Acta Psychologica Sinica ›› 2025, Vol. 57 ›› Issue (1): 36-57.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2025.0036
• Reports of Empirical Studies • Previous Articles Next Articles
LIN Shuang1,2,3, CHENG Gonglu3, LIU Wen2, BAI Xuejun3
Received:
2023-10-22
Published:
2025-01-20
Online:
2024-11-20
LIN Shuang, CHENG Gonglu, LIU Wen, BAI Xuejun. (2025). The effect of cognitive control on displaced aggressive behavior in junior high school students: Situation boundary conditions of provocation and trigger. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 57(1), 36-57.
Add to citation manager EndNote|Ris|BibTeX
URL: https://journal.psych.ac.cn/acps/EN/10.3724/SP.J.1041.2025.0036
[1] Aviles F., Earleywine M., Pollock V., Stratton J., & Miller N. (2005). Alcohol's effect on triggered displaced aggression. [2] Barlett C. P., Helmstetter K. M., Kowalewski D. A., & Pezzillo L. (2017). Piecing together the aggression puzzle: Testing the mediating variables linking early to later aggression. [3] Benedek, M., & Zöhrer, L. (2020). Creativity on tap 2: Investigating dose effects of alcohol on cognitive control and creative cognition. [4] Braver, T. S., & Barch, D. M. (2002). A theory of cognitive control, aging cognition, and neuromodulation. [5] Braver T. S., Kizhner A., Tang R., Freund M. C., & Etzel J. A. (2021). The dual mechanisms of cognitive control project. [6] Beames J. R., Gilam G., Schofield T. P., Schira M. M., & Denson T. F. (2020). The impact of self-control training on neural responses following anger provocation. [7] Bushman, B. J., & Baumeister, R. F. (1998). Threatened egotism, narcissism, self-esteem, and direct and displaced aggression: Does self-love or self-hate lead to violence? [8] Chester D. S., Eisenberger N. I., Pond R. S., Jr., Richman S. B., Bushman B. J., & DeWall C. N. (2014). The interactive effect of social pain and executive functioning on aggression: An fMRI experiment. [9] Denson T. F., Pedersen W. C., & Miller N. (2006). The displaced aggression questionnaire. [10] Dillon K. H., Allan N. P., Cougle J. R., & Fincham F. D. (2016). Measuring hostile interpretation bias: The WSAP- Hostility Scale. [11] Espy, K. A. (2004). Using developmental, cognitive, and neuroscience approaches to understand executive control in young children. [12] Finkel, E. J., & Hall, A. N. (2018). The I3 Model: A metatheoretical framework for understanding aggression. [13] Garcia-Sancho E., Martin S. J., Vasquez E. A., & Fernandez-Berrocal P. (2016). Validity and reliability of the Spanish version of the Displaced Aggression Questionnaire. [14] Gonthier C., Zira M., Cole P., & Blaye A. (2019). Evidencing the developmental shift from reactive to proactive control in early childhood and its relationship to working memory. [15] Gu C., Peng Y., Nastase S. A., Mayer R. E., & Li P. (2024). Onscreen presence of instructors in video lectures affects learners' neural synchrony and visual attention during multimedia learning. [16] Hsieh, I., & Chen, Y. Y. (2017). Determinants of aggressive behavior: Interactive effects of emotional regulation and inhibitory control. [17] Huang S., Zhu Z., Zhang W., Chen Y., & Zhen S. (2017). Trait impulsivity components correlate differently with proactive and reactive control. [18] Jiang S., Meng Y., & Chen B. (2024). The impact of emotional states on bilingual language control in cued and voluntary switching contexts. [19] Johnson, R. R. (2017). Suspect demeanor and arrest: A triggered displacement of aggression explanation. [20] Kim E., Shin Y., & Kim J. (2019). Conflicts between work and family and displaced aggression with working parents in South Korea: An exploratory study. [21] Lawrence, C. (2006). Measuring individual responses to aggression-triggering events: Development of the situational triggers of aggressive responses (STAR) scale. [22] Leander, N. P., & Chartrand, T. L. (2017). On thwarted goals and displaced aggression: A compensatory competence model. [23] Li, J. (2013). The mechanism of why self-control resources and cognitive resources influence each other: An integrated model. [黎建斌. (2013). 自我控制资源与认知资源相互影响的机制: 整合模型. [24] Li Q., Nan W., Wang K., & Liu X. (2014). Independent processing of stimulus-stimulus and stimulus-response conflicts. [25] Li Z., Yang G., Wu H., Li Q., Xu H., Göschl F., Nolte G., & Liu X. (2021). Modality-specific neural mechanisms of cognitive control in a Stroop-like task. [26] Lin S., Cheng G., Sun S., Feng M., & Bai X. (2024a). The effect of bystander features on displaced Aggression in Provocative Situations among Male Juvenile Delinquents.Behavioral Sciences, 14(6), 496. [27] Lin S., Cheng G., Sun S., Feng M., & Bai X. (2024b). Emotional regulation of displaced aggression in provocative situations among junior high school students. [28] Liu Y., Wang M., Chang C., Shi J., Zhou L., & Shao R. (2015). Work-family conflict, emotional exhaustion, and displaced aggression toward others: The moderating roles of workplace interpersonal conflict and perceived managerial family support. [29] Liu Y., Zhou B., & Yang B. (2022). How does emotion shape aggressive behavior of violent offenders? An explanation based on emotion regulation theory. [刘宇平, 周冰涛, 杨波. (2022). 情绪如何引发暴力犯的攻击? 基于情绪调节理论的解释. [30] Loeffler L., Satterthwaite T. D., Habel U., Schneider F., Radke S., & Derntl B. (2019). Attention control and its emotion-specific association with cognitive emotion regulation in depression. [31] Luo Y., Zhang D., Liu Y., & Liu Y. (2011). Reliability and validity of the Chinese version of trait Anger Scale applied to college students. [罗亚莉, 张大均, 刘云波, 刘衍玲. (2011). 特质愤怒量表中文版在大学生人群应用的信度和效度. [32] Ma Y., Xiao C., Che J., Wang H., & Li A. (2020). Ego depletion impedes rational decision making: Mechanisms and boundary conditions. [马钰, 肖晨洁, 车敬上, 王海侠, 李爱梅. (2020). 自我损耗降低决策理性: 心理机制与边界条件. [33] Marcus-Newhall A., Pedersen W. C., Carlson M., & Miller N. (2000). Displaced aggression is alive and well: A meta-analytic review. [34] Miller N., Pedersen W. C., Earleywine M., & Pollock V. E. (2003). Artificial a theoretical model of triggered displaced aggression. [35] Pedersen W. C., Bushman B. J., Vasquez E. A., & Miller N. (2008). Kicking the (barking) dog effect: The moderating role of target attributes on triggered displaced aggression. [36] Qiu L., Zheng X., & Wang Y. (2008). Revision of the Positive Affect and Negative Affect Scale. [邱林, 郑雪, 王雁飞. (2008). 积极情感消极情感量表(PANAS)的修订. [37] Quan, F. (2019). [权方英. (2019). [38] Rajchert J., Konopka K., & Huesmann L. (2017). It is more than thought that counts: The role of readiness for aggression in the relationship between ostracism and displaced aggression. [39] Rajchert J., Konopka K., Oreziak H., & Dziechciarska W. (2022). Direct and displaced aggression after exclusion: role of gender differences. [40] Reijntjes A., Thomaes S., Kamphuis J. H., Bushman B. J., Reitz E., & Telch M. J. (2013). Youths' displaced aggression against in- and out-group peers: An experimental examination. [41] Salguero J. M., García-Sancho E., Ramos-Cejudo J., & Kannis-Dymand L. (2020). Individual differences in anger and displaced aggression: The role of metacognitive beliefs and anger rumination. [42] Scott J. P., DiLillo D., Maldonado R. C., & Watkins L. E. (2015). Negative urgency and emotion regulation strategy use: Associations with displaced aggression. [43] Skvarc D. R., Harries T., Harris N., Hyder S., Patafio B., Howard D., Mayshak R. (2023). Cognitive control moderates the maturation of relational aggression in adults. [44] Slotter E. B., Grom J. L., & Tervo-Clemmens B. (2020). Don't take it out on me: Displaced aggression after provocation by a romantic partner as a function of attachment anxiety and self-control. [45] Smits F. M., Geuze E., Schutter D. J. L. G., van Honk J., & Gladwin T. E. (2022). Effects of tDCS during inhibitory control training on performance and PTSD, aggression and anxiety symptoms: A randomized-controlled trial in a military sample. [46] Song P., Zhang Z., Wang B., David N., Zhao H., Wang Q., Xiao Y., & Yang B. (2018). The influence of trait empathy on reactive aggression: An ERP study. [47] Soto, B. (2016). [48] Spielberger C. D., Reheiser E. C., & Sydeman S. J. (1995). Measuring the experience, expression, and control of anger. [49] Su, S., & Xia, L. (2020). Reliability and validity analysis of Chinese version of displaced aggression questionnaire in college students. [苏姝, 夏凌翔. (2020). 替代性攻击问卷中文版在大学生中的信效度分析. [50] Szczepocka E., Mokros L., Kaźimierski J., Nowakowska K., Lucka A., Antoszczyk A., Bagger K. (2024). Virtual reality-based training may improve visual memory and some aspects of sustained attention among healthy older adults-preliminary results of a randomized controlled study. [51] Tan, S., & Guo, Y. (2008). Revision of self-control scale for college students.Chinese Journal of Clinical Psychology, 5, 468-470. [谭树华, 郭永玉. (2008). 大学生自我控制量表的修订. [52] Tangney J. P., Baumeister R. F., & Boone A. L. (2004). High self-control predicts good adjustment, less pathology, better grades, and interpersonal success. [53] Van Gerven P. W., Hurks P. P., Bovend'Eerdt T. J., & Adam J. J. (2016). Switch hands! Mapping proactive and reactive cognitive control across the life span. [54] Vasquez E. A., Denson T. F., Pedersen W. C., Stenstrom D. M., & Miller N. (2005). The moderating effect of trigger intensity on triggered displaced aggression. [55] Vasquez, E. A., & Howard-Field, J. (2016). Too (mentally) busy to chill: Cognitive load and inhibitory cues interact to moderate triggered displaced aggression. [56] Vasquez E. A., Pedersen W. C., Bushman B. J., Kelley N. J., Demeestere P., & Miller N. (2013). Lashing out after stewing over public insults: The effects of public provocation, provocation intensity, and rumination on triggered displaced aggression. [57] Wang C., Li X., & Xia L. (2023). Long-term effect of cybervictimization on displaced aggressive behavior across two years: Mutually predicting mediators of hostile emotion and moral disengagement. [58] Wang H., Zhu W., & Xia L. (2022). Brain structural correlates of aggression types from the perspective of disinhibition-control: A voxel-based morphometric study. [59] Wang Y., She Y., Colarelli S. M., Fang Y., Meng H., Chen Q., Zhang X., & Zhu H. (2018). Exposure to nature counteracts aggression after depletion. [60] Wang Y., Yang Q., Zhu B., Ye S., Tian X., & Krueger F. (2020). High levels of psychopathic traits increase the risk of transferring reactive aggression to innocent people after provocation: Evidence from an ERP study. [61] Warburton, W. A., & Bushman, B. J. (2019). The competitive reaction time task: The development and scientific utility of a flexible laboratory aggression paradigm. [62] Watson D., Clark L. A., & Tellegen A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS Scales. [63] Weidler C., Habel U., Wallheinke P., Wagels L., Hofhansel L., Ling S., Blendy J. A., & Clemens B. (2022). Consequences of prefrontal tDCS on inhibitory control and reactive aggression. [64] West S. J., Lasko E. N., Hall C. J., Khan N. G., & Chester D. S. (2021). Some revenge now or more revenge later? Applying an intertemporal framework to retaliatory aggression. [65] Wilkowski B. M., Crowe S. E., & Ferguson E. L. (2014). Learning to keep your cool: Reducing aggression through the experimental modification of cognitive control. [66] Xiao, J. (2020). Research on the orientation of special schools from the perspective of law's rule.Journal of Juvenile Delinquency, 1, 6-12. [肖建国. (2020). 法治视角下的专门学校办学定位探究. [67] Yang G., Li Z., Wu H., & Liu X. (2019). Generality and specificity of cognitive control: Research logics and debates. [杨国春, 李政汉, 伍海燕, 刘勋. (2019). 认知控制的一般性/特异性机制: 研究逻辑和争论. [68] Zhang R., Stock A. K., Rzepus A., & Beste C. (2017). Self-regulatory capacities are depleted in a domain-specific manner. [69] Zhao Q., Wang Z., Yang C., Chen H., Zhang Y., Zeb I., Qiu M. (2024). Anxiety symptoms without depression are associated with cognitive control network (CNN) dysfunction: An fNIRS study. [70] Zhuang Q., Xu L., Zhou F., Yao S., Zheng X., Zhou X., Becker B. (2021). Segregating domain-general from emotional context-specific inhibitory control systems-ventral striatum and orbitofrontal cortex serve as emotion-cognition integration hubs. |
[1] | PAN Yuean, JIANG Yunpeng, GUO Maojie, WU Xia. The influence of uncertainty and validity of expectation on the perceptual decision of motion direction [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2022, 54(6): 595-603. |
[2] | CHEN Li, SHI Xiao-ke, LI Wei-na, HU Yan. Influence of cognitive control based on different conflict levels on the expression of gender stereotypes [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2022, 54(6): 628-645. |
[3] | LI Jianhua, XIE Jiajia, ZHUANG Jin-Ying. An effect of menstrual cycle phase on episodic memory [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2022, 54(5): 466-480. |
[4] | WU Jianxiao, CAO Bihua, CHEN Yun, LI Zixia, LI Fuhong. Hierarchical control in task switching: Electrophysiological evidence [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2022, 54(10): 1167-1180. |
[5] | ZHANG Mengke, LI Qing, YIN Shouhang, CHEN Antao. Changes in the level of conflict trigger conflict adaptation [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2021, 53(2): 128-138. |
[6] | HUANG Yuesheng, ZHANG Bao, FAN Xinhua, HUANG Jie. Can negative emotion of task-irrelevant working memory representation affect its attentional capture? A study of eye movements [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2021, 53(1): 26-37. |
[7] | SUN Yan, LV Jiaojiao, LAN Fan, ZHANG Lina. Emotion regulation strategy of self-focused and situation-focused reappraisal and their impact on subsequent cognitive control [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2020, 52(12): 1393-1406. |
[8] | CUI Yichen, WANG Pei, CUI Yajuan. Cognitive control strategies from the perspective of perceptual conflict: An example of stereotyped information and counterstereotyped information [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2019, 51(10): 1157-1170. |
[9] | WANG Yanqing, CHEN Antao, HU Xueping, YIN Shouhang. Reward improves cognitive control by enhancing signal monitoring [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2019, 51(1): 48-57. |
[10] | HU Cenlou, ZHANG Bao, HUANG Sai. Does irrelevant long-term memory representation guide the deployment of visual attention? [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2017, 49(5): 590-601. |
[11] | ZHANG Bao, HU Cenlou, Huang Sai. What do eye movements reveal about the role of cognitive control in attention guidance from working memory representation [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2016, 48(9): 1105-1118. |
[12] | LIU Cong, JIAO Lu, SUN Xun, WANG Ruiming. Immediate effect of language switch on non-proficient bilinguals’ cognitive control components [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2016, 48(5): 472-481. |
[13] | WANG Jiaying, JIAO Runkai, ZHANG Ming. The mechanism of the effect of task setting on negative compatibility effect: The effect of top-down cognition control on subliminal prime processing [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2016, 48(11): 1370-1378. |
[14] | LIU Xiaoyu; HE Chaodan; CHEN Jun; DENG Qinli. The Bilingual Cognitive Control Mechanism of Highly Proficient Cantonese-Mandarin Speakers: Evidence from A Dual-task Switching Paradigm [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2015, 47(4): 439-454. |
[15] | DOU Weiwei;ZHENG Xifu;YANG Huifang;WANG Junfang;LI Yue;E Xiaotian;Chen Qianqian. The Effect of Cognitive Distraction’s Intensity on the Process of Trauma-related Information: Evidence from ERP [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2014, 46(5): 656-665. |
Viewed | ||||||
Full text |
|
|||||
Abstract |
|
|||||