[1] |
Casarotti, M., Michielin, M., Zorzi, M., & Umiltà, C. (2007). Temporal order judgment reveals how number magnitude affects visuospatial attention. Cognition, 102(1), 101-117.
pmid: 17046735
|
[2] |
Craft, J. L., & Simon, J. R. (1970). Processing symbolic information from a visual display: Interference from an irrelevant directional cue. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 83(3), 415-420.
pmid: 4098174
|
[3] |
Dehaene, S., & Akhavein, R. (1995). Attention, automaticity, and levels of representation in number processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21(2), 314-326.
|
[4] |
Dehaene, S., Bossini, S., & Giraux, P. (1993). The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology General, 122(3), 371-396.
|
[5] |
Dehaene, S., Dupoux, E., & Mehler, J. (1990). Is numerical comparison digital? Analogical and symbolic effects in two-digit number comparison. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16(3), 626-641.
|
[6] |
Dodd, M. D., van der Stigchel, S., Leghari, M. A., Fung, G., & Kingstone, A. (2008). Attentional SNARC: There’s something special about numbers (let us count the ways). Cognition, 108(3), 810-818.
|
[7] |
Fias, W., Lauwereyns, J., & Lammertyn, J. (2001). Irrelevant digits affect feature-based attention depending on the overlap of neural circuits. Cognitive Brain Research, 12(3), 415-423.
pmid: 11689301
|
[8] |
Fischer, M. H., Warlop, N., Hill, R. L., & Fias, W. (2004). Oculomotor bias induced by number perception. Experimental Psychology, 51(2), 91-97.
pmid: 15114901
|
[9] |
Gevers, W., Caessens, B., & Fias, W. (2005). Towards a common processing architecture underlying Simon and SNARC effects. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 17(5), 659-673.
|
[10] |
Gevers, W., Ratinckx, E., De Baene, W., & Fias, W. (2006). Further evidence that the SNARC effect is processed along a dual-route architecture: Evidence from the lateralized readiness potential. Experimental Psychology, 53(1), 58-68.
pmid: 16610273
|
[11] |
Gevers, W., Verguts, T., Reynvoet, B., Caessens, B., & Fias, W. (2006). Numbers and space: A computational model of the SNARC effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32(1), 32-44.
|
[12] |
He, H., Yu, H. H., & Liu, J. P. (2015). Study on number processing with global-local aspects of compound visual stimulations. Psychological Exploration, 35(6), 499-501.
|
|
[何华, 俞华华, 刘建平. (2015). 整体-局部条件下的数字加工研究. 心理学探新, 35(6), 499-501.]
|
[13] |
Hedge, A., & Marsh, N. W. A. (1975). The effect of irrelevant spatial correspondences on two-choice response-time. Act Psychologica, 39(6), 427-439.
|
[14] |
Hu, C. P., Kong, X. Z., Wagenmakers, E.-J., Ly, A., & Peng, K. P. (2018). The Bayes factor and its implementation in JASP: A practical primer. Advances in Psychological Science, 26(6), 951-965.
|
|
[胡传鹏, 孔祥祯, Wagenmakers, E.-J., Ly, A., 彭凯平. (2018). 贝叶斯因子及其在JASP中的实现. 心理科学进展, 26(6), 951-965.]
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2018.00951
|
[15] |
Jeffreys, H. (1961). Theory of probability (3rd ed.). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
|
[16] |
Kang, W., Yang, M., & Wang, L. (2013). SNARC effect: Current research, theories and suggestions. Journal of Psychological Science, 36(5), 1242-1248.
|
|
[康武, 杨敏, 王丽. (2013). SNARC效应: 现状、理论及建议. 心理科学, 36(5), 1242-1248.]
|
[17] |
Keus, I. M., Jenks, K. M., & Schwarz, W. (2005). Psychophysiological evidence that the SNARC effect has its functional locus in a response selection stage. Cognitive Brain Research, 24(1), 48-56.
pmid: 15922157
|
[18] |
Keus, I. M., & Schwarz, W. (2005). Searching for the functional locus of the SNARC effect: Evidence for a response-related origin. Memory and Cognition, 33(4), 681-695.
|
[19] |
Kornblum, S., Hasbroucq, T., & Osman, A. (1990). Dimensional overlap: Cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility- a model and taxonomy. Psychological Review, 97(2), 253-270.
doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.97.2.253
pmid: 2186425
|
[20] |
Li, Q., Nan, W., Wang, K., & Liu, X. (2014). Independent processing of stimulus-stimulus and stimulus-response conflicts. PLoS ONE, 9(2), e89249.
|
[21] |
Luo, T., Qiu, R. Y., Chen, B., & Fu, S. M. (2018). The stimulus representation of unconscious information and its temporal characteristics. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 50(5), 473-482.
|
|
[罗婷, 邱茹依, 陈斌, 傅世敏. (2018). 无意识信息的刺激表征及其时间特性. 心理学报, 50(5), 473-482.]
|
[22] |
Luo, C., & Proctor, R. W. (2021). Word-and arrow-based Simon effects emerge for eccentrically presented location words and arrows. Psychological Research, 85(2), 816-827.
|
[23] |
Nan, W. Z., Yan, L. Z., Yang, G. C., Liu, X., & Fu, S. M. (2021). Two processing stages of the SNARC effect. Psychological Research, 86(2), 375-385.
doi: 10.1007/s00426-021-01506-5
pmid: 33847782
|
[24] |
Navon, D. (1977). Forest before trees: The precedence of global features in visual perception. Cognitive Psychology, 9(3), 353-383.
|
[25] |
Restle, F. (1970). Speed of adding and comparing numbers. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 83(2), 274-278.
|
[26] |
Salillas, E., El Yagoubi, R., & Semenza, C. (2008). Sensory and cognitive processes of shifts of spatial attention induced by numbers: An ERP study. Cortex, 44(4), 406-413.
doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2007.08.006
pmid: 18387572
|
[27] |
Scerrati, E., Lugli, L., Nicoletti, R., & Umilta, C. (2017). Comparing Stroop-like and Simon effects on perceptual features. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 17815.
|
[28] |
Schwarz, W., & Keus, I. M. (2004). Moving the eyes along the mental number line: Comparing SNARC effects with saccadic and manual responses. Perception and Psychophysics, 66(4), 651-664.
pmid: 15311664
|
[29] |
Shi, X. L. (2010). Spatial representations of numbers in paradigm of Global-Local processing [Unpublished master’s thesis]. Soochow University, China.
|
|
[时秀兰. (2010). 整体-局部加工范式下的数字空间效应 (硕士学位论文). 苏州大学.]
|
[30] |
Simon, J. R. (1968). Effect of ear stimulated on reaction time and movement time. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 78(2), 344-346.
pmid: 5722451
|
[31] |
Simon, J. R., & Rudell, A. P. (1967). Auditory S-R compatibility: The effect of an irrelevant cue on information processing. Journal of Applied Psychology, 51(3), 300-304.
pmid: 6045637
|
[32] |
Simon, J. R., & Small, A. M.,Jr. (1969). Processing auditory information: Interference from an irrelevant cue. Journal of Applied Psychology, 53(5), 433-435.
pmid: 5366316
|
[33] |
Sternberg, S. (1969). The discovery of processing stages: Extensions of Donders’ method. Acta Psychologica, 30, 276-315.
|
[34] |
Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18(6), 643-662.
|
[35] |
Tlauka, M. (2002). The processing of numbers in choice- reaction tasks. Australian Journal of Psychology, 54(2), 94-98.
|
[36] |
Wagenmakers, E.-J., Love, J., Marsman, M., Jamil, T., Ly, A., Verhagen, J., ... Morey, R. D. (2017). Bayesian inference for psychology. Part II: Example applications with JASP. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25(1), 58-76.
|
[37] |
Wang, Q. Q., Shi, W. D., & Ye, J. (2018). The impact effect of the task switch on the SNARC effect. Chinese Journal of Applied Psychology, 24(3), 271-279.
|
|
[王强强, 石文典, 叶晶. (2018). 任务转换对SNARC效应的抑制. 应用心理学, 24(3), 271-279.]
|
[38] |
Wang, Q. Q., Zhang, Q., Shi, W. D., Wang, Z. W., & Zhang, P. C. (2022). Online construction of spatial representation of numbers: Evidence from the SNARC effect in number processing in interferential situations. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 54(7), 761-771.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2022.00761
|
|
[王强强, 张琦, 石文典, 王志伟, 章鹏程. (2022). 数字空间表征的在线建构:来自干扰情境中数字SNARC效应的证据. 心理学报, 54(7), 761-771.]
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2022.00761
|
[39] |
Wang, Y. H., Bergh, D., Aust, F., Ly, A., Wagenmakers, E.-J., & Hu, C. P. (2023). The Implementation of Bayesian ANOVA in JASP: A Practical Primer. Psychology: Techniques and Applications, 11(9), 528-541.
|
|
[王允宏, Bergh, D., Aust, F., Ly, A., Wagenmakers, E.-J., 胡传鹏.(2023). 贝叶斯方差分析在JASP中的实现. 心理技术与应用, 11(9), 528-541.]
|
[40] |
Wu, F., Gu, Q., Shi, Z. H., Gao, Z. F., & Shen, M. W. (2018). Striding over the “Classical Statistical Inference Trap”— Application of Bayes Factors in Psychological Studies. Chinese Journal of Applied Psychology, 24(3), 195-202.
|
|
[吴凡, 顾全, 施壮华, 高在峰, 沈模卫. (2018). 跳出传统假设检验方法的陷阱——贝叶斯因子在心理学研究领域的应用. 应用心理学, 24(3), 195-202.]
|
[41] |
Xiang, X. R., Yan, L. Z., Fu, S. M., & Nan, W. Z. (2022). Processing stage flexibility of the SNARC effect: Task relevance or magnitude relevance? Frontiers in psychology, 13, 1022999.
|
[42] |
Xu, X. D., & Liu, C. (2006). The spatial character of number. Advances in Psychological Science, 14(6), 851-858.
|
|
[徐晓东, 刘昌. (2006). 数字的空间特性. 心理科学进展, 14(6), 851-858.]
|
[43] |
Yan, L. Z., Chen, Y. X., Liu, X., Fu, S. M., & Nan, W. Z. (2022). The flexibility of spatial-numerical associations and its internal mechanism. Advances in Psychological Science, 30(1), 51-64.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2022.00051
|
|
[颜丽珠, 陈妍秀, 刘勋, 傅世敏, 南威治. (2022). 数字空间联结的灵活性及其内在机制. 心理科学进展, 30(1), 51-64.]
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2022.00051
|
[44] |
Yan, L. Z., Yang, G. C., Nan, W. Z., Liu, X., & Fu, S. M. (2021). The SNARC effect occurs in the response-selection stage. Acta Psychologica, 215, 103292.
|