Loading...
ISSN 0439-755X
CN 11-1911/B
主办:中国心理学会
   中国科学院心理研究所
出版:科学出版社

Archive

    25 October 2022, Volume 54 Issue 10 Previous Issue    Next Issue

    Reports of Empirical Studies
    For Selected: Toggle Thumbnails
    Reports of Empirical Studies
    The role of text familiarity in Chinese word segmentation and Chinese vocabulary recognition
    CHEN Mingjing, WANG Yongsheng, ZHAO Bingjie, LI Xin, BAI Xuejun
    2022, 54 (10):  1151-1166.  doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2022.01151
    Abstract ( 4126 )   HTML ( 322 )  
    PDF (831KB) ( 3632 )   Peer Review Comments

    In alphabetic writing systems (such as English), the spaces between words mark the word boundaries, and the basic unit of reading is distinguished during visual-level processing. The visual-level information of word boundaries facilitates reading. Chinese is an ideographic language whose text contains no intrinsic inter-word spaces as the marker of word boundaries. Previous studies have shown that the basic processing unit of Chinese reading is also a word. However, findings remain inconsistent regarding whether inserting spaces between words in Chinese text promotes reading performance. Researchers have proposed that there may be a trade-off between text familiarity and the facilitation effect of inter-word spaces.

    The purpose of Experiment 1 was to examine whether there was trade-off between text familiarity and facilitation of inter-word spaces. Before reading training, Experiment 1 was conducted that 40 native Chinese undergraduates read Chinese sentences from right to left on four text conditions. The results showed faster reading speed and shorter total reading time for the inter-word spaced text. Based on this finding, 40 native Chinese undergraduates who did not participate in the first stage read Chinese sentences from right to left on four text conditions after ten-day reading training, then, the eye tracking data of participants during Chinese reading were recorded in Experiment 1. Experiment 1 verified there was trade-off between text familiarity and inter-word spaces’ facilitation in Chinese, then, the Experiment 2 examined the role of text familiarity and word frequency in vocabulary recognition. Forty students read Chinese sentences under familiar (from left to right) and unfamiliar (from right to left) texts. The target words were high frequency or low frequency. Using Eyelink 1000, the eye tracking data of 32 undergraduates during Chinese reading were recorded in Experiment 2. Second, right-to-left reading training was conducted over 10 days to improve right-to-left reading experience. Then, the eye tracking data of participants during Chinese reading were recorded in Experiment 2.

    The results in Experiment 1 showed that: (1) Before training, there was significant different between the total reading time and reading speed under unfamiliarity text, which were shorter reading time and faster reading speed in the inter-word spaced text. (2) After training, there was no significant difference between the total reading time and reading speed in the inter-word spaced text and unspaced text, which suggests that the facilitation effect of inter-word spaces in Chinese reading changed smaller. The results in Experiment 2 showed that: (1) The effect of text familiarity was significant. The fixated time was shorter, and the skipping rate was higher under the familiar text. (2) The main effect of word frequency was significant. Low-frequency words had longer fixation times and a higher skipping rate. (3) Right-to-left training improves reading performance from right to left. (4) The early indexes showed a significant interaction between text familiarity and word frequency. A word-frequency effect occurred under the familiar but not under the unfamiliar texts. The late indexes showed that the interaction between text familiarity and word-frequency was not significant.

    Results in experiment 1 suggested that there was trade-off between text familiarity and the facilitation of inter-word spaces, which supported the assumption in previous studies. In addition, results in experiment 2 showed the text familiarity may affect the early processing in vocabulary recognition. Based on the Chinese integrated reading model, the word segmentation and vocabulary recognition are unified processing. The E-Z reader model holds the opposite point. Combine the experiment 1 and experiment 2, research showed that word segmentation and vocabulary recognition may be sequential processing in Chinese reading, which provided empirical evidence for Chinese E-Z reader model rather than Chinese integrated model.

    Figures and Tables | References | Related Articles | Metrics
    Hierarchical control in task switching: Electrophysiological evidence
    WU Jianxiao, CAO Bihua, CHEN Yun, LI Zixia, LI Fuhong
    2022, 54 (10):  1167-1180.  doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2022.01167
    Abstract ( 2062 )   HTML ( 194 )  
    PDF (1681KB) ( 2341 )   Peer Review Comments

    The task-switching paradigm is one of the leading research paradigms that is widely used to explore cognitive control. Previous studies have shown that switch costs are greater for high hierarchical tasks than for low hierarchical tasks, and a number of ERP studies on rule structure learning, rule switching, task complexity, and asymmetric task switching have coherently found that N2, P3, and late components are associated with the hierarchical control process. For example, Lu et al. (2017) designed three levels of tasks, but were not concerned with switch costs. Li et al. (2019) also designed three levels of tasks, but were concerned with asymmetric switch costs. The other two studies focused on stimulus or rule switching without concern for task switching. However, to date, no study has clearly addressed the ERP correlates of hierarchical effects in task-switching.

    A nested cue-task switching paradigm was used to investigate the brain responses associated with different hierarchical effects in task switching. Participants were asked to perform two hierarchical tasks. In the low hierarchical task, participants judged digits (1-9, except 5) as large/small or odd/even, respectively. In the high hierarchical task, participants identified the semantic features of the presented digits (e.g., whether the digit was an even number) before they performed the low hierarchical task (e.g., the large/small task). For example, participants first identified whether the current number was a large digit (i.e., greater than five) and then made an odd/even judgment on it. If the current number was not greater than five, then they did not respond (no-go trials). The proportion of no-go trials was 16%, and the no-go and subsequent go trials were excluded from data analysis. Thirty Chinese students (15 males) participated in the EEG experiment. They were asked to press the “F” key for odd or large numbers and the “J” key for even or small numbers. The links between the attributes of the cues and response keys were counterbalanced between participants.

    Behavioral results showed that the RT was longer for the high hierarchical trials than for the low hierarchical trials, indicating that the high hierarchical task was more complex than the low hierarchical task. Furthermore, there was a significant interaction between transition type and hierarchical level, with greater switch costs occurring in the high hierarchical task than in the low hierarchical task, indicating that switching in a low hierarchical task is easier than in a high hierarchical task. Cue-locked ERP results showed that the main effect of the hierarchical level was significant in P2, with higher P2 amplitudes for the high hierarchical trials than for the low hierarchical trials. A significant main effect of transition type was found in the CNV, with higher CNV amplitudes for the high hierarchical trials than for the low hierarchical trials, and there was a significant interaction between transition type and hierarchical level. Further analysis of this interaction revealed that task-switching trials elicited larger CNV amplitudes than task-repeating trials in the high hierarchical task, but not in the low hierarchical task. The target-locked ERP results showed that the main effect of transition type was significant for N2, P3, and SP. The difference in N2 and SP amplitudes between high hierarchical task switching and task repetition was significantly greater than between low hierarchical task switching and task repetition.

    The purpose of the present study was to explore the ERP correlates of hierarchical effects in task-switching. The behavioral results replicated previous findings. Cue-locked ERP results indicated that the hierarchical effect first appeared in the P2 component and that the switch effect on the CNV component was modulated by the task hierarchy, reflecting more selective attention given to high hierarchical tasks and higher proactive control during the task-set reconfiguration stage. The target-locked ERP results indicated that task switching induced more negative N2 amplitudes and smaller P3 and SP amplitudes compared to task repetition. The difference wave amplitudes between high hierarchical task switching and repetition were significantly greater for the N2 and SP amplitudes than for the low hierarchical task, reflecting that the process of inhibiting the old task-set and reconfiguring the new response set is more complex, resulting in increased reactive control. These findings provide new evidence for the task-set reconfiguration theory and the hierarchical nature of cognitive control.

    Figures and Tables | References | Related Articles | Metrics
    Relationship between empathy and emotion recognition in Chinese national music: An event-related potential study evidence
    YANG Jimei, CHAI Jieyu, QIU Tianlong, QUAN Xiaoshan, ZHENG Maoping
    2022, 54 (10):  1181-1192.  doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2022.01181
    Abstract ( 3163 )   HTML ( 329 )  
    PDF (3167KB) ( 3928 )   Peer Review Comments

    Musical emotion is an emotional response induced by musical stimulation. Music emotion recognition is a process in which individuals evaluate the emotions contained in or intended to be expressed in music. The ability to recognize musical emotions is one manifestation of the development of social cognition, which is also the premise of music appreciation and the basis of emotional regulation by music. If the emotional recognition ability of music is impaired, the aesthetic, spiritual, and practical values will be reduced, and the individual’s emotional understanding and communication will also be affected. Therefore, the factors related to music emotion recognition and how to improve the ability of individual music emotion recognition have become a concern for researchers in music psychology. The study found that personality’s “empathy” trait may be related to recognizing musical emotions. Empathy is the ability to perceive and experience other people’s emotional states accurately. Individuals with strong empathy are more susceptible to emotional infections and are more sensitive to emotions. Thus, from the perspective of musical psychological localization, this study investigated the possible influence of empathy differences on emotion recognition in Chinese national music. The hypotheses were as follows: first, in the emotional judgment stage, compared with the low empathy group, the high empathy group is more accurate in the recognition of musical emotion, easier to judge the consistency and inconsistency, and the amplitude of the P2 and N400 components are smaller. Second, in the stage of emotional meaning integration, the high empathy group processes the musical emotion more deeply and invests more attention resources, and the amplitude of the late positive component (LPC) is larger.

    The cross-channel emotion-priming paradigm was used to study individuals with high and low empathy. The G*Power 3.1.9.2 software was used to calculate the required sample size to achieve sufficient statistical power. The study had a mixed design, with a median effect (f = 0.25) and a significance level α = 0.05, requiring at least 12 participants per class to achieve an 80% statistical power level. In the early stages of the experiment, the Chinese revised Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI-C) was distributed online to recruit college students. Based on responses to 522 questionnaires, 72 college students with high and low empathy levels were chosen to participate in the electroencephalogram experiment. Before the experiment, all participants completed a basic information statistics form and signed an informed consent form. After the experiment, they were given course credit and experimental remuneration.

    The behavioral results showed no significant difference in the response time and accuracy of emotion recognition between the high and low empathy groups. However, the response time of the high-empathy group was slightly lower than that of the low-empathy group, and the accuracy rate of the high-empathy group was higher than that of the low-empathy group under the emotional consistency condition for Gong-tone music. Contrastingly, there was no difference between the low-empathy groups under the two conditions. The event-related potential results showed that P2, N400, and LPC components were induced in the middle and late stages of Chinese national music emotion recognition. The amplitudes of P2 and N400 in the low-empathy group were more significant than those in the high-empathy group, and the amplitude of the LPC in the high-empathy group was more extensive than that in the low-empathy group.

    According to our findings, in the emotion recognition process in Chinese national music, the high- and low-empathy groups showed differences in some electroencephalogram components. This shows that event-related potential technology can explore the differences between the two groups of participants in the time course of music emotion recognition. This study found three main components related to recognizing musical emotions: P2, N400, and LPC. P2 is an indicator of perceptual analysis of musical and emotional stimuli, N400 is more sensitive to the inconsistent effect of musical emotion, and LPC is an indicator of the later evaluation of the emotional significance of musical stimuli. The attentional involvement of high- and low-empathy groups in different stages of Chinese national music emotion recognition may affect the depth of processing emotional stimuli and music emotion recognition. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to investigate the difference in neural responses of individuals with different levels of empathy in emotion recognition of Chinese national music from the electrophysiological level, which has important exploratory significance.

    Figures and Tables | References | Related Articles | Metrics
    Automatic drug use behavior: Characteristics of cue-induced reactivity and behavior extinction
    ZENG Hong, ZHENG Zhiling, LUO Xiaohong, WANG Pengfei, WANG Mengcheng, SU Dequan, YANG Wendeng, HUANG Haijiao, PENG Shuna
    2022, 54 (10):  1193-1205.  doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2022.01193
    Abstract ( 1324 )   HTML ( 125 )  
    PDF (1084KB) ( 1533 )   Peer Review Comments

    Under relevant cues, people who are addicted tend to use drugs with little attention, purpose or cognitive effort, conforming to an automated “habitual” response behavior. When the substance is not available, this habitual response behavior will be transformed into psychological craving.

    A common goal of addiction treatment is to reduce cue-induced reactivity that has automatic and unconscious features. Based on previous research, this study used a self-report questionnaire, two behavioral experiments, and measures of Event-Related Potentials (ERPs) to explore the characteristics and neural mechanisms of cue-induced reactivity in heroin addicts under drug-related cues. On this basis, a training program using Virtual Reality (VR) technology and an Approach-Avoidance task (AAT) was designed to weaken the association between drug-related cues and automatic drug use behavior.

    In Study 1, the participants (N= 38) were men who were addicted to heroin (n=19) and a matched sample of healthy controls (n = 19). The Visual Analog Craving Scale (VAS) was used to investigate cue-induced reactivity by self-report. We then assessed ERPs to explore the nature and neural mechanism of cue-induced reactivity. We hypothesized that compared to the healthy controls, the participants who were addicted to heroin would show more cue-induced reactivity in the form of automatic response outside of awareness.

    Corroborating our hypotheses in Study 1, we proceeded to Study 2. N= 60 men who were addicted to heroin were equally divided into a treatment group (n= 30) and a no-treatment group (n= 30). The treatment was VR-AAT training for 10 sessions over the course of two months, with the goal of reducing or eliminating cue-induced reactivity. Before and after the treatment, VR-AAT and the Craving Automatized Scale-Substances (CAS-S) questionnaire were used to test the difference between the two groups.

    The results showed that 1) In between-group comparisons, VAS scores were higher in the addicts than in the control group. And in Study 2, the within-group analyses showed that self-reports of drug use behavior being “unconscious” and “involuntary” were significantly correlated with the duration of addiction. 2) In within-group analyses, N1 amplitude was smaller, latency was shorter, and reaction time was longer in response to drug-related cues versus neutral cues. Other between-group analyses of the ERP data, addicts demonstrated smaller N1 amplitude and longer reaction time in response to drug-related cues compared to the healthy controls, and these differences were concentrated in the central regions of the brain (i.e., the parietal lobe). 3) After the VR-AAT training, the physiological index (based on temperature, diastolic pressure and systolic pressure) decreased significantly, and the coefficient of approach bias of AAT [(Avoid behavior RTs drug-related- Approach behavior RTs drug-related) - (Avoid behavior RTs neutral- Approach behavior RTs neutral)] was significantly smaller than before training. The results were the same at a two-month follow-up.

    Our findings revealed that cue-induced reactivity showed characteristics of being automatic and unconscious, with greater N1 reactivity in sensorimotor and related brain areas. The VR-AAT training was effective in reducing cue-induced reactions among men addicted to heroin, suggesting that this method has potential applied value in treating heroin addiction and in designing relapse prevention programs. This research contributes to the addiction literature in two ways. First, AAT training has been used in the treatment of alcohol addiction, but not for other forms of addiction. This is the first research to use AAT training to treat heroin addiction. Second, the treatment in the current study is the first one to combine AAT with virtual reality technology. The VR-AAT method might also be a feasible approach for other substance abuse treatments, although more experimental evidence would be needed to support this. One limitation of this study was that the basis of automatic drug use behavior and automatic refusing drug use behavior cannot be distinguished. This issue can be investigated in future research.

    Figures and Tables | References | Related Articles | Metrics
    The unique role of sleep problems among symptoms of posttraumatic stress disorder: A cross-lagged panel network analysis
    LIANG Yiming, YANG Luxi, XI Juzhe, LIU Zhengkui
    2022, 54 (10):  1206-1215.  doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2022.01206
    Abstract ( 2814 )   HTML ( 360 )  
    PDF (9561KB) ( 4164 )   Peer Review Comments

    Traumatic events have been recognized as important precipitants of sleep problems. Meanwhile, traumatic insomnia is one of the criteria for diagnosing post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). However, whether trauma-induced sleep problems are secondary symptoms of PTSD or a core feature of PTSD has not yet reached a consistent conclusion. Recently, the emerging cross-lagged panel network analysis method has played an important role in understanding the role of symptoms in psychopathology. The advantage is that the role of each symptom can be systematically analyzed, and the longitudinal predictive pathway of each symptom can be estimated, thereby inferring the leading symptoms of psychiatric disorders. The present study aims to explore the role of trauma-induced sleep problems in the evolution of PTSD among children and adolescents through the cross-lagged panel network analysis.

    Three months after the Zhouqu debris flow, we started this 2-year longitudinal study. Three assessments were performed at 3 months (T1), 15 months (T2) and 27 months (T3) after the disaster. We enrolled students from 2 primary schools and 3 secondary schools in the hardest-hit areas. Ultimately, 1, 460 children and adolescents completed three rounds of evaluation. At T1, there were 702 students from grades 4 to 6, and 758 students from grades 7 to 9. The average age of the participant was 12.89 (SD = 2.29). Symptoms of PTSD were assessed with the University of California at Los Angeles PTSD Reaction Index based on the DSM-IV. The cross-lagged panel network analysis was conducted using R packages glmnet and qgraph.

    Results showed that at T1→T2, sleep problems had the highest out-expected influence centrality, followed by physiological cue reactivity. They were sources of activation for the nodes receiving its edges, that is, they were easy to activate other symptoms in the PTSD network. Sleep problems at T1 positively predicted a lot of other PTSD symptoms at T2, including intrusive thoughts, nightmares, flashbacks, emotional cue reactivity, restricted positive affect, restricted negative affect, irritability/anger, hypervigilance and exaggerated startle response. The results also revealed several indirect influence paths such as sleep problems predicting nightmares then affecting flashbacks. However, when it comes to T2→T3, it is detachment rather than sleep problems that had the highest out- expected influence. It positively predicted diminished interest, restricted positive affect, sleep problems and irritability/anger. We also found some feedback loop: detachment→ restricted positive affect→diminished interest→detachment.

    This is the first study to explore activation paths of PTSD symptoms among children and adolescents through the cross-lagged panel network analysis. These findings have improved the understanding of the role of trauma-induced sleep problems in the long-term development of PTSD. The results showed that sleep problems at 3 months after the disaster activated a large number of symptoms in PTSD at 15 months after the disaster. Therefore, it is inferred that early sleep problems were the core symptom in the development of PTSD among children and adolescents in the early post-disaster period. However, its predictability decreased in the later period (15 months to 27 months). In conclusion, these findings emphasize the time specificity of the impact of traumatic sleep problems on PTSD symptoms. We recommend that trauma-induced sleep problems should be given greater priority in the diagnostic criteria for PTSD among children and adolescents in the early post-disaster period. Meanwhile, early psychological assistance should vigorously develop treatments based on sleep problems to prevent the occurrence and development of PTSD. One year or longer after the traumatic event, the intervention target should be set to physiological cue reactivity and detachment.

    Figures and Tables | References | Related Articles | Metrics
    The effects of marital quality on coparenting: A cross-level mediation analysis based on the common fate model
    LIU Yiting, FAN Jieqiong, CHEN Bin-Bin
    2022, 54 (10):  1216-1233.  doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2022.01216
    Abstract ( 4062 )   HTML ( 323 )  
    PDF (1060KB) ( 5302 )   Peer Review Comments

    Bronfenbrenner’s ecosystem theory suggests that the different experiences of family members jointly shape the ecological environment and atmosphere of the family, and there may be interdependences between two parents in a family. Previous literature aimed to solve research questions at the family or dyadic level, but their analyses were mainly conducted at the individual level. To solve this issue, we adopted the common fate model (CFM), which better explains the interdependence in intimate relationships at the family or dyadic level. The common fate variable is modeled as a potential variable with two indicators. Based on the common fate model, this study explored the effect of family marital quality on the coparenting and the cross-level mediating role of the parenting sense of competence between couples. Questionnaire data were collected from 650 families including both fathers and mothers whose children (ranging from 4 to 7 years old) are in Shanghai kindergartens by using stratified cluster sampling. The results indicated that: (1) marital quality positively predicted supportive coparenting and negatively predicted undermining coparenting within a family; (2) family marital quality positively predicted each parent’s parenting sense of competence; (3) each parent's parenting sense of competence positively predicted supportive coparenting and negatively predicted undermining coparenting; (4) each parent’s parenting sense of competence mediated the relationship between family marital quality and supportive and undermining coparenting. Higher marital quality in the family helped to increase each parent's parenting sense of competence, which in turn increased supportive coparenting and decreased undermining coparenting.

    This study explored the cross-level mediating effect of parenting sense of competence between marital quality and parental coparenting using the common fate model in a longitudinal study. It combined the family level (marital quality and coparenting) and the individual level (parenting sense of competence), providing theoretical support and guidance for creating a good family environment and further promoting the development of the common fate model. To the best of our knowledge, this study is the first application of the common fate model in the study of Chinese families. The findings may have practical implications on the wellbeing of Chinese families.

    Figures and Tables | References | Related Articles | Metrics
    Gain or Loss? Examining the double-edged sword effect of challenge demand on work-family enrichment
    XU Shan, ZHANG Yucheng, ZHANG Bingran, SHI Junqi, YUAN Mengsha, REN Yingwei
    2022, 54 (10):  1234-1247.  doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2022.01234
    Abstract ( 2051 )   HTML ( 249 )  
    PDF (813KB) ( 2462 )   Peer Review Comments

    The balance of work and family has received widespread attention from managers and researchers in recent years. Previous research claims that job demand, as a kind of pressure felt by employees, reduces employees’ work motivation, increases employee fatigue and anxiety, and hinders work-family enrichment. However, different job demands (e.g., challenge demand and hindrance demand) have different effects on employees. Challenge demand, which gives individuals the opportunity to acquire new knowledge and promote personal growth, influences work-family enrichment in a complex way. However, few researches pay sufficient attention to the mechanism of the double-edged effect in the relationship between challenge demand and work-family enrichment. In addition, although scholarly research on job demand and work-family enrichment has increasingly focused on within-person fluctuations in recent years, how to examine the “dynamic” effects of challenge demand on work-family enrichment has received little attention. Therefore, based on conservation of resource theory and uncertainty management theory and from static and dynamic perspectives, we comprehensively examined the effects of challenge demand on work-family enrichment by clarifying the different attributes of challenge demand (daily level model, average level model, daily shift model, and variability model).

    We tested our hypothesis by conducting a diary study of 105 participations over 10 working days. Data were collected via a job demand scale, work absorption scale, relaxation scale, and work-family enrichment scale. First, we recorded control variables (gender, marital status, and spouse’s work status) during the weekends. Second, researchers collected daily data (challenge demand, hindrance demand, work absorption, relaxation, and work-family enrichment) from 4:00 p.m. to 10:00 p.m. each workday. Ultimately, 645 valid data points at the within-person level were available for 81 participants. Using SPSS 24.0, Mplus 7.0, and R software, we conducted a multilevel path analysis to examine the theoretical model.

    Confirmatory factor analysis was conducted to examine the validity of the key variables (challenge demand, work absorption, relaxation, and work-family enrichment), and the results confirmed that the present study had good discriminant validity. The results of multilevel path analysis showed that the following. (1) In the static model, challenge demand had double-edged sword effects on work-family enrichment at the within-person level (daily level model); in the average level model (between-person level), the average level of challenge demand positively predicted chronic work absorption and negatively predicted chronic relaxation, and the mediating effect of chronic relaxation was stronger than that of chronic work absorption. (2) In the dynamic model, directionality of daily shifts in challenge demand negatively affected daily work absorption and daily relaxation in the daily shift model (within-person level). Only the mediating role of daily relaxation was significant; in the variability model (between-person level), the variability level of challenge demand had a negative impact on chronic work absorption and chronic relaxation, and only the mediating role of chronic relaxation was significant.

    The study makes critical contributions both theoretically and practically. (1) The static model indicated that, through the gained and deleted paths, the double-edged sword effect of the relationship between daily challenge demand, average level of change demand, and work-family enrichment was significant. (2) Through the dynamic model, this study explored the negative effect of challenge demand fluctuations on work-family enrichment in two forms, namely, daily shift directionality and the variability of challenge demand. Practically, this study suggests that managers should fully recognize the double-edged sword effect of challenge demand.

    Figures and Tables | References | Related Articles | Metrics
    The impact of guanxi human resource management practices on beneficiaries’ altruistic behavior: The perspective of compensatory ethics
    YANG Huan, WEI Xuhua
    2022, 54 (10):  1248-1261.  doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2022.01248
    Abstract ( 2354 )   HTML ( 281 )  
    PDF (772KB) ( 2806 )   Peer Review Comments

    Guanxi HRM practices depict how performance and contributions give their way to interpersonal relationships for personnel decisions. That is, an employee’s relationships with the supervisor can largely factor in job tasks, performance appraisal, promotion, compensation, and training opportunities. Prevalent and conspicuous, guanxi HRM practices compromise organizational justice and undermine the legitimacy of personnel decisions. The upshot would include a group of demotivated employees, discredited employer branding, and intensified organizational politics. The extant literature has paid much attention to the detriments to individuals, organizations, and society. Yet, reactions from the beneficiaries have been largely overlook. Would they take it for granted? Or would they otherwise seek to “call it even”? We can hardly reach to a firm conclusion on what guanxi HRM practices implicate until those with vested interests are involved in the analysis.

    Drawing upon compensatory ethics theory, we made the first attempt to theoretically explicate and empirically investigate the position that guanxi HRM practices could spur the beneficiaries’ altruistic behavior by arousing the intention to “call it even.” We conducted four independent experiments with various samples and materials to allow for causal inferences. Specifically, we tested the main effect of guanxi HRM practices on the beneficiaries’ altruistic behavior (Hypothesis 1) and the mediation effect of guilt (Hypothesis 2) with a vignette of performance evaluation and bonus allocation in Study 1a (N = 124) and 1b (N = 184), respectively. The serial mediating effect involving moral self-image (Hypothesis 3) was tested in Study 2 (N = 211), with a vignette of promotion. Finally, we tested the moderating effect of coworker relationship closeness (Hypothesis 4) in Study 3 with a 2 (guanxi HRM practices: high vs. low) × 2 (coworker relationship closeness: high vs. low) factorial design (N = 211) using the vignette of bonus allocation.

    The results of four experiments fully supported our theoretical model. We found that the beneficiaries’ moral self-image and sense of guilt serially mediated the effect of guanxi HRM practices on their altruistic behavior (Study 1 and Study 2). Moreover, coworker relationship closeness moderated the serial mediating effect such that guanxi HRM practice sparked a more substantial impact on the beneficiaries’ altruistic behavior when they enjoy more close relationships with coworkers than vice versa (Study 3). Collectively, the reported effect size - being from medium to large, the various samples, and the diverse set of materials lead to robust findings.

    Therefore, those beneficiaries would “call it even” than take it for granted. This research stressed the power of moral compensation and revealed a bright side of guanxi HRM practices for non-beneficiaries, those deprived of justice and resources. We added a meaningful complement to the extant sentiments that deem guanxi HRM practices overwhelmingly detrimental. It is also essential to differentiate between supervisors, beneficiaries, and non-beneficiaries to better understand this phenomenon. In short, this research broadens the theoretical spectra and informs organizations on how to balance the pros and cons of guanxi HRM practices.

    Figures and Tables | References | Related Articles | Metrics
    A new method for estimating the optimal sample size in generalizability theory based on evolutionary algorithm: Comparisons with three traditional methods
    LI Guangming, QIN Yue
    2022, 54 (10):  1262-1276.  doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2022.01262
    Abstract ( 570 )   HTML ( 51 )  
    PDF (857KB) ( 637 )   Peer Review Comments

    Generalizability Theory (GT) is widely applied in psychological measurement and evaluation. A larger generalizability coefficient often indicates a higher reliability the test may have. Generalizability coefficients can be improved by increasing sample sizes. However, the size of a sample would be subject to budget constraints. Therefore, it is important to examine how to effectively determine the size of a sample considering the budget constraints. The existing literature has been largely limited to traditional methods, such as the differential optimization method, the Lagrange method and the Cauchy Schwartz inequality method.

    These traditional methods have limited scope of application and their typical conditions are hard to satisfy. In addition, there is no unified comparison available. Fortunately, with the increased use of high performance computing, the Constrained Optimization Evolutionary Algorithms (COEAs) becomes highly feasible.

    This paper expands and compares the four methods—the differential optimization method, Lagrange method, Cauchy Schwartz inequality method, and COEAs—determine the best solution to the optimal sample size problem under the budget constraints in GT. Specifically, this paper compares the applicability of the four methods using three generalizability designs, including p × i × r, (r: p) × i and p × i × r × o designs. The results are presented as follows:

    (1) In the optimization performance of two-facet generalizability design of p × i × r and (r: p) × i, the performance of COEAs is slightly better than that of the traditional methods, whereas the performance of three traditional methods is equivalent. Although COEAs and the traditional methods have showed similar accuracy, the former has better compliance concerning budget constraints.

    (2) In the optimization performance of three-facet generalizability design of p × i × r × o, the performance of COEAs is obviously better than that of the traditional methods. The least ideal generalizability coefficient is obtained using the differential optimization method, whereas its budget compliance is the best; the generalizability coefficient obtained by Lagrange method is the best, but higher than the budget. The Cauchy inequality method obtains a better generalizability coefficient under special budget constraints. But, the performance of COEAs is slightly better than that of Cauchy Schwartz inequality method, especially closer to the budget constraints.

    (3) In terms of the algorithm complexity, COEAs obtains an obviously smaller algorithm complexity than do the traditional methods. The complexity of the three traditional methods is relatively high. However, COEAs does not rely on the derivation of mathematical formulas, and the algorithm is relatively less complex.

    (4) In terms of the algorithm applicability, COEAs is significantly better than the traditional methods. The applicability of the three traditional methods is relatively narrow. However, COEAs does not rely on a specific generalizability design or a budget expression, and, therefore, the applicability of COEAs is stronger.

    (5) In terms of the algorithm generalizability, COEAs is obviously better than the traditional methods. The limited mathematical principles make it difficult to extend the three traditional methods to more complex generalizability designs, and thus, the feasibility of calculation is poor. Howerve, COEAs has revealed stronger generalizability.

    (6) In terms of the possibility of getting the best solution, COEAs is also better than the traditional methods. Because evolutionary algorithm is a probabilistic algorithm, multiple tests can be conducted to obtain better results for optimal sample sizes. Under some conditions, COEAs can determine better solutions, which, however, is impossible for three traditional methods.

    (7) These results suggest that COEAs is superior to three traditional methods in estimating the optimal sample size problem under the budget constraints in GT. It is recommended that researchers use COEAs in future research to determine an optimal sample size in their psychological measurement and evaluation.

    Figures and Tables | References | Related Articles | Metrics
    Exploration of change point analysis in detecting speededness based on response time data with known/unknown item parameters
    ZHONG Xiaoyuan, YU Xiaofeng, MIAO Ying, QIN Chunying, PENG Yafeng, TONG Hao
    2022, 54 (10):  1277-1292.  doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2022.01277
    Abstract ( 585 )   HTML ( 30 )  
    PDF (847KB) ( 847 )   Peer Review Comments

    In recent years, response time has received a rapidly growing amount of attention in psychometric research, likely due to the increasing availability of (item-level) response time data through computer-based testing and online survey data collection. Compared to the conventional item response data that are often dichotomous or polytomous, the response time is continuous and can provide much more information. Aberrant response behaviors are frequently encountered during testing. It could cause various negative effects. Change point analysis (CPA) is a well-established statistical process control method to detect changes in a sequence, and it has provided testing professionals a new lens through to understand test-taking behavior at both the examinee and item levels.

    In this paper, we took test speededness as an example to illustrate how the CPA method can be used to detect aberrant behavior using item response time data. Response time under speededness was simulated using the gradual-change log-normal model for response time. Two CPA-based test statistics, the Likelihood Ratio Test and Wald Test, were used to detect aberrant response behaviors. The critical values were obtained through Monte Carlo simulations and compared with the approximate critical values in a previous study. Based on the chosen critical values, we examined the performance of the likelihood ratio test and Wald test in detecting speeded responses, specifically in terms of power and empirical Type-I error.

    On the one hand, the critical values are almost identical for Wald and the likelihood ratio test. They vary substantially at different nominal α levels, but do not differ much across different test lengths. On the other hand, compared to approximate critical values, the critical values are not too far away from them but are different. That may be because the approximate critical values are suitable for situations where the change point appears in the middle of the test. Results indicate that the proposed method is much more powerful based on the critical values than conventional methods that use item response data. The power was close to 1 for most of the conditions while keeping the type-I error rate well-controlled. Real data analysis also demonstrates the performance of the method.

    This study uses CPA with response time data and offers a very promising approach to detecting aberrant response behavior. Through the simulation study, we demonstrated that it is possible to use fixed critical values in different test lengths, which makes the application of the method straightforward. It also means that it is unnecessary to reconduct the simulation to update critical values when small changes occur in the test. CPA is very flexible. This study assumed that the log-normal model fits the response time data, but the method is not bounded by that assumption.

    Figures and Tables | References | Related Articles | Metrics