心理学报 ›› 2024, Vol. 56 ›› Issue (10): 1340-1350.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2024.01340
收稿日期:
2022-12-30
发布日期:
2024-07-10
出版日期:
2024-10-25
通讯作者:
翟舒怡, E-mail: shuyizhai@zjut.edu.cn;基金资助:
PAN Laike1, ZHAI Shuyi2(), HE Jie1()
Received:
2022-12-30
Online:
2024-07-10
Published:
2024-10-25
摘要:
父母教养和儿童行为存在密不可分的双向作用, 但少有研究在微观层面检验亲子互动进程中的儿童驱动效应与父母驱动效应。本研究考察了113对5~6岁学龄前儿童及其家长在冲突对话中的行为表现, 基于贝叶斯统计构建动态结构方程模型, 捕捉实时互动引发的父母教养行为和儿童行为反应的变化, 并通过回归分析考察父母和儿童行为对对话结局的影响。 结果发现: (1)儿童在15 s间隔内的消极行为反应能够正向预测随后15 s父母的非支持性教养行为; (2)儿童的积极行为反应对对话结局起主要预测作用, 儿童行为反应的积极水平越高, 越有助于冲突的建设性解决。这些结果从微观层面验证了儿童驱动效应, 强调了儿童反应在亲子互动中的重要作用。
中图分类号:
潘莱珂, 翟舒怡, 何洁. (2024). 亲子对话中父母教养行为与5~6岁儿童行为反应的动态关系. 心理学报, 56(10), 1340-1350.
PAN Laike, ZHAI Shuyi, HE Jie. (2024). Dynamics of parenting behaviors and 5-6-year-old children’s behavioral response during a conflict discussion task. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 56(10), 1340-1350.
变量 | M ± SD | 1 | 2 | 3 |
---|---|---|---|---|
1支持性教养行为 | 1.77 ± 0.30 | |||
2非支持性教养行为 | 1.37 ± 0.31 | −0.50*** (−0.48***) | ||
3儿童积极行为反应 | 1.97 ± 0.48 | 0.31** (0.31**) | 0.09 (0.10) | |
4儿童消极行为反应 | 1.69 ± 0.36 | −0.17 (−0.16) | 0.41*** (0.42***) | −0.05 (0.01) |
表1 父母教养行为和儿童行为反应的描述性统计及相关分析
变量 | M ± SD | 1 | 2 | 3 |
---|---|---|---|---|
1支持性教养行为 | 1.77 ± 0.30 | |||
2非支持性教养行为 | 1.37 ± 0.31 | −0.50*** (−0.48***) | ||
3儿童积极行为反应 | 1.97 ± 0.48 | 0.31** (0.31**) | 0.09 (0.10) | |
4儿童消极行为反应 | 1.69 ± 0.36 | −0.17 (−0.16) | 0.41*** (0.42***) | −0.05 (0.01) |
参数 | 固定效应 |
---|---|
均值 | |
父母教养μ1 | 1.432* [1.339, 1.532] |
儿童反应μ2 | 1.709* [1.608, 1.815] |
惯性 | |
父母教养φ1 | 0.144* [0.040, 0.250] |
儿童反应φ2 | 0.136* [0.027, 0.246] |
交叉滞后 | |
父母对儿童φ3 | −0.028 [−0.148, 0.103] |
儿童对父母φ4 | 0.118* [0.034, 0.202] |
新息方差 | |
Log (父母) | −1.795* [−2.123, −1.506] |
Log (儿童) | −1.137* [−1.344, −0.933] |
Log (共同) | −2.979* [−3.511, −2.458] |
表2 模型2均值、惯性和交叉滞后等参数以及95%可信区间的非标准化估计
参数 | 固定效应 |
---|---|
均值 | |
父母教养μ1 | 1.432* [1.339, 1.532] |
儿童反应μ2 | 1.709* [1.608, 1.815] |
惯性 | |
父母教养φ1 | 0.144* [0.040, 0.250] |
儿童反应φ2 | 0.136* [0.027, 0.246] |
交叉滞后 | |
父母对儿童φ3 | −0.028 [−0.148, 0.103] |
儿童对父母φ4 | 0.118* [0.034, 0.202] |
新息方差 | |
Log (父母) | −1.795* [−2.123, −1.506] |
Log (儿童) | −1.137* [−1.344, −0.933] |
Log (共同) | −2.979* [−3.511, −2.458] |
预测变量 | B | β | t | F | R2 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
第一步(enter) | 3.215* | 0.08 | |||
儿童年龄 | −0.18 | −0.06 | −0.70 | ||
儿童性别 | −0.46 | −0.27 | −2.89** | ||
主要抚养者受教育程度 | −0.04 | 0.04 | −0.42 | ||
第二步(enter) | 5.305*** | 0.26 | |||
儿童年龄 | −0.00 | −0.00 | 0.02 | ||
儿童性别 | −0.31 | −0.18 | −2.09* | ||
主要抚养者受教育程度 | −0.02 | 0.02 | −0.26 | ||
父母支持性教养行为 | −0.17 | −0.06 | −0.55 | ||
父母非支持性教养行为 | −0.42 | −0.15 | −1.34 | ||
儿童积极行为反应 | 0.72 | 0.40 | 4.10*** | ||
儿童消极行为反应 | −0.39 | −0.16 | −1.73 |
表3 儿童行为反应、父母教养行为对冲突解决结局的回归分析
预测变量 | B | β | t | F | R2 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
第一步(enter) | 3.215* | 0.08 | |||
儿童年龄 | −0.18 | −0.06 | −0.70 | ||
儿童性别 | −0.46 | −0.27 | −2.89** | ||
主要抚养者受教育程度 | −0.04 | 0.04 | −0.42 | ||
第二步(enter) | 5.305*** | 0.26 | |||
儿童年龄 | −0.00 | −0.00 | 0.02 | ||
儿童性别 | −0.31 | −0.18 | −2.09* | ||
主要抚养者受教育程度 | −0.02 | 0.02 | −0.26 | ||
父母支持性教养行为 | −0.17 | −0.06 | −0.55 | ||
父母非支持性教养行为 | −0.42 | −0.15 | −1.34 | ||
儿童积极行为反应 | 0.72 | 0.40 | 4.10*** | ||
儿童消极行为反应 | −0.39 | −0.16 | −1.73 |
固定效应 | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|
模型1 | 模型3 | 模型4 | ||
均值 | 父母教养μ1 | 1.688* [1.610, 1.768] | 1.671* [1.596, 1.752] | 1.423* [1.328, 1.530] |
儿童反应μ2 | 1.887* [1.780, 2.001] | 1.676* [1.582, 1.777] | 1.879* [1.744, 2.011] | |
惯性 | 父母教养φ1 | 0.122* [0.025, 0.217] | 0.121* [0.029, 0.211] | 0.177* [0.065, 0.285] |
儿童反应φ2 | 0.125* [0.024, 0.223] | 0.159* [0.053, 0.261] | 0.127* [0.015, 0.239] | |
交叉滞后 | 父母对儿童φ3 | 0.033 [−0.082, 0.146] | 0.028 [−0.066, 0.118] | −0.028 [−0.183, 0.119] |
儿童对父母φ4 | −0.002 [−0.084, 0.081] | −0.030 [−0.118, 0.061] | −0.016 [−0.090, 0.057] | |
新息方差 | Log (父母) | −1.426* [−1.653, −1.223] | −1.119* [−1.279, −0.967] | −1.649* [−1.963, −1.369] |
Log (儿童) | −1.004* [−1.191, −0.827] | −1.058* [−1.232, −0.889] | −0.825* [−1.008, −0.647] | |
Log (共同) | −2.634* [−3.229, −2.157] | −4.127* [−4.720, −3.633] | −3.432* [−4.071, −2.822] |
附表1 模型均值、惯性和交叉滞后等参数以及95%可信区间的非标准化估计
固定效应 | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|
模型1 | 模型3 | 模型4 | ||
均值 | 父母教养μ1 | 1.688* [1.610, 1.768] | 1.671* [1.596, 1.752] | 1.423* [1.328, 1.530] |
儿童反应μ2 | 1.887* [1.780, 2.001] | 1.676* [1.582, 1.777] | 1.879* [1.744, 2.011] | |
惯性 | 父母教养φ1 | 0.122* [0.025, 0.217] | 0.121* [0.029, 0.211] | 0.177* [0.065, 0.285] |
儿童反应φ2 | 0.125* [0.024, 0.223] | 0.159* [0.053, 0.261] | 0.127* [0.015, 0.239] | |
交叉滞后 | 父母对儿童φ3 | 0.033 [−0.082, 0.146] | 0.028 [−0.066, 0.118] | −0.028 [−0.183, 0.119] |
儿童对父母φ4 | −0.002 [−0.084, 0.081] | −0.030 [−0.118, 0.061] | −0.016 [−0.090, 0.057] | |
新息方差 | Log (父母) | −1.426* [−1.653, −1.223] | −1.119* [−1.279, −0.967] | −1.649* [−1.963, −1.369] |
Log (儿童) | −1.004* [−1.191, −0.827] | −1.058* [−1.232, −0.889] | −0.825* [−1.008, −0.647] | |
Log (共同) | −2.634* [−3.229, −2.157] | −4.127* [−4.720, −3.633] | −3.432* [−4.071, −2.822] |
分类 | 具体表现 | |
---|---|---|
支持性 | 积极情感 | 爱抚儿童、表露积极情绪 |
鼓励表达 | 让儿童说出他内心的感受、想法 | |
情感关注 | 关注儿童流露的情绪并通过安慰、承诺、转移注意等方式帮助其调节 | |
问题解决 | 鼓励儿童解决问题, 或共同协商问题解决的方式 | |
非支持性 | 消极情感 | 表露不耐烦、生气等消极情绪 |
惩罚 | 对儿童的言语和行为不满, 提出惩罚措施或执行惩罚行为 | |
忽略 | 采用转移话题或不理睬的方式应对 | |
权威控制 | 忽视儿童意愿, 强制提出要求 |
补表1 家长教养行为分类与具体表现
分类 | 具体表现 | |
---|---|---|
支持性 | 积极情感 | 爱抚儿童、表露积极情绪 |
鼓励表达 | 让儿童说出他内心的感受、想法 | |
情感关注 | 关注儿童流露的情绪并通过安慰、承诺、转移注意等方式帮助其调节 | |
问题解决 | 鼓励儿童解决问题, 或共同协商问题解决的方式 | |
非支持性 | 消极情感 | 表露不耐烦、生气等消极情绪 |
惩罚 | 对儿童的言语和行为不满, 提出惩罚措施或执行惩罚行为 | |
忽略 | 采用转移话题或不理睬的方式应对 | |
权威控制 | 忽视儿童意愿, 强制提出要求 |
分类 | 具体表现 | |
---|---|---|
积极 | 问题解决 | 通过协商、讨价还价等方式同父母达成一致的解决方案 |
注意力转移 | 将注意力转移到其他不引起消极情绪的事物上 | |
坚持 | 使用言语坚持或言语拒绝等方式坚持自己的立场, 且不具伤害性 | |
寻求帮助 | 寻求不在场他人帮助 | |
消极 | 回避 | 背离或逃离冲突情境 |
妥协 | 退让或接受父母的方案 | |
发泄 | 情绪失控, 采用肢体或语言攻击 | |
强制 | 强势表达自己的意愿且拒绝商量 |
补表2 儿童行为反应分类与具体表现
分类 | 具体表现 | |
---|---|---|
积极 | 问题解决 | 通过协商、讨价还价等方式同父母达成一致的解决方案 |
注意力转移 | 将注意力转移到其他不引起消极情绪的事物上 | |
坚持 | 使用言语坚持或言语拒绝等方式坚持自己的立场, 且不具伤害性 | |
寻求帮助 | 寻求不在场他人帮助 | |
消极 | 回避 | 背离或逃离冲突情境 |
妥协 | 退让或接受父母的方案 | |
发泄 | 情绪失控, 采用肢体或语言攻击 | |
强制 | 强势表达自己的意愿且拒绝商量 |
[1] | Asparouhov T., Hamaker E. L., & Muthén B. (2018). Dynamic structural equation models. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 25(3), 359-388. |
[2] | Barbato C. A., Graham E. E., & Perse E. M. (2003). Communicating in the family: An examination of the relationship of family communication climate and interpersonal communication motives. Journal of Family Communication, 3(3), 123-148. |
[3] | Barger M. M., Kim E. M., Kuncel N. R., & Pomerantz E. M. (2019). The relation between parents’ involvement in children’s schooling and children’s adjustment: A meta- analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 145(9), 855-890. |
[4] | Barnes H. L., & Olson D. H. (1985). Parent-adolescent communication and the circumplex model. Child Development, 56(2), 438-447. |
[5] | Bauer A., Fairchild G., Halligan S. L., Hammerton G., Murray J., Santos I. S., ... Matijasevich A. (2022). Harsh parenting and child conduct and emotional problems: Parent- and child-effects in the 2004 Pelotas Birth Cohort. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 31(8), 1-11. |
[6] |
Beauchaine T. P. (2012). Physiological markers of emotional and behavioral dysregulation in externalizing psychopathology. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 77(2), 79-86.
doi: 10.1111/j.1540-5834.2011.00665.x pmid: 25242827 |
[7] |
Beebe B., Messinger D., Bahrick L. E., Margolis A., Buck K. A., & Chen H. (2016). A systems view of mother-infant face-to-face communication. Developmental Psychology, 52(4), 556-571.
doi: 10.1037/a0040085 pmid: 26882118 |
[8] | Bell R. Q. (1968). A reinterpretation of the direction of effects in studies of socialization. Psychological Review, 75(2), 81-95. |
[9] | Belsky J. (1980). Mother-infant interaction at home and in the laboratory: A comparative study. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 137(1), 37-47. |
[10] | Belsky J., Rha J. H., & Park S. Y. (2000). Exploring reciprocal parent and child effects in the case of child inhibition in US and Korean samples. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 24(3), 338-347. |
[11] | Berg-Nielsen T. S., Vikan A., & Dahl A. A. (2002). Parenting related to child and parental psychopathology: A descriptive review of the literature. Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 7(4), 529-552. |
[12] | Black B., & Logan A. (1995). Links between communication patterns in mother-child, father-child, and child‐peer interactions and children's social status. Child Development, 66(1), 255-271. |
[13] |
Bronfenbrenner U., & Ceci S. J. (1994). Nature-nuture reconceptualized in developmental perspective: A bioecological model. Psychological Review, 101(4), 568-586.
doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.101.4.568 pmid: 7984707 |
[14] | Chi L., & Yu G. (2010). The approaches of survey and observation applied in research of parent-child communication. Advances in Psychological Science, 18(6), 932-939. |
[池丽萍, 俞国良. (2010). 测量和观察法在亲子沟通研究中的应用. 心理科学进展, 18(6), 932-939.] | |
[15] | Combrinck-Graham L. E. E. (1990). Developments in family systems theory and research. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 29(4), 501-512. |
[16] |
Combs-Ronto L. A., Olson S. L., Lunkenheimer E. S., & Sameroff A. J. (2009). Interactions between maternal parenting and children’s early disruptive behavior: Bidirectional associations across the transition from preschool to school entry. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 37(8), 1151-1163.
doi: 10.1007/s10802-009-9332-2 pmid: 19533326 |
[17] | Dallaire D. H., & Weinraub M. (2005). The stability of parenting behaviors over the first 6 years of life. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 20(2), 201-219. |
[18] | Davidov M., Knafo-Noam A., Serbin L. A., & Moss E. (2015). The influential child: How children affect their environment and influence their own risk and resilience. Development and Psychopathology, 27(4pt1), 947-951. |
[19] |
Del Vecchio T., & Rhoades K. A. (2010). Bidirectional influences in mother-toddler dyads: An examination of the relative influence of mothers' and children's behaviors. Infant and Child Development, 19(5), 516-529.
pmid: 20953329 |
[20] |
Ding X., Ansari A., Li X., Liu Y., & Yan N. (2020). Transactional effects between parental sensitivity and child social adjustment: Specifying trait-state aspects of parenting. Developmental Psychology, 56(7), 1331-1342.
doi: 10.1037/dev0000963 pmid: 32406703 |
[21] | Dix T., & Reinhold D. P. (1991). Chronic and temporary influences on mothers' attributions for children's disobedience. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 37(1), 251-271. |
[22] | Duncombe M. E., Havighurst S. S., Holland K. A., & Frankling E. J. (2012). The contribution of parenting practices and parent emotion factors in children at risk for disruptive behavior disorders. Child Psychiatry & Human Development, 43(5), 715-733. |
[23] |
Eisenberg N., Fabes R. A., & Murphy B. C. (1996). Parents' reactions to children's negative emotions: Relations to children's social competence and comforting behavior. Child Development, 67(5), 2227-2247.
pmid: 9022240 |
[24] | Eisenberg N., & Morris A. S. (2002). Children’s emotion- related regulation. In R. Kail (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior (Vol.30, pp. 189-229). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. |
[25] | Fite P. J., Colder C. R., Lochman J. E., & Wells K. C. (2006). The mutual influence of parenting and boys' externalizing behavior problems. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 27(2), 151-164. |
[26] |
Fivush R., Haden C. A., & Reese E. (2006). Elaborating on elaborations: Role of maternal reminiscing style in cognitive and socioemotional development. Child Development, 77(6), 1568-1588.
pmid: 17107447 |
[27] | Gershoff E. T., Lansford J. E., Sexton H. R., Davis‐Kean P., & Sameroff A. J. (2012). Longitudinal links between spanking and children’s externalizing behaviors in a national sample of White, Black, Hispanic, and Asian American families. Child Development, 83(3), 838-843. |
[28] |
Granic I., & Patterson G. R. (2006). Toward a comprehensive model of antisocial development: A dynamic systems approach. Psychological Review, 113(1), 101-131.
pmid: 16478303 |
[29] | Groh A. M., Fearon R. P., van IJzendoorn M. H., Bakermans-Kranenburg M. J., & Roisman G. I. (2017). Attachment in the early life course: Meta-analytic evidence for its role in socioemotional development. Child Development Perspectives, 11(1), 70-76. |
[30] |
Hao Y., Wang Z., Dong S., Liu S., Wu M., & Lu S. (2019). The predictive effects of maternal life event stress in early childhood on 5-year-old child behavioral problems: A chained mediation model. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 51(1), 85-95.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2019.00085 |
[郝叶芳, 王争艳, 董书阳, 刘斯漫, 武萌, 卢珊. (2019). 儿童早期的母亲生活压力对其5岁行为问题的预测效应: 链式中介分析. 心理学报, 51(1), 85-95.]
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2019.00085 |
|
[31] |
Hamaker E. L., Asparouhov T., Brose A., Schmiedek F., & Muthén B. (2018). At the frontiers of modeling intensive longitudinal data: Dynamic structural equation models for the affective measurements from the COGITO study. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 53(6), 820-841.
doi: 10.1080/00273171.2018.1446819 pmid: 29624092 |
[32] |
Hamra G., MacLehose R., & Richardson D. (2013). Markov chain Monte Carlo: An introduction for epidemiologists. International Journal of Epidemiology, 42(2), 627-634.
doi: 10.1093/ije/dyt043 pmid: 23569196 |
[33] |
Hastings P. D., & Grusec J. E. (1998). Parenting goals as organizers of responses to parent-child disagreement. Developmental Psychology, 34(3), 465-479.
pmid: 9597357 |
[34] | Heilmann A., Mehay A., Watt R. G., Kelly Y., Durrant J. E., van Turnhout J., & Gershoff E. T. (2021). Physical punishment and child outcomes: A narrative review of prospective studies. The Lancet, 398(10297), 355-364. |
[35] |
Jaffee S. R., Caspi A., Moffitt T. E., Polo-Tomas M., Price T. S., & Taylor A. (2004). The limits of child effects: Evidence for genetically mediated child effects on corporal punishment but not on physical maltreatment. Developmental Psychology, 40(6), 1047-1058.
pmid: 15535755 |
[36] | Kuczynski L., & Kochanska G. (1990). Development of children's noncompliance strategies from toddlerhood to age 5. Developmental Psychology, 26(3), 398-408. |
[37] | Kuebli J., Butler S., & Fivush R. (1995). Mother-child talk about past emotions: Relations of maternal language and child gender over time. Cognition & Emotion, 9(2-3), 265-283. |
[38] |
Larsson H., Viding E., Rijsdijk F. V., & Plomin R. (2008). Relationships between parental negativity and childhood antisocial behavior over time: A bidirectional effects model in a longitudinal genetically informative design. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36(5), 633-645.
doi: 10.1007/s10802-007-9151-2 pmid: 17602294 |
[39] |
Lewis-Morrarty E., Degnan K. A., Chronis-Tuscano A., Rubin K. H., Cheah C. S., Pine D. S., ... Fox N. A. (2012). Maternal over-control moderates the association between early childhood behavioral inhibition and adolescent social anxiety symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 40(8), 1363-1373.
doi: 10.1007/s10802-012-9663-2 pmid: 22821448 |
[40] | Lin C. Y. C., & Fu V. R. (1990). A comparison of child- rearing practices among Chinese, immigrant Chinese, and Caucasian-American parents. Child Development, 61(2), 429-433. |
[41] |
Lonigan C. J., Spiegel J. A., Goodrich J. M., Morris B. M., Osborne C. M., Lerner M. D., & Phillips B. M. (2017). Does preschool self-regulation predict later behavior problems in general or specific problem behaviors? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 45(8), 1491-1502.
doi: 10.1007/s10802-016-0260-7 pmid: 28130704 |
[42] | Lu F. (2004). The development of emotion regulation strategies in preschool children and its related research (Unpublished master's thesis). Shanghai: The School of Psychology and Cognitive Science, East China Normal University. |
[陆芳. (2004). 学龄前儿童情绪调节策略的发展及其相关研究 (硕士学位论文). 上海: 华东师范大学心理学系.] | |
[43] | Lu F., & Chen G. (2007). The development of preschool children’s emotion regulation strategies. Journal of Psychological Science, 30(5), 1202-1204. |
[陆芳, 陈国鹏. (2007). 学龄前儿童情绪调节策略的发展研究. 心理科学, 30(5), 1202-1204.] | |
[44] | Luo R., Tamis-LeMonda C. S., & Song L. (2013). Chinese parents’ goals and practices in early childhood. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 28(4), 843-857. |
[45] |
Markova G., Nguyen T., & Hoehl S. (2019). Neurobehavioral interpersonal synchrony in early development: The role of interactional rhythms. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 2078.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02078 pmid: 31620046 |
[46] | McDonald G. W. (1980). Family power: The assessment of a decade of theory and research, 1970-1979. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 42(4), 841-854. |
[47] |
Morris A. S., Silk J. S., Steinberg L., Myers S. S., & Robinson L. R. (2007). The role of the family context in the development of emotion regulation. Social Development, 16(2), 361-388.
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00389.x pmid: 19756175 |
[48] |
Nelson J. A., Boyer B. P., Sang S. A., & Wilson E. K. (2014). Characteristics of mother-child conflict and child sex predicting resolution. Journal of Family Psychology, 28(2), 160-167.
doi: 10.1037/a0035990 pmid: 24588605 |
[49] | Nelson J. A., Boyer B. P., Villarreal D. L., & Smith O. A. (2017). Relations between mothers’ daily work, home, and relationship stress with characteristics of mother-child conflict interactions. Journal of Family Psychology, 31(4), 431-441. |
[50] | Nelson K. (1998). Language in cognitive development: The emergence of the mediated mind. Cambridge University Press. |
[51] | Paschall K. W., & Mastergeorge A. M. (2016). A review of 25 years of research in bidirectionality in parent-child relationships: An examination of methodological approaches. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 40(5), 442-451. |
[52] | Patterson G. R. (1995). Coercion:A basis for early age of onset for arrest. In J. McCord (Ed.), Coercion and punishment in long-term perspective (pp. 81-105). New York: Cambridge University Press. |
[53] | Perlman M., Siddiqui R., Ram A., & Ross H. S. (2000). An analysis of sources of power in children’s conflict interactions. In R. S. L. Mills & S. Duck (Eds.), The developmental psychology of personal relationships (pp.155-174). New York, NY: Wiley. |
[54] |
Plamondon A., Browne D. T., Madigan S., & Jenkins J. M. (2018). Disentangling child-specific and family-wide processes underlying negative mother-child transactions. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 46(3), 437-447.
doi: 10.1007/s10802-017-0309-2 pmid: 28534276 |
[55] |
Ravindran N., Berry D., & McElwain N. L. (2019). Dynamic bidirectional associations in negative behavior: Mother- toddler interaction during a snack delay. Developmental Psychology, 55(6), 1191-1198.
doi: 10.1037/dev0000703 pmid: 30742468 |
[56] |
Ravindran N., McElwain N. L., Berry D., & Kramer L. (2022). Dynamic fluctuations in maternal cardiac vagal tone moderate moment-to-moment associations between children’s negative behavior and maternal emotional support. Developmental Psychology, 58(2), 286-296.
doi: 10.1037/dev0001299 pmid: 34990199 |
[57] |
Recchia H. E., Ross H. S., & Vickar M. (2010). Power and conflict resolution in sibling, parent-child, and spousal negotiations. Journal of Family Psychology, 24(5), 605-615.
doi: 10.1037/a0020871 pmid: 20954771 |
[58] | Reese E., Bird A., & Tripp G. (2007). Children's self-esteem and moral self: Links to parent-child conversations regarding emotion. Social Development, 16(3), 460-478. |
[59] | Sameroff A. J. (1975). Early influences on development: Fact or fancy? Merrill-Palmer Quarterly of Behavior and Development, 21(4), 267-294. |
[60] |
Sameroff A. J., & Mackenzie M. J. (2003). Research strategies for capturing transactional models of development: The limits of the possible. Development and Psychopathology, 15(3), 613-640.
doi: 10.1017/s0954579403000312 pmid: 14582934 |
[61] | Sanders M. R., & Turner K. M. (2018). The importance of parenting in influencing the lives of children. In Sanders, M., Morawska, A. (Eds.), Handbook of parenting and child development across the lifespan (pp. 3-26). Springer, Cham. |
[62] |
Schuurman N. K., Ferrer E., de Boer-Sonnenschein M., & Hamaker E. L. (2016). How to compare cross-lagged associations in a multilevel autoregressive model. Psychological Methods, 21(2), 206-221.
doi: 10.1037/met0000062 pmid: 27045851 |
[63] | Serbin L. A., Kingdon D., Ruttle P. L., & Stack D. M. (2015). The impact of children's internalizing and externalizing problems on parenting: Transactional processes and reciprocal change over time. Development and Psychopathology, 27(4), 969-986. |
[64] | Sokolovic N., Plamondon A., Rodrigues M., Borairi S., Perlman M., & Jenkins J. M. (2021). Do mothers or children lead the dance? Disentangling individual and influence effects during conflict. Child Development, 92(2), e143-e157. |
[65] | Spiegelhalter D. J., Best N. G., Carlin B. P., & Van Der Linde A. (2002). Bayesian measures of model complexity and fit. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series B (Statistical Methodology), 64(4), 583-639. |
[66] | Stansbury K., & Sigman M. (2000). Responses of preschoolers in two frustrating episodes: Emergence of complex strategies for emotion regulation. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 161(2), 182-202. |
[67] | Stein N. L., & Albro E. R. (2001). The origins and nature of arguments: Studies in conflict understanding, emotion, and negotiation. Discourse Processes, 32(2-3), 113-133. |
[68] |
Sulik M. J., Blair C., Mills‐Koonce R., Berry D., & Greenberg M. (2015). Early parenting and the development of externalizing behavior problems: Longitudinal mediation through children's executive function. Child Development, 86(5), 1588-1603.
doi: 10.1111/cdev.12386 pmid: 26082032 |
[69] |
Tager-Flusberg H., & Sullivan K. (2000). A componential view of theory of mind: Evidence from Williams syndrome. Cognition, 76(1), 59-90.
doi: 10.1016/s0010-0277(00)00069-x pmid: 10822043 |
[70] | Verhoeven M., Junger M., van Aken C., Deković M., & van Aken M. A. (2010). Parenting and children's externalizing behavior: Bidirectionality during toddlerhood. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 31(1), 93-105. |
[71] | Wang Q., & Fivush R. (2005). Mother-child conversations of emotionally salient events: Exploring the functions of emotional reminiscing in European-American and Chinese families. Social Development, 14(3), 473-495. |
[72] | West K. B., Shaffer A., Wickrama K. A., Han Z. R., & Suveg C. (2021). Preschoolers’ dynamic respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA) change during a challenging parent-child interactive task: Relations with preschoolers’ socioemotional health. Developmental Psychobiology, 63(5), 1132-1145. |
[73] | Wilson S., & Durbin C. E. (2013). Mother-child and father-child dyadic interaction: Parental and child bids and responsiveness to each other during early childhood. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 59(3), 249-279. |
[74] |
Yan N., Ansari A., & Peng P. (2021). Reconsidering the relation between parental functioning and child externalizing behaviors: A meta-analysis on child-driven effects. Journal of Family Psychology, 35(2), 225-235.
doi: 10.1037/fam0000805 pmid: 33104378 |
[75] |
Zeytinoglu S., Calkins S. D., Swingler M. M., & Leerkes E. M. (2017). Pathways from maternal effortful control to child self-regulation: The role of maternal emotional support. Journal of Family Psychology, 31(2), 170-180.
doi: 10.1037/fam0000271 pmid: 27929315 |
[76] | Zhang X., Wang Y., Li H., Liu W., & Wang L. (2013). Children's behavioral inhibition and maternal parenting: A longitudinal study. Journal of Psychological Science, 36(4), 910-914. |
[张晓, 王玉英, 李环, 刘文玲, 王莉. (2013). 儿童行为抑制性与母亲教养行为: 一项追踪研究. 心理科学, 36(4), 910-914.] | |
[77] | Zinsser K. M., Gordon R. A., & Jiang X. (2021). Parents’ socialization of preschool-aged children's emotion skills: A meta-analysis using an emotion-focused parenting practices framework. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 55, 377-390. |
[1] | 王美萍, 郑晓洁, 夏桂芝, 刘迪迪, 陈翩, 张文新. 负性生活事件与青少年早期抑郁的关系:COMT基因Val158Met多态性与父母教养行为的调节作用[J]. 心理学报, 2019, 51(8): 903-913. |
[2] | 王美萍;张文新;陈欣银. 5-HTR1A基因rs6295多态性与父母教养行为对青少年早期抑郁的交互作用:不同易感性模型的验证[J]. 心理学报, 2015, 47(5): 600-610. |
[3] | 王美萍;张文新. COMT基因rs6267多态性与青少年期亲子亲合与冲突的关系:性别与父母教养行为的调节作用分析[J]. 心理学报, 2014, 46(7): 931-941. |
[4] | 于凤杰;赵景欣;张文新. 早中期青少年未来规划的发展及其与父母教养行为的关系:行为自主的中介效应[J]. 心理学报, 2013, 45(6): 658-671. |
[5] | Richard J. Rose,Jaakko Kaprio. 遗传、环境和青少年的吸烟饮酒行为:芬兰双生子研究的回溯与前瞻[J]. 心理学报, 2008, 40(10): 1062-1072. |
[6] | 吕勤,陈会昌,王莉,陈欣银. 父母教养态度与儿童在2-4岁期间的问题行[J]. 心理学报, 2003, 35(01): 89-92. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||