心理学报 ›› 2022, Vol. 54 ›› Issue (7): 736-747.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2022.00736
收稿日期:
2021-07-01
发布日期:
2022-05-16
出版日期:
2022-07-25
通讯作者:
宛小昂
E-mail:wanxa@tsinghua.edu.cn
基金资助:
Received:
2021-07-01
Online:
2022-05-16
Published:
2022-07-25
Contact:
WAN Xiaoang
E-mail:wanxa@tsinghua.edu.cn
摘要:
人们常将特定的颜色与风味联系起来形成色味联结, 而且颜色线索会影响风味感知, 这体现了视觉信息对味觉信息加工的跨通道影响。本研究通过两个行为实验, 考察了味觉信息是否也会影响视觉搜索。实验任务要求被试品尝饮品后对形状靶子进行视觉搜索, 并且操纵了味觉刺激的提示性以及靶子颜色与风味之间的关联性。实验1的结果表明, 当风味线索提示的靶子颜色是与风味关联的颜色时可以加快视觉搜索。但如果与该风味关联的颜色出现在干扰项上, 风味线索则无法促进视觉搜索。其后, 我们在实验2中排除了语义启动的混淆, 也发现了与实验1一致的结果。本研究的结果表明, 风味线索会引发对关联颜色的注意偏向, 证实了味觉刺激对视觉注意的跨通道影响, 为色味交互提供了新的实验证据, 揭示了跨通道影响可能的发生机制。
中图分类号:
彭宇彬, 宛小昂. (2022). 视觉搜索中风味引发对关联颜色的注意偏向. 心理学报, 54(7), 736-747.
PENG Yubin, WAN Xiaoang. (2022). Flavors bias attention toward associated colors in visual search. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 54(7), 736-747.
[1] |
Brunetti, R., Indraccolo, A., Del Gatto, C., Spence, C., & Santangelo, V. (2018). Are crossmodal correspondences relative or absolute? Sequential effects on speeded classification. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 80(2), 527-534.
doi: 10.3758/s13414-017-1445-z URL |
[2] | Delwiche, J. (2004). The impact of perceptual interactions on perceived flavor. Food Quality & Preference, 15(2), 137-146. |
[3] |
Delwiche, J. F. (2012). You eat with your eyes first. Physiology & Behavior, 107(4), 502-504.
doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2012.07.007 URL |
[4] |
Deroy, O., & Spence, C. (2013). Why we are not all synesthetes (not even weakly so). Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20(4), 643-664.
doi: 10.3758/s13423-013-0387-2 URL |
[5] |
DuBose, C. N., Cardello, A. V., & Maller, O. (1980). Effects of colorants and flavorants on identification, perceived flavor intensity, and hedonic quality of fruit-flavored beverages and cake. Journal of Food Science, 45(5), 1393-1399, 1415.
doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2621.1980.tb06562.x URL |
[6] |
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., & Lang, A.-G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behavior Research Methods, 41, 1149-1160.
doi: 10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149 URL |
[7] |
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 175-191.
doi: 10.3758/BF03193146 URL |
[8] | Grubert, A., Krummenacher, J., & Eimer, M. (2011). Redundancy gains in pop-out visual search are determined by top-down task set: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. Journal of Vision, 11(14), 10. |
[9] |
Hörberg, T., Larsson, M., Ekström, I., Sandöy, C., Lundén, P., & Olofsson, J. K. (2020). Olfactory influences on visual categorization: Behavioral and ERP evidence. Cerebral Cortex, 30(7), 4220-4237.
doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhaa050 URL |
[10] |
Huang, F., Wang, C., & Wan, X. (2022). Color-flavor interactions in associative learning: Evidence from a computerized matching task. Food Quality and Preference, 95, 104364.
doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2021.104364 URL |
[11] | Huang, J., Peng, Y., & Wan, X. (2021). The color-flavor incongruency effect in visual search for food labels: An eye-tracking study. Food Quality & Preference, 88(2), 104078. |
[12] |
Huang, J., Wang, F., Sui, J., & Wan, X. (2019). Functional and structural basis of the color-flavor incongruency effect in visual search. Neuropsychologia, 127, 66-74.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2019.02.013 URL |
[13] |
Iordanescu, L., Guzman-Martinez, E., Grabowecky, M., & Suzuki, S. (2008). Characteristic sounds facilitate visual search. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15(3), 548-554.
doi: 10.3758/PBR.15.3.548 URL |
[14] |
Klapetek, A., Ngo, M. K., & Spence, C. (2012). Does crossmodal correspondence modulate the facilitatory effect of auditory cues on visual search? Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 74(6), 1154-1167.
doi: 10.3758/s13414-012-0317-9 URL |
[15] | Kleiner, M. B., Brainard, D. H., Pelli, D. G., Ingling, A., & Broussard, C. (2007). What's new in psychtoolbox-3? Perception, 36(2), 301-307. |
[16] |
Knoeferle, K. M., Knoeferle, P., Velasco, C., & Spence, C. (2016). Multisensory brand search: How the meaning of sounds guides consumers’ visual attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 22(2), 196-210.
doi: 10.1037/xap0000084 URL |
[17] |
Koppen, C., & Spence, C. (2007). Seeing the light: Exploring the Colavita visual dominance effect. Experimental Brain Research, 180, 737-754.
doi: 10.1007/s00221-007-0894-3 URL |
[18] | Moriya, J. (2018). Visual mental imagery influences attentional guidance in a visual-search task. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 80(5), 1127-1142. |
[19] | Munneke, J., Corbett, J. E., & van der Burg, E. (2020). Learned prioritization yields attentional biases through selection history. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 82, 2244-2256. |
[20] | Ngo, M. K., & Spence, C. (2010). Auditory, tactile, and multisensory cues facilitate search for dynamic visual stimuli. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72(6), 1654-1665. |
[21] | Orchard-Mills, E., Alais, D., & van der Burg, E. (2013). Cross- modal associations between vision, touch, and audition influence visual search through top-down attention, not bottom-up capture. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 75(8), 1892-1905. |
[22] |
Parise, C. V. (2016). Crossmodal correspondences: Standing issues and experimental guidelines. Multisensory Research, 29(1-3), 7-28.
doi: 10.1163/22134808-00002517 URL |
[23] | Parise, C. V., & Spence, C. (2013). Audiovisual crossmodal correspondences in the general population. In J. Simner & E. M. Hubbard (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of synesthesia (pp.790-815). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. |
[24] |
Peng, Y., Sun, Y., & Wan, X. (2022). Influence of the crossmodal congruency between color and flavor on product evaluations: Evidence from behavioral and oscillatory brain responses. Food Quality and Preference, 97, 104480.
doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2021.104480 URL |
[25] |
Posner, M. I., Nissen, M. J., & Klein, R. M. (1976). Visual dominance: An information-processing account of its origins and significance. Psychological Review, 83(2), 157-171.
pmid: 769017 |
[26] | Qi, Y., Zhao, H., Wang, C., & Wan, X. (2020). Transfer of repeated exposure cost via color-flavor associations. Journal of Sensory Studies, 35(4), e12578. |
[27] |
Saluja, S., & Stevenson, R. J. (2018). Cross-modal associations between real tastes and colors. Chemical Senses, 43(7), 475-480.
doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjy033 URL |
[28] |
Shankar, M. U., Levitan, C. A., Prescott, J., & Spence, C. (2009). The influence of color and label information on flavor perception. Chemosensory Perception, 2, 53-58.
doi: 10.1007/s12078-009-9046-4 URL |
[29] | Shermer, D. Z., & Levitan, C. A. (2014). Red hot: The crossmodal effect of color intensity on perceived piquancy. Multisensory Research, 27(3-4), 207-223. |
[30] |
Spence, C. (2011). Crossmodal correspondences: A tutorial review. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 73(4), 971-995.
doi: 10.3758/s13414-010-0073-7 URL |
[31] |
Spence, C. (2019). On the relationship (s) between color and taste/flavor. Experimental Psychology, 66(2), 99-111.
doi: 10.1027/1618-3169/a000439 URL |
[32] | Spence, C., Parise, C., & Chen, Y. C. (2012). The Colavita visual dominance effect. In: M. M, Murray & M. T. Wallace (Eds.), The neural bases of multisensory processes (pp. 529-556). Boca Raton (FL): CRC Press. |
[33] |
Stäger, L., Roel Lesur, M., & Lenggenhager, B. (2021). What am I drinking? Vision modulates the perceived flavor of drinks, but no evidence of flavor altering color perception in a mixed reality paradigm. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 641069.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.641069 URL |
[34] |
Topolinski, S., Lindner, S., & Freudenberg, A. (2014). Popcorn in the cinema: Oral interference sabotages advertising effects. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 24(2), 169-176.
doi: 10.1016/j.jcps.2013.09.008 URL |
[35] |
van der Burg, E., Cass, J., Olivers, C. N., Theeuwes, J., & Alais, D. (2010). Efficient visual search from synchronized auditory signals requires transient audiovisual events. PLoS One, 5(5), e10664.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0010664 URL |
[36] |
van der Burg, E., Olivers, C. N. L., Bronkhorst, A. W., & Theeuwes, J. (2008). Pip and pop: Nonspatial auditory signals improve spatial visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34, 1053-1065.
doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.34.5.1053 URL |
[37] |
Velasco, C., Wan, X., Knoeferle, K., Zhou, X., Salgado-Montejo, A., & Spence, C. (2015). Searching for flavor labels in food products: The influence of color-flavor congruence and association strength. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 301.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00301 pmid: 25870568 |
[38] | Velasco, C., Wan, X., Salgado-Montejo, A., Woods, A., Oñate, G. A., Mu, B., & Spence, C. (2014). The context of colour- flavour associations in crisps packaging: A cross-cultural study comparing Chinese, Colombian, and British consumers. Food Quality & Preference, 38, 49-57. |
[39] |
Wadhera, D., & Capaldi-Phillips, E. D. (2014). A review of visual cues associated with food on food acceptance and consumption. Eating Behaviors, 15, 132-143.
doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2013.11.003 pmid: 24411766 |
[40] |
Wan, X., Velasco, C., Michel, C., Mu, B., Woods, A. T., & Spence, C. (2014). Does the type of receptacle influence the crossmodal association between colour and flavour? A cross-cultural comparison. Flavour, 3(1), 3.
doi: 10.1186/2044-7248-3-3 URL |
[41] | Weierich, M. R., Treat, T. A., & Hollingworth, A. (2008). Theories and measurement of visual attentional processing in anxiety. Cognition & Emotion, 22, 985-1018. |
[42] |
Wilton, M., Stancak, A., Giesbrecht, T., Thomas, A., & Kirkham, T. (2019). Intensity expectation modifies gustatory evoked potentials to sweet taste: Evidence of bidirectional assimilation in early perceptual processing. Psychophysiology, 56, e13299.
doi: 10.1111/psyp.13299 URL |
[43] |
Zampini, M., Sanabria, D., Phillips, N., & Spence, C. (2007). The multisensory perception of flavor: Assessing the influence of color cues on flavor discrimination responses. Food Quality and Preference, 18(7), 975-984.
doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2007.04.001 URL |
[1] | 尹华站, 张丽, 刘鹏玉, 李丹. 负性情绪的动机维度对时距知觉的影响:注意控制和注意偏向的中介作用[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(12): 1917-1931. |
[2] | 张妮, 刘文, 刘方, 郭鑫. 8~12岁儿童抑郁与认知重评的关系:悲伤面孔注意偏向的中介作用[J]. 心理学报, 2022, 54(1): 25-39. |
[3] | 侯娟, 朱英格, 方晓义. 手机成瘾与抑郁:社交焦虑和负性情绪信息注意偏向的多重中介作用[J]. 心理学报, 2021, 53(4): 362-373. |
[4] | 华艳, 李明霞, 王巧婷, 冯彩霞, 张晶. 左侧眶额皮层在自动情绪调节下注意选择中的作用:来自经颅直流电刺激的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(9): 1048-1056. |
[5] | 雷怡, 夏琦, 莫志凤, 李红. 面孔可爱度和客观熟悉度对婴儿面孔注意偏向效应的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(7): 811-822. |
[6] | 任志洪, 赵子仪, 余香莲, 赵春晓, 张琳, 林羽中, 张微. 睾酮素与反社会倾向未成年犯的攻击行为:敌意注意偏向的中介和皮质醇的调节作用[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(11): 1288-1300. |
[7] | 刘静远, 李虹. 状态焦虑对时距知觉的影响:认知评价和注意偏向有调节的中介作用[J]. 心理学报, 2019, 51(7): 747-758. |
[8] | 袁小钧, 崔晓霞, 曹正操, 阚红, 王晓, 汪亚珉. 虚拟仿真场景中威胁性视觉刺激搜索的注意偏向效应 *[J]. 心理学报, 2018, 50(6): 622-636. |
[9] | 胡金生, 李骋诗, 王琦, 李松泽, 李涛涛, 刘淑清. 孤独症青少年的情绪韵律注意偏向缺陷:低效率的知觉模式*[J]. 心理学报, 2018, 50(6): 637-646. |
[10] | 孙俊才; 石荣. 哭泣表情面孔的注意偏向:眼动的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2017, 49(2): 155-163. |
[11] | 寇慧;苏艳华;罗小春;陈红. 相貌负面身体自我女性对相貌词的注意偏向眼动的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2015, 47(10): 1213-1222. |
[12] | 王敬欣;贾丽萍;白学军;罗跃嘉. 返回抑制过程中情绪面孔加工优先:ERPs研究[J]. 心理学报, 2013, 45(1): 1-10. |
[13] | 翁春燕,陈红,朱岚. 限制性饮食者对食物线索的注意偏向:基于目标矛盾理论模型[J]. 心理学报, 2012, 44(5): 680-689. |
[14] | 李海江,杨娟,贾磊,张庆林. 不同自尊水平者的注意偏向[J]. 心理学报, 2011, 43(08): 907-916. |
[15] | 杨智辉,王建平. 广泛性焦虑个体的注意偏向[J]. 心理学报, 2011, 43(02): 164-174. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||