate the quality of Tai Chi form and its skill level (conducted by professor specializing in Tai Chi). Outcome measures were conducted at Week 2, Week 8, and Week 14. Meanwhile, 10 age- and sex-matched college students were considered as controls, who were asked to maintain normal lifestyle and schedule the same outcome measurements at baseline and Week 14. A Siemens 3.0 T MRI scanner was used to synchronously collect data on brain function while participants performed a motor imagery task. Group differences on Tai Chi skill level, temporal congruence, and functional activations were investigated using ANOVA while Pearson product-moment correlation was performed to examine relationships between them.

    Behavioral results showed a learning curve on Tai Chi skill level from slow (Week 2 to Week 8) to fast (after Week 8) as the quality of motor imagery gradually improved. fMRI results showed a similar pattern of change on brain activity, which changed slowly (Week 2 to Week 8) and increased fastly (after Week 8). Such behavioral changes on skill level were significantly linked to functional activation of the left superior temporal gyrus and the left precuneus.

    The learning of motor skills follows a pattern of “slow first, fast later”, which is supported by changes on brain activity in the left superior temporal gyrus and the left precuneus. In addition, Tai Chi is a type of a motor-cognitive exercise with relatively complex movement and its unique routine provides learners with the opportunity to optimize brain function.

    Figures and Tables | References | Related Articles | Metrics
    The effect of social value orientation on third-party altruistic behaviors in children aged 10-12 years: The role of emotion
    CHEN Peiqi, ZHANG Yinling, HU Xinmu, WANG Jing, MAI Xiaoqin
    2023, 55 (8):  1255-1269.  doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2023.01255
    Abstract ( 222 )   HTML ( 21 )  
    PDF (642KB) ( 208 )  

    Fairness plays a critical role in maintaining the social order. To understand fairness development, numerous studies have examined children's upholding fairness behaviors, such as resource allocation. In particular, the emergence of third-party altruism in Chinese children at the age of 8-10 is an important turning point in fairness development. Third-party altruism refers to the behavior of individuals voluntarily paying costs to punish violators, which is a form of prosocial behavior. Most previous studies have confirmed that social value orientation (SVO) affects prosocial behaviors, and some cognitive neuroscience studies have found that SVO and emotion together affect prosocial behaviors. However, we do not know the specific mechanisms by which children's SVO and emotions affect their third-party altruistic behaviors. Additionally, because third-party altruism can adopt punishment and compensation, the mechanisms may be different. Therefore, through two experiments, this study aimed to investigate the mechanisms of children's SVO and emotion on third-party altruism and the difference between children's third-party punishment and compensation behaviors in three offer conditions (i.e., high inequality, moderate inequality, and equality).

    Experiment 1 was based on the third-party punishment task and aimed to investigate the effect of children’s SVO on their emotions and punitive behaviors and to verify the mediating role of emotion. We recruited 233 children aged 10-12 years. After completing the demographic information, they were instructed to complete three third-party punishment tasks revised from the dictator game. The proposer in the dictator game offered one, three, and five coins from 10 coins to the recipient successively. As the third party, children reported the level of pleasure and decided whether to spend any integer of their endowed five coins to punish the proposer in each task. For every coin spent, two coins were deducted from the proposer’s endowment. The results revealed that prosocial children (vs. the pro-self) were more unpleased and spent more coins to punish the proposer, and emotion played a mediating role in the relationship between SVO and third-party punitive behaviors in the high inequality condition but not in the moderate inequality or equality conditions.

    To deeply understand children’s third-party altruistic behaviors and compare the two kinds of behaviors, we conducted Experiment 2 based on the third-party compensation task. We recruited 238 children aged 10-12 years. The experimental procedure was similar to that of Experiment 1, except that children in Experiment 2 spent coins to compensate the recipient rather than punish the proposer. The results revealed that prosocial children (vs. the pro-self) spent more coins to compensate the recipient in the high and moderate inequality conditions; emotion mediated the relationship between SVO and third-party compensation behaviors only in the high inequality condition. As for the difference between the two kinds of third-party altruistic behaviors, children in the third-party compensation (vs. punishment) task had less emotional fluctuation when confronted with three offers and spent more coins to maintain a fair order in the moderate unequal condition.

    These findings suggest that SVO had a stable effect on third-party punishment and compensation in 10- to 12-year-old children under all three offer conditions, and that emotion mediated the relationship between SVO and each kind of third-party altruistic behavior when children were confronted with an extremely unfair offer. Additionally, the children showed different levels of pleasure and behavior in the two third-party altruistic tasks. Our study contributes to revealing the mechanisms of SVO and emotion on two kinds of third-party altruistic behaviors and suggests that prosocial orientation is a critical factor in fostering children’s third-party altruism.

    Figures and Tables | References | Related Articles | Metrics
    The effect of cumulative risk related to family adversity on social adjustment among left-behind children in China: The mediating role of stress and the moderating role of psychosocial resources
    FAN Xing-hua, FANG Xiao-yi, ZHAO Xian, CHEN Feng-ju
    2023, 55 (8):  1270-1284.  doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2023.01270
    Abstract ( 295 )   HTML ( 8 )  
    PDF (503KB) ( 191 )  

    Relative to non-left-behind children (NLBC), left-behind children (LBC) are exposed to various risk factors related to family, such as lack of parental care and insufficient family support, which could increase their vulnerability to psychological and behavioral problems. Based on Bronfenbrenner’s bioecological theory and the cumulative risk model, this study used two-wave data (T1 and T2) to examine the association between cumulative risk related to family adversity (T1) and social adjustment outcomes (T1/T2), in which stress (T1/T2) is a mediator, and examined the moderating role of psychosocial resources (T1) in this association. The two-wave longitudinal household surveys were conducted among six hundred and fifty-one families of rural children. A total of 285 children whose both parents migrated for work throughout the study waves were categorized as the LBC group, while 366 children who reported living with their parents at least one of waves were categorized as the control group. All measures in the surveys showed good reliability, including family adversity, stress, psychosocial resources (i.e., psychological capital and social support) and social adjustment (i.e., subjective well-being, depression, positive behaviors and problem behaviors). Data analyses were performed using SPSS 24.0 and AMOS 22.0. Results showed that: (1) LBC’s T1 cumulative risk related to family adversity was linearly associated with their T1/T2 social adjustment; (2) After controlling for gender and age, LBC’s T1 cumulative risk related to family adversity was negatively associated with T1 social adjustment (β = ?0.42, p < 0.001), and T1 stress mediated this association. The association between stress and social adjustment was moderated by psychosocial resources, with a higher level of psychosocial resources associated with a smaller mediating effect of stress. (3) After controlling for gender and age, T1 stress and T1 social adjustment, T1 cumulative risk related to family adversity were negatively associated with T2 social adjustment (β = ?0.23, p < 0.001), and T2 stress mediated this relationship. T1 psychosocial resources moderated the association of T1 cumulative risk related to family adversity on both T2 social adjustment and T2 stress. This showed that with the level of T1 psychosocial resources increasing, the main effect of T1 cumulative risk related to family adversity on T2 social adjustment and the mediation effect of T2 stress decreased and eventually became statistically non-significant. The findings of this study demonstrate the detrimental impact of cumulative risk related to family adversity on social adjustment, as well as the mediating role of stress and the moderating role of psychosocial resources among LBC. Overall, these findings suggest that family risk factors are proximal factors for LBC’s social maladjustment, and future intervention should attend to psychosocial resource promotion.

    Figures and Tables | References | Related Articles | Metrics
    Association between childhood maltreatment and empathy:A three-level meta-analytic review
    MENG Xianxin, YU Delin, CHEN Yijing, ZHANG Lin, FU Xiaolan
    2023, 55 (8):  1285-1300.  doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2023.01285
    Abstract ( 268 )   HTML ( 32 )  
    PDF (296KB) ( 205 )  

    A considerable number of studies have discussed the association between childhood maltreatment and empathy, but the results have been mixed. Theoretically, there are four main arguments regarding the association between childhood maltreatment and empathy. Attachment theory suggests that childhood maltreatment predisposes individuals to an insecure attachment style that is detrimental to empathy development. The facial feedback hypothesis and “like-me” hypothesis suggest that neglected children have fewer opportunities to imitate others’ facial expressions, and show deficits in empathy. However, the perception-action model argues that individuals with childhood maltreatment are more likely to empathize with others who have traumatic experiences, while the Russian doll model implies that the association between childhood maltreatment and empathy may not be linear. Empirically, current research has reported an inconsistent correlation between childhood maltreatment and empathy, with r values ranging from −0.451 to 0.86. Therefore, the present meta-analysis aimed to estimate the extent to which childhood maltreatment is associated with empathy and whether these associations vary depending on the study or sample characteristics such as the type of childhood maltreatment, gender, and age.

    A systematic literature review was conducted using Web of Science, ScienceDirect, PubMed, PsycARTICLES, CNKI (China National Knowledge Infrastructure), CSTJD (China Science and Technology Journal Database) and WFD (Wan Fang Data). The literature screening process is shown in Figure 1. Three-level meta-analyses were performed using R to synthesize the effect sizes and conduct moderator analyses. Publication bias was assessed using funnel plots and Egger's regression tests. No significant publication bias was observed in any of the studies.

    A total of 46 studies (N = 23039 participants) producing 352 effect sizes were included. The results showed that the correlation between childhood maltreatment and empathy was significantly negative but only to a small extent (r = −0.076, 95% CI [−0.117, −0.035]). Table 1 showed the results of moderator analysis, which revealed that the association between childhood maltreatment and empathy was moderated by the type of childhood maltreatment. The association between childhood maltreatment and empathy was stronger for physical neglect (r = −0.095) and emotional neglect (r = −0.128) than for physical abuse (r = 0.005). Furthermore, the association between childhood maltreatment and empathy was moderated by the dimensions of empathy. Specifically, the association between childhood maltreatment and empathy was negative for perspective-taking (r = −0.127), fantasy (r = −0.044), and empathic concern (r = −0.148), but positive for personal distress (r = 0.153). In addition, the mean age of the participants moderated the association between childhood maltreatment and empathy, with the mean age predicting a reduced negative association between childhood maltreatment and empathy (β = 0.004). However, the percentage of females did not moderate the observed association, which may indicate consistency in the association between childhood maltreatment and empathy across genders.

    The results supported the attachment theory, the facial feedback hypothesis, the "like-me" hypothesis, the perception-action model, and the Russian doll model, suggesting that the association between childhood maltreatment and empathy was complicated. These findings not only deepen our understanding of the association between childhood maltreatment and empathy but also produce meaningful practical implications for future research and intervention program design regarding how to promote the development of empathy.

    Figures and Tables | References | Related Articles | Metrics
    How to teach resourcefully? The mechanism of teacher dialectical feedback on team creativity of college students
    ZHANG Jianwei, ZHOU Yufan, LI Linying, LI Haihong, HUA Weijun
    2023, 55 (8):  1301-1316.  doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2023.01301
    Abstract ( 118 )   HTML ( 11 )  
    PDF (233KB) ( 83 )  
    Figures and Tables | References | Related Articles | Metrics
    Self-compassion writing facilitates fear extinction
    MEI Ying, LIU Juntong, LIU Honghong, FU Yang, LUO Xi, LEI Yi
    2023, 55 (8):  1317-1329.  doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2023.01317
    Abstract ( 234 )   HTML ( 12 )  
    PDF (402KB) ( 136 )  

    Previous studies have demonstrated that self-compassion interventions can effectively alleviate psychological symptoms associated with anxiety-related psychiatric disorders. However, the underlying mechanism through which self-compassion improves anxiety remains unclear. Exposure therapy, which is based on fear extinction, is a key treatment for anxiety disorders. Investigating how self-compassion interventions can promote fear extinction may provide insights into the mechanism of exposure therapy. To investigate this question, we randomly assigned 56 healthy participants to complete either a self-compassion writing (N = 29) or a control writing (N = 27) task before fear extinction. We measured positive and negative affects before and after writing. US expectancy and skin conductance responses were recorded during the early and late phases of fear extinction. Results revealed that the negative affect reduced in both writing tasks, but self-compassion writing group displayed lower US expectancy to both conditioned threat and safety cues during early and late fear extinction compared to control writing group. Furthermore, in the self-compassion group, skin conductance responses were found to be smaller during the early fear extinction phase compared to the late fear extinction phase. These results suggest that self-compassion writing may facilitate fear extinction through updating the cognition that the conditioned cues is no longer dangerous. This providing insights into the psychological and physiological mechanisms that underlie the beneficial effects of self-compassion on anxiety symptoms. Moreover, our results emphasize the need to incorporate cognitive interventions into exposure therapy based on the fear extinction paradigm.

    Figures and Tables | References | Related Articles | Metrics
    Psychological richness increases behavioral intention to protect the environment
    WEI Xinni, YU Feng, PENG Kaiping, ZHONG Nian
    2023, 55 (8):  1330-1343.  doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2023.01330
    Abstract ( 439 )   HTML ( 33 )  
    PDF (369KB) ( 531 )  

    Understanding the relationship between happiness and positive factors and pro-environmental behavior offers important practical implications for sustainable social development. To investigate the positive antecedents of pro-environmental behavior, the current study focused on psychological richness and examined its influence on pro-environmental behavior as well as potential mechanisms and boundary conditions through 10 studies (N = 2979). It is shown that psychological richness facilitates engagement in sustainable activities (Studies 1.1-1.4) through an increased level of self-expansion (Studies 2.1-2.4). Furthermore, the effect of self-expansion on pro-environmental behavior was more significant when individuals viewed nature as smaller than themselves (Studies 3.1-3.2). These findings reveal the positive effect of happiness on pro-environmental behaviors and provide insights to promote people's participation in building a sustainable society.

    Figures and Tables | References | Related Articles | Metrics
    Familiarity promotes resident cooperation with volunteers in waste separation
    ZHANG Xuan, LIU Ping-Ping
    2023, 55 (8):  1358-1371.  doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2023.01358
    Abstract ( 103 )   HTML ( 4 )  
    PDF (240KB) ( 74 )  

    Despite the growing urgency of waste separation as environmental concern, the rates of contamination from unsorted waste remain high without the monitoring or feedback from volunteers. How can we facilitate collaboration between residents and volunteers? To address the dilemma of waste separation, the present study conducted three experiments and a qualitative interview based on the theories of competitive altruism and social influence to investigate the effects of familiarity and age on residents' cooperation, as well as the mediating roles of reputational concern and social distance.

    Using scenarios, participants were asked to read the instructions (each representing one of the conditions), and then to predict the extent of probability that they would cooperate in each condition. Experiment 1 adopted a 3 (age group of volunteers: primary children/younger/older) × 3 (volunteer familiarity: high/low/unfamiliar) within-subjects design to examine the differences in the cooperation of young people (M = 20.16 ± 1.01 years) with volunteers in different conditions. Experiment 2 adopted a 2 (age group of participants: younger/older; between-subjects variable) × 3 (age group of volunteers: primary children/younger/older; within-subjects variable) × 3 (volunteer familiarity: high/low/unfamiliar; within-subjects variable) mixed design, to examine the age-related differences of the cooperation between younger people (M = 40.63 ± 7.60 years) and older people (M = 68.90 ± 4.97 years) with volunteers in different conditions. The findings of Experiment 1 were replicated. Experiment 3 adopted a 2 (age group of volunteers: younger/older) × 2 (volunteer familiarity: high/unfamiliar) within-subjects design to replicate the results of Experiment 1 and Experiment 2, and it lasted for four weeks at an interval of one week for each participant. Experiment 3 recruited 84 participants (M = 26.05 ± 11.01 years) and aimed to examine the mediating role of reputational concern and social distance in the relationship between familiarity and cooperation.

    Experiment 1 showed that participants cooperated more with the high familiar volunteers (79.11%) than with the low familiar (61.60%) or strange volunteers (27.64%). The higher the familiarity, the greater the cooperative intention (see Figure 1), χ2(16, N= 1422) = 285.64, p< 0.001. As age and familiarity interact to affect the cooperative intention, participants cooperated more with older volunteers (88.98%) than with primary children (77.12%) in the low familiar condition, χ2(1, N= 236) = 5.90, p = 0.015, odds ratio = 2.40, 95% CI [1.17, 4.92].

    Experiment 2 replicated the results of Experiment 1 (see Figure 2), and found that familiarity had a significant effect on the cooperative intention of both younger and older adults [current: F(2, 344) = 128.04, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.43; next time: F(2, 344) = 99.05, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.37]. As age and familiarity interact to affect the cooperative intention, F(4, 688) = 3.12, p = 0.015, η2 = 0.02, participants cooperated more with older volunteers (M = 5.36, SD = 1.15) than with primary children (M = 5.14, SD =1.37) in the low familiar condition. Besides, older adults cooperated more with older volunteers (M = 5.54, SD = 0.11) than younger adults (M = 5.20, SD = 0.11), p = 0.025, while less with strangers (M = 4.28, SD = 0.13) than younger adults (M= 4.77, SD = 0.13), p = 0.008.

    Experiment 3 replicated the results of the first two experiments, and examined the underlying mechanism (see Figure 3). A bootstrap analysis (bootstrap resamples = 5,000) revealed that the bias-corrected 95% CI of both social distance [−0.027, 0.090] and reputational concern [−0.020, 0.030] did not exclude zero, indicating that the indirect effects of social distance (β = 0.031, SE = 0.030) and reputational concern (β = 0.004, SE = 0.013) were not significant. Moreover, when social distance and reputational concern were simultaneously treated as mediators in the multiple mediation model, the indirect effect of social distance and reputational concern (β = 0.034, 95% CI [0.019, 0.053]) was significant. These findings indicated that social distance and reputational concern play a serial mediating role in the effect of familiarity on cooperation. The results of multiple regression analysis and qualitative interviews showed that the monitoring and feedback from volunteers, familiarity with the residents, and individual prosociality, play key roles in promoting waste separation.

    These findings provide suggestions and scientific support for the practice of waste separation and saving management costs. The present study demonstrates that helping residents to become familiar with volunteers in advance, or recruiting people who are familiar with residents as volunteers, could promote waste separation. These strategies can promote the cooperative intention of residents, and help them form the habit of waste separation. Social distance and reputational concern play a serial mediating role in the effect of familiarity on cooperation.

    Figures and Tables | References | Related Articles | Metrics
    Scaling methods of second-order latent growth models and their comparable first-order latent growth models
    WEN Zhonglin, WANG Yifan, DU Mingshi, YU Yahui, ZHANG Yuhui, JIN Tonglin
    2023, 55 (8):  1372-1382.  doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2023.01372
    Abstract ( 195 )  
    Latent growth models (LGMs) are a powerful tool for analyzing longitudinal data, and have attracted the attention of scholars in psychology and other social science disciplines. For a latent variable measured by multiple indicators, we can establish both a univariate LGM (also called first-order LGM) based on composite scores and a latent variable LGM (also called second-order LGM) based on indicators. The two model types are special cases of the first-order and second-order factor models respectively. In either case, we need to scale the factors, that is, to specify their origin and unit. Under the condition of strong measurement invariance across time, the estimation of growth parameters in second-order LGMs depends on the scaling method of factors/latent variables. There are three scaling methods: the scaled-indicator method (also called the marker-variable identification method), the effect-coding method (also called the effect-coding identification method), and the latent-standardization method.
    The existing latent-standardization method depends on the reliability of the scaled-indicator or the composite scores at the first time point. In this paper, we propose an operable latent-standardization method with two steps. In the first step, a CFA with strong measurement invariance is conducted by fixing the mean and variance of the latent variable at the first time point to 0 and 1 respectively. In the second step, estimated loadings in the first step are employed to establish the second-order LGM. If the standardization is based on the scaled-indicator method, the loading of the scaled-indicator is fixed to that obtained in the first step, and the intercept of the scaled-indicator is fixed to the sample mean of the scaled-indicator at the first time point. If the standardization is based on the effect-coding method, the sum of loadings is constrained to the sum of loadings obtained in the first step, and the sum of intercepts is constrained to the sum of the sample mean of all indicators at the first time point. We also propose a first-order LGM standardization procedure based on the composite scores. First, we standardize the composite scores at the first time point, and make the same linear transformation of the composite scores at the other time points. Then we establish the first-order LGM, which is comparable with the second-order LGM scaled by the latent-standardization method.
    The scaling methods of second-order LGMs and their comparable first-order LGMs are systematically summarized. The comparability is illustrated by modeling the empirical data of a Moral Evasion Questionnaire. For the scaled-indicator method, second-order LGMs and their comparable first-order LGMs are rather different in parameter estimates (especially when the reliability of the scale-indicator is low). For the effect-coding method, second-order LGMs and their comparable first-order LGMs are relatively close in parameter estimates. When the latent variable at the first time point is standardized, the mean of the intercept-factor of the first-order LGM is close to 0 and not statistically significant; so is the mean of the intercept-factor of the second-order LGM through the effect-coding method, but those through two scaled-indicator methods are statistically significant and different from each other.
    According to our research results, the effect-coding method is recommended to scale and standardize the second-order LGMs, then comparable first-order LGMs are those based on the composite scores and their standardized models. For either the first-order or second-order LGM, the standardized results obtained by modeling composite total scores and composite mean scores are identical.
    Related Articles | Metrics
    Binary modeling of action sequences in problem-solving tasks: One- and two-parameter action sequence model
    FU Yanbin, CHEN Qipeng, ZHAN Peida
    2023, 55 (8):  1383-1396.  doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2023.01383
    Abstract ( 156 )   HTML ( 8 )  
    PDF (4040KB) ( 76 )  

    Process data refers to the human-computer or human-human interaction data recorded in computerized learning and assessment systems that reflect respondents’ problem-solving processes. Among the process data, action sequences are the most typical data because they reflect how respondents solve the problem step by step. However, the non-standardized format of action sequences (i.e., different data lengths for different participants) also poses difficulties for the direct application of traditional psychometric models. Han, Liu, and Ji (2022) proposed the sequential response model (SRM) by combining dynamic Bayesian networks with the nominal response model (NRM) to address the shortcomings of existing methods. Similar to the NRM, the SRM uses multinomial logistic modeling, which in turn assigns different parameters to each possible action or state transition in the task, leading to high model complexity. Given that actions or state transitions in problem-solving tasks have correct and incorrect outcomes rather than equivalence relations without quantitative order, this study proposed two action sequence models based on binary logistic modeling with relatively low model complexity: the one- and two-parameter action sequence models (1P and 2P-ASM). Unlike the SRM, which applies the NRM to action sequence analysis, the 1P-ASM and 2P-ASM adapt the simpler one- and two-parameter IRT models to action sequence analysis, respectively. An empirical example was provided to compare the performance of SRM and two ASMs with a real-world computer-based interactive item, “Tickets,” in the PISA 2012, and a simulation study was further conducted to explore the psychometric performance of the proposed models in different test scenarios.

    Figures and Tables | References | Related Articles | Metrics