Acta Psychologica Sinica ›› 2023, Vol. 55 ›› Issue (8): 1255-1269.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2023.01255
• Reports of Empirical Studies • Previous Articles Next Articles
CHEN Peiqi1, ZHANG Yinling1, HU Xinmu1, WANG Jing1,4, MAI Xiaoqin1,2,3()
Received:
2022-09-27
Published:
2023-08-25
Online:
2023-05-16
Contact:
MAI Xiaoqin
E-mail:maixq@ruc.edu.cn
Supported by:
CHEN Peiqi, ZHANG Yinling, HU Xinmu, WANG Jing, MAI Xiaoqin. (2023). The effect of social value orientation on third-party altruistic behaviors in children aged 10-12 years: The role of emotion. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 55(8), 1255-1269.
Variables | Study 1 Third-party punishment (n = 233) | Study 2 Third-party compensation (n = 238) | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
M/% | SD | M/% | SD | |
Gender (male) | 50.6% | - | 50.4% | - |
Age (years old) | 11.42 | 0.84 | 11.43 | 0.87 |
Social value orientation (Pro-self) | 33% | - | 26.5% | - |
Degree of social value orientation | 27.33 | 17.52 | 28.70 | 14.27 |
Emotion (High Inequality) | 2.15 | 1.20 | 2.36 | 1.33 |
Coins for third-party altruism (High Inequality) | 2.68 | 1.60 | 2.77 | 1.38 |
Emotion (Moderate Inequality) | 3.03 | 0.95 | 3.24 | 1.03 |
Coins for third-party altruism (Moderate Inequality) | 1.60 | 1.14 | 1.79 | 1.13 |
Emotion (Equality) | 5.46 | 1.35 | 5.00 | 1.25 |
Coins for third-party altruism (Equality) | 0.24 | 0.85 | 0.47 | 1.14 |
Table 1 Descriptive results
Variables | Study 1 Third-party punishment (n = 233) | Study 2 Third-party compensation (n = 238) | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
M/% | SD | M/% | SD | |
Gender (male) | 50.6% | - | 50.4% | - |
Age (years old) | 11.42 | 0.84 | 11.43 | 0.87 |
Social value orientation (Pro-self) | 33% | - | 26.5% | - |
Degree of social value orientation | 27.33 | 17.52 | 28.70 | 14.27 |
Emotion (High Inequality) | 2.15 | 1.20 | 2.36 | 1.33 |
Coins for third-party altruism (High Inequality) | 2.68 | 1.60 | 2.77 | 1.38 |
Emotion (Moderate Inequality) | 3.03 | 0.95 | 3.24 | 1.03 |
Coins for third-party altruism (Moderate Inequality) | 1.60 | 1.14 | 1.79 | 1.13 |
Emotion (Equality) | 5.46 | 1.35 | 5.00 | 1.25 |
Coins for third-party altruism (Equality) | 0.24 | 0.85 | 0.47 | 1.14 |
Figure 1. Pro-self and Prosocial Children’s Emotions (A) and Coins for Third-party Punishment (B) Note: Error bars indicate standard errors; ? p < 0.1, *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001, same below.
Variables | Study 1 High inequality context | Study 2 High inequality context | Study 1 Moderate inequality context | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
punishment | unhappiness | punishment | compensation | unhappiness | compensation | compensation | unhappiness | compensation | |
Model 1 | Model 2 | Model 3 | Model 4 | Model 5 | Model 6 | Model 7 | Model 8 | Model 9 | |
SVO° | 0.27*** | 0.17** | 0.21*** | 0.50*** | 0.44*** | 0.41*** | 0.40*** | 0.18** | 0.37*** |
unhappiness | 0.34*** | 0.20** | 0.18** | ||||||
Age | -0.15* | -0.20** | -0.08 | -0.10 | -0.20*** | -0.06 | -0.08 | -0.12 | -0.06 |
Gender | -0.27* | -0.12 | -0.23 | -0.03 | 0.14 | -0.05 | -0.00 | 0.03 | -0.01 |
F | 8.29*** | 4.84** | 15.09*** | 26.64*** | 21.71*** | 23.45*** | 14.90*** | 3.62* | 13.64*** |
R2 | 0.10 | 0.06 | 0.21 | 0.25 | 0.22 | 0.29 | 0.16 | 0.04 | 0.19 |
Adjusted R2 | 0.09 | 0.05 | 0.20 | 0.25 | 0.21 | 0.27 | 0.15 | 0.03 | 0.18 |
Table 2 Results of regression analysis (test of the mediating effect of emotions between social value orientation and third-party altruistic behavior
Variables | Study 1 High inequality context | Study 2 High inequality context | Study 1 Moderate inequality context | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
punishment | unhappiness | punishment | compensation | unhappiness | compensation | compensation | unhappiness | compensation | |
Model 1 | Model 2 | Model 3 | Model 4 | Model 5 | Model 6 | Model 7 | Model 8 | Model 9 | |
SVO° | 0.27*** | 0.17** | 0.21*** | 0.50*** | 0.44*** | 0.41*** | 0.40*** | 0.18** | 0.37*** |
unhappiness | 0.34*** | 0.20** | 0.18** | ||||||
Age | -0.15* | -0.20** | -0.08 | -0.10 | -0.20*** | -0.06 | -0.08 | -0.12 | -0.06 |
Gender | -0.27* | -0.12 | -0.23 | -0.03 | 0.14 | -0.05 | -0.00 | 0.03 | -0.01 |
F | 8.29*** | 4.84** | 15.09*** | 26.64*** | 21.71*** | 23.45*** | 14.90*** | 3.62* | 13.64*** |
R2 | 0.10 | 0.06 | 0.21 | 0.25 | 0.22 | 0.29 | 0.16 | 0.04 | 0.19 |
Adjusted R2 | 0.09 | 0.05 | 0.20 | 0.25 | 0.21 | 0.27 | 0.15 | 0.03 | 0.18 |
Variables | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1 | SVO° | ||||||||
2 | Emotion (High Inequality) | -0.136* | |||||||
3 | Coins for Punishment (High Inequality) | 0.241*** | -0.393*** | ||||||
4 | Emotion (Moderate Inequality) | -0.081 | 0.636*** | -0.307*** | |||||
5 | Coins for Punishment (Moderate Inequality) | 0.126 | -0.251*** | 0.668*** | -0.289*** | ||||
6 | Emotion (Equality) | 0.098 | -0.311*** | 0.323*** | -0.174** | 0.212** | |||
7 | Coins for Punishment (Equality) | -0.068 | 0.288*** | 0.049 | 0.179** | 0.263*** | -0.233*** | ||
8 | Age | 0.165* | 0.178** | -0.113 | 0.181** | 0.016 | -0.215** | 0.093 | |
9 | Gender | 0.044 | 0.052 | -0.121 | 0.091 | -0.099 | -0.062 | -0.113 | -0.004 |
Table S1 Results of Correlation Analysis for Study 1
Variables | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1 | SVO° | ||||||||
2 | Emotion (High Inequality) | -0.136* | |||||||
3 | Coins for Punishment (High Inequality) | 0.241*** | -0.393*** | ||||||
4 | Emotion (Moderate Inequality) | -0.081 | 0.636*** | -0.307*** | |||||
5 | Coins for Punishment (Moderate Inequality) | 0.126 | -0.251*** | 0.668*** | -0.289*** | ||||
6 | Emotion (Equality) | 0.098 | -0.311*** | 0.323*** | -0.174** | 0.212** | |||
7 | Coins for Punishment (Equality) | -0.068 | 0.288*** | 0.049 | 0.179** | 0.263*** | -0.233*** | ||
8 | Age | 0.165* | 0.178** | -0.113 | 0.181** | 0.016 | -0.215** | 0.093 | |
9 | Gender | 0.044 | 0.052 | -0.121 | 0.091 | -0.099 | -0.062 | -0.113 | -0.004 |
Effect | Effect Size | SE | LLCI | ULCI |
---|---|---|---|---|
Direct Effect | 0.21 | 0.06 | 0.09 | 0.33 |
Indirect Effect | 0.06 | 0.03 | 0.01 | 0.11 |
Total Effect | 0.27 | 0.06 | 0.15 | 0.40 |
Table S2 Results of the Test for Indirect Effects of Emotions in the High Inequality Context in Study 1
Effect | Effect Size | SE | LLCI | ULCI |
---|---|---|---|---|
Direct Effect | 0.21 | 0.06 | 0.09 | 0.33 |
Indirect Effect | 0.06 | 0.03 | 0.01 | 0.11 |
Total Effect | 0.27 | 0.06 | 0.15 | 0.40 |
Variables | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1 | SVO° | ||||||||
2 | Emotion (High Inequality) | -0.417*** | |||||||
3 | Coins for Compensation (High Inequality) | 0.494*** | -0.385*** | ||||||
4 | Emotion (Moderate Inequality) | -0.172** | 0.563*** | -0.300*** | |||||
5 | Coins for Compensation (Moderate Inequality) | 0.392*** | -0.273** | 0.699*** | -0.245*** | ||||
6 | Emotion (Equality) | 0.206** | -0.296*** | 0.206** | 0.017 | 0.167** | |||
7 | Coins for Compensation (Equality) | 0.167** | -0.317*** | 0.317*** | -0.034 | 0.486*** | -0.002 | ||
8 | Age | 0.065 | 0.241*** | -0.075 | 0.115 | -0.055 | -0.144* | 0.020 | |
9 | Gender | -0.061 | -0.036 | -0.047 | 0.002 | -0.028 | 0.145* | -0.111 | 0.040 |
Table S3 Results of Correlation Analysis for Study 2
Variables | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1 | SVO° | ||||||||
2 | Emotion (High Inequality) | -0.417*** | |||||||
3 | Coins for Compensation (High Inequality) | 0.494*** | -0.385*** | ||||||
4 | Emotion (Moderate Inequality) | -0.172** | 0.563*** | -0.300*** | |||||
5 | Coins for Compensation (Moderate Inequality) | 0.392*** | -0.273** | 0.699*** | -0.245*** | ||||
6 | Emotion (Equality) | 0.206** | -0.296*** | 0.206** | 0.017 | 0.167** | |||
7 | Coins for Compensation (Equality) | 0.167** | -0.317*** | 0.317*** | -0.034 | 0.486*** | -0.002 | ||
8 | Age | 0.065 | 0.241*** | -0.075 | 0.115 | -0.055 | -0.144* | 0.020 | |
9 | Gender | -0.061 | -0.036 | -0.047 | 0.002 | -0.028 | 0.145* | -0.111 | 0.040 |
Context | Effect | Effect Size | SE | LLCI | ULCI |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
High Inequality | Direct Effect | 0.41 | 0.06 | 0.29 | 0.53 |
Indirect Effect | 0.09 | 0.04 | 0.02 | 0.17 | |
Total Effect | 0.50 | 0.06 | 0.39 | 0.61 | |
Moderate Inequality | Direct Effect | 0.37 | 0.06 | 0.25 | 0.48 |
Indirect Effect | 0.03 | 0.03 | -0.003 | 0.09 | |
Total Effect | 0.40 | 0.06 | 0.28 | 0.52 |
Table S4 Results of the Test for Indirect Effects of Emotions in the High Inequality Context in Study 2
Context | Effect | Effect Size | SE | LLCI | ULCI |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
High Inequality | Direct Effect | 0.41 | 0.06 | 0.29 | 0.53 |
Indirect Effect | 0.09 | 0.04 | 0.02 | 0.17 | |
Total Effect | 0.50 | 0.06 | 0.39 | 0.61 | |
Moderate Inequality | Direct Effect | 0.37 | 0.06 | 0.25 | 0.48 |
Indirect Effect | 0.03 | 0.03 | -0.003 | 0.09 | |
Total Effect | 0.40 | 0.06 | 0.28 | 0.52 |
[1] |
Blake, P. R., & McAuliffe, K. (2011). "I had so much it didn't seem fair": Eight-year-olds reject two forms of inequity. Cognition, 120(2), 215-224. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2011.04.006
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2011.04.006 URL pmid: 21616483 |
[2] |
Blake, P. R., McAuliffe, K., Corbit, J., Callaghan, T. C., Barry, O., Bowie, A., ... Warneken, F. (2015). The ontogeny of fairness in seven societies. Nature, 528(7581), 258-+. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature15703
doi: 10.1038/nature15703 URL |
[3] |
Carlsmith, K. M. (2006). The roles of retribution and utility in determining punishment. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42(4), 437-451. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2005.06.007
doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2005.06.007 URL |
[4] |
Carlsmith, K. M., Darley, J. M., & Robinson, P. H. (2002). Why do we punish? Deterrence and just deserts as motives for punishment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(2), 284-299. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.83.2.284
doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.83.2.284 URL pmid: 12150228 |
[5] |
Chavez, A. K., & Bicchieri, C. (2013). Third-party sanctioning and compensation behavior: Findings from the ultimatum game. Journal of Economic Psychology, 39, 268-277. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.joep.2013.09.004
doi: 10.1016/j.joep.2013.09.004 URL |
[6] | Chen, J., Sun, X. Y., & Li, H. (2012). The development of ultimatum game decision-making with complete information in children. Studies of Psychology and Behavior, 10(2), 104-114. (in Chinese) |
[7] |
Chen, S. J., He, Q., & Ma, J. H. (2015). The influence of third-party punishment on cooperation: An explanation of social norm activation. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 47(3), 389-405. (in Chinese)
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2015.00389 |
[8] |
Civai, C., Corradi-Dell'Acqua, C., Gamer, M., & Rumiati, R. I. (2010). Are irrational reactions to unfairness truly emotionally-driven? dissociated behavioral and emotional responses in the ultimatum game task. Cognition, 114(1), 89-95. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.09.001
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.09.001 URL |
[9] |
Dhaliwal, N. A., Patil, I., & Cushman, F. (2021). Reputational and cooperative benefits of third-party compensation. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 164, 27-51. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2021.01.003
doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2021.01.003 URL |
[10] |
Edwards, J. R., & Lambert, L. S. (2007). Methods for integrating moderation and mediation: A general analytical framework using moderated path analysis. Psychological Methods, 12(1), 1-22. https://doi.org/10.1037/1082-989x.12.1.1
doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.12.1.1 URL pmid: 17402809 |
[11] | Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., & Spinard, T. L. (2006). Handbook of child psychology: Prosocial development. New York, NY: Wiley. |
[12] | Fang, X. M., & Chen, S. (2011). The influence of emotion and social value orientations on distributional fairness. Soft Science, 25(12), 129-132. (in Chinese) |
[13] |
Fehr, E., & Fischbacher, U. (2004). Third-party punishment and social norms. Evolution and Human Behavior, 25(2), 63-87. https://doi.org/10.1016/s1090-5138(04)00005-4
doi: 10.1016/S1090-5138(04)00005-4 URL |
[14] |
Fehr, E., & Gächter, S. (2002). Altruistic punishment in humans. Nature, 415(6868), 137-140. https://doi.org/10.1038/415137a
doi: 10.1038/415137a URL |
[15] |
Fehr, E., & Schmidt, K. M. (1999). A theory of fairness, competition, and cooperation. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 114(3), 817-868. https://doi.org/10.1162/003355399556151
doi: 10.1162/003355399556151 URL |
[16] |
Ginther, M. R., Bonnie, R. J., Hoffman, M. B., Shen, F. X., Simons, K. W., Jones, O. D., & Marois, R. (2016). Parsing the behavioral and brain mechanisms of third-party punishment. Journal of Neuroscience, 36(36), 9420-9434. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.4499-15.2016
doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4499-15.2016 URL pmid: 27605616 |
[17] |
Guitart-Masip, M., Duzel, E., Dolan, R., & Dayan, P. (2014). Action versus valence in decision making. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(4), 194-202. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2014.01.003
doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2014.01.003 URL pmid: 24581556 |
[18] |
Gummerum, M., & Chu, M. T. (2014). Outcomes and intentions in children's, adolescents', and adults' second- and third-party punishment behavior. Cognition, 133(1), 97-103. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2014.06.001
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.06.001 URL pmid: 24997554 |
[19] |
Gummerum, M., López-Pérez, B., van Dijk, E., & van Dillen, L. F. (2020). When punishment is emotion-driven: Children's, adolescents', and adults' costly punishment of unfair allocations. Social Development, 29(1), 126-142. https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12387
doi: 10.1111/sode.v29.1 URL |
[20] |
Gummerum, M., van Dillen, L. F., van Dijk, E., & Lopez-Perez, B. (2016). Costly third-party interventions: The role of incidental anger and attention focus in punishment of the perpetrator and compensation of the victim. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 65, 94-104. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2016.04.004
doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2016.04.004 URL |
[21] |
Guo, Q. K., Xu, P., Wu, R., & Hu, S. S. (2016). Effects of in-group favoritism and grade on the altruistic punishment behavior of primary school students. Psychological Development and Education, 32(4), 402-408. doi:10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2016.04.03 (in Chinese)
doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2016.04.03 |
[22] |
Hamlin, J. K., Wynn, K., Bloom, P., & Mahajan, N. (2011). How infants and toddlers react to antisocial others. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108(50), 19931-19936. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1110306108
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1110306108 URL pmid: 22123953 |
[23] | Hao, J., Yang, Y., & Wang, Z. W. (2016). Face-to-face sharing with strangers and altruistic punishment of acquaintances for strangers: Young adolescents exhibit greater altruism than adults. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 9, Article 1512. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01512 |
[24] |
Haruno, M., & Frith, C. D. (2010). Activity in the amygdala elicited by unfair divisions predicts social value orientation. Nature Neuroscience, 13(2), 160-161. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2468
doi: 10.1038/nn.2468 URL pmid: 20023652 |
[25] |
Hayes, A. F., Montoya, A. K., & Rockwood, N. J. (2017). The analysis of mechanisms and their contingencies: PROCESS versus structural equation modeling. Australasian Marketing Journal, 25(1), 76-81. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ausmj.2017.02.001
doi: 10.1016/j.ausmj.2017.02.001 URL |
[26] |
Hong, H. F., Kou, Y., & Wu, J. H. (2012). Fairness decision-making of college students in social dilemmas: Effect of social value orientation. Psychological Development and Education, 28(5), 487-494 doi:10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2012.05.004 (in Chinese)
doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2012.05.004 |
[27] | Hu, Y., Scheele, D., Becker, B., Voos, G., David, B., Hurlemann, R., & Weber, B. (2016). The effect of oxytocin on third-party altruistic decisions in unfair situations: An fMRI study. Scientific Reports, 6, 10, Article 20236. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep20236 |
[28] | Hu, Y., Strang, S., & Weber, B. (2015). Helping or punishing strangers: Neural correlates of altruistic decisions as third-party and of its relation to empathic concern. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 9, 11, Article 24. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00024 |
[29] |
Jordan, J. J., Hoffman, M., Bloom, P., & Rand, D. G. (2016). Third-party punishment as a costly signal of trustworthiness. Nature, 530(7591), 473-476. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature16981
doi: 10.1038/nature16981 URL |
[30] |
Karagonlar, G., & Kuhlman, D. M. (2013). The role of social value orientation in response to an unfair offer in the ultimatum game. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 120(2), 228-239. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2012.07.006
doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2012.07.006 URL |
[31] |
Kohlberg, L., & Kramer, R. (1969). Continuities and discontinuities in childhood and adult moral development. Human development, 12(2), 93-120. https://doi.org/10.1159/000270857
URL pmid: 5791921 |
[32] | Lee, Y.-E., Marshall, J., Deutchman, P., McAuliffe, K., & Warneken, F. (2022). Children's judgments of interventions against norm violations: COVID-19 as a naturalistic case study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 221, 16, Article 105452. https://doi.org/ 10.1016/j.jecp.2022.105452 |
[33] | Lee, Y.-E., & Warneken, F. (2020). Children's evaluations of third-party responses to unfairness: Children prefer helping over punishment. Cognition, 205, 14, Article 104374. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104374 |
[34] |
Leliveld, M. C., Vandijk, E., & Vanbeest, I. (2012). Punishing and compensating others at your own expense: The role of empathic concern on reactions to distributive injustice. European Journal of Social Psychology, 42(2), 135-140. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.872
doi: 10.1002/ejsp.v42.2 URL |
[35] |
Li, J., Zhu, L. Q., Gummerum, M., & Sun, Y. L. (2013). The development of social value orientation across different contexts. International Journal of Psychology, 48(4), 469-480. https://doi.org/10.1080/00207594.2012.673725
doi: 10.1080/00207594.2012.673725 URL pmid: 22551355 |
[36] |
Liang, F. C., Wang, X. Y., & Tang, W. H. (2015). The development of children's fair behavior in different distribution situations. Psychological Development and Education, 31(6), 648-653. doi:10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2015.06.02 (in Chinese)
doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2015.06.02 |
[37] |
Ma, Y., Fang, P., & Jiang, Y. (2011). Research on the relationship between affective forecasting bias and decision making in adolescents. Journal of Psychological Science. 34(4), 852-855. doi:10.16719/j.cnki.1671-6981.2011.04.022. (in Chinese)
doi: 10.16719/j.cnki.1671-6981.2011.04.022 |
[38] | McAuliffe, K., Blake, P. R., Steinbeis, N., & Warneken, F. (2017). The developmental foundations of human fairness. Nature Human Behaviour, 1(2), 9, Article 0042. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-016-0042 |
[39] |
McAuliffe, K., Jordan, J. J., & Warneken, F. (2015). Costly third-party punishment in young children. Cognition, 134, 1-10. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2014.08.013
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.08.013 URL pmid: 25460374 |
[40] | Meristo, M., & Surian, L. (2014). Infants distinguish antisocial actions directed towards fair and unfair agents. Plos One, 9(10), 7, Article e110553. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0110553 |
[41] | Mu, Y., & Su, Y. J. (2005). Relationship between peer acceptance and social strategies in 10-12 years old children. Psychological Development and Education, 21(2), 24-29. (in Chinese) |
[42] |
Murnighan, J. K., & Saxon, M. S. (1998). Ultimatum bargaining by children and adults. Journal of Economic Psychology, 19(4), 415-445. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0167-4870(98)00017-8
doi: 10.1016/S0167-4870(98)00017-8 URL |
[43] |
Murphy, R. O., Ackermann, K. A., & Handgraaf, M. J. J. (2011). Measuring social value orientation. Judgment and Decision Making, 6(8), 771-781.
doi: 10.1017/S1930297500004204 URL |
[44] |
Nowak, M. A. (2006). Five rules for the evolution of cooperation. Science, 314(5805), 1560-1563. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1133755
doi: 10.1126/science.1133755 URL pmid: 17158317 |
[45] |
Pillutla, M. M., & Murnighan, J. K. (1996). Unfairness, anger, and spite: Emotional rejections of ultimatum offers. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 68(3), 208-224. https://doi.org/10.1006/obhd.1996.0100
doi: 10.1006/obhd.1996.0100 URL |
[46] |
Qi, Y. Y., Wu, H. Y., & Liu, X. (2017). The influences of social value orientation on prosocial behaviors: The evidences from behavioral and neuroimaging studies. Chinese Science Bulletin, 62(11), 1136-1144. doi:10.1360/N972016-00631 (in Chinese)
doi: 10.1360/N972016-00631 |
[47] |
Raihani, N. J., & Bshary, R. (2015). Third-party punishers are rewarded, but third-party helpers even more so. Evolution, 69(4), 993-1003. https://doi.org/10.1111/evo.12637
doi: 10.1111/evo.12637 URL pmid: 25756463 |
[48] |
Reno, R. R., Cialdini, R. B. & Kallgren, C. A. (1993). The transsituational influence of social norms. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 104-112. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.64.1.104
doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.64.1.104 URL |
[49] |
Sanfey, A. G., Rilling, J. K., Aronson, J. A., Nystrom, L. E., & Cohen, J. D. (2003). The neural basis of economic decision-making in the ultimatum game. Science, 300(5626), 1755-1758. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1082976
doi: 10.1126/science.1082976 URL pmid: 12805551 |
[50] | Schwartz, S. H. (1977). Normative influence on altruism. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 10, pp. 221-279). New York: Academic Press. |
[51] |
Stallen, M., Rossi, F., Heijne, A., Smidts, A., de Dreu, C. K. W., & Sanfey, A. G. (2018). Neurobiological mechanisms of responding to injustice. Journal of Neuroscience, 38(12), 2944-2954.
doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1242-17.2018 URL pmid: 29459373 |
[52] |
Vaish, A., Missana, M., & Tomasello, M. (2011). Three-year-old children intervene in third-party moral transgressions. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 29(1), 124-130. https://doi.org/10.1348/026151010x532888
doi: 10.1348/026151010X532888 URL |
[53] |
van Doorn, J., Zeelenberg, M., & Breugelmans, S. M. (2018). An exploration of third parties' preference for compensation over punishment: Six experimental demonstrations. Theory and Decision, 85(3-4), 333-351. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11238-018-9665-9
doi: 10.1007/s11238-018-9665-9 URL |
[54] |
van Lange, P. A. M., Otten, W., de Bruin, E. M. N., & Joireman, J. A. (1997). Development of prosocial, individualistic, and competitive orientations: Theory and preliminary evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73(4), 733-746. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.73.4.733
URL pmid: 9325591 |
[55] |
Wenzel, M., & Thielmann, I. (2006). Why we punish in the name of justice: Just desert versus value restoration and the role of social identity. Social Justice Research, 19(4), 450-470.
doi: 10.1007/s11211-006-0028-2 URL |
[56] | Wu, C. H. (2018). The social value orientation of students in the period of compulsory education - A survey in Shanghai, Hangzhou and Hefei [Unpublished master’s thesis]. East China Normal University. |
[57] |
Wu, J. H., Luan, S. H., & Raihani, N. (2022). Reward, punishment, and prosocial behavior: Recent developments and implications. Current Opinion in Psychology, 44, 117-123. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2021.09.003
doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2021.09.003 URL |
[58] |
Xie, E. H., Liu, M. D., Liu, J. Q., Gao, X. X., & Li, X. C. (2022). Neural mechanisms of the mood effects on third-party responses to injustice after unfair experiences. Human Brain Mapping, 43(12), 3646-3661. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.25874
doi: 10.1002/hbm.v43.12 URL |
[59] | Zhang, W. N. (2011). 2-12 years old Chinese children’s emotional words usage of research [Unpublished master’s thesis]. Liaoning Normal University, China. |
[60] |
Zhang, Z., Zhang, F., Huang, L., Yuan, B., & Wang, Y. W. (2014). Theories and measurement methods of social value orientation related to decision making. Advances in Psychological Science, 22(1), 48-56. (in Chinese)
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2014.00048 |
Viewed | ||||||
Full text |
|
|||||
Abstract |
|
|||||