心理学报 ›› 2023, Vol. 55 ›› Issue (9): 1441-1452.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2023.01441
收稿日期:
2022-07-18
发布日期:
2023-06-09
出版日期:
2023-09-25
通讯作者:
张良
E-mail:Zhangliang1@sdnu.edu.cn
基金资助:
CHEN Guanghui, LI Yihan, DING Wen, CHEN Jing, ZHANG Liang(), ZHANG Wenxin
Received:
2022-07-18
Online:
2023-06-09
Published:
2023-09-25
Contact:
ZHANG Liang
E-mail:Zhangliang1@sdnu.edu.cn
摘要:
人际伤害事件发生后, 犯错者的懊悔与受害者的宽恕是修复人际关系的两个关键因素。本研究采用经典行为实验范式, 以139名4~5岁幼儿为对象, 考察犯错者的懊悔表现对受害者宽恕水平的影响, 以及不同类型旁观者在二者关系中的调节作用。结果表明:4岁和5岁幼儿均已经能够识别犯错者的懊悔表现, 且对懊悔犯错者的宽恕水平显著高于无懊悔者; 旁观者的存在会影响幼儿的宽恕水平, 且教师旁观和好朋友旁观比陌生人旁观更能降低幼儿对懊悔者的宽恕水平, 更能提升对无懊悔者的宽恕水平。旁观者的存在没有显著提升幼儿对懊悔者的宽恕水平, 这可能与幼儿社会化过程中内化的社会期望(如“以德报怨”)有关, 基于此, 本文尝试提出“社会期望的旁观者激活假说”, 并进行了讨论。
中图分类号:
陈光辉, 李一涵, 丁雯, 陈静, 张良, 张文新. (2023). 犯错者懊悔与受害幼儿宽恕的关系:旁观者的影响. 心理学报, 55(9), 1441-1452.
CHEN Guanghui, LI Yihan, DING Wen, CHEN Jing, ZHANG Liang, ZHANG Wenxin. (2023). The association between transgressor’s remorse and victim’s forgiveness among young children: The activation effect of bystanders. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 55(9), 1441-1452.
[1] |
Botto S. V., & Rochat P. (2018). Sensitivity to the evaluation of others emerges by 24 months. Developmental Psychology, 54(9), 1723-1734.
doi: 10.1037/dev0000548 pmid: 30148400 |
[2] |
Botto S. V., & Rochat P. (2019). Evaluative audience perception (EAP): How children come to care about reputation. Child Development Perspectives, 13(3), 180-185.
doi: 10.1111/cdep.2019.13.issue-3 URL |
[3] | Brooks J. H., & Reddon J. R. (2003). The two dimensional nature of remorse: An empirical inquiry into internal and external aspects. Journal of Offender Rehabilitation, 38(2), 1-15. |
[4] | Cigala A., & Mori A. (2022). Perspective taking ability in psychologically maltreated children: A protective factor in peer social adjustment. Frontiers in Psychology, 13. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.816514 |
[5] |
Davis J. R., & Gold G. J. (2011). An examination of emotional empathy, attributions of stability, and the link between perceived remorse and forgiveness. Personality and Individual Differences, 50(3), 392-397.
doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2010.10.031 URL |
[6] |
Deem M. J., & Ramsey G. (2016). Guilt by association? Philosophical Psychology, 29(4), 570-585.
doi: 10.1080/09515089.2015.1126706 URL |
[7] |
DiFonzo N., Alongi A., & Wiele P. (2020). Apology, restitution, and forgiveness after psychological contract breach. Journal of Business Ethics, 161(1), 53-69.
doi: 10.1007/s10551-018-3984-1 |
[8] |
Domagała-Zyśk E. (2006). The significance of adolescents’ relationships with significant others and school failure. School Psychology International, 27(2), 232-247.
doi: 10.1177/0143034306064550 URL |
[9] |
Draper K., Siegel C., White J., Solis C. M., & Mishna F. (2009). Preschoolers, parents, and teachers (PPT): A preventive intervention with an at risk population. International Journal of Group Psychotherapy, 59(2), 221-242.
doi: 10.1521/ijgp.2009.59.2.221 pmid: 19441968 |
[10] | Engelmann J. M., Herrmann E., & Tomasello M. (2012). Five-year olds, but not chimpanzees, attempt to manage their reputations. PLOS ONE, 7(10), Article e48433. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0048433 |
[11] |
Engelmann J. M., Over H., Herrmann E., & Tomasello M. (2013). Young children care more about their reputation with ingroup members and potential reciprocators. Developmental Science, 16(6), 952-958.
doi: 10.1111/desc.12086 pmid: 24118719 |
[12] | Enright R. D. (1991). The moral development of forgiveness. In W. M. Kurtines & J. L. Gewirtz (Eds.), Handbook of moral behavior and development, Vol. 1. Theory; Vol. 2. Research; Vol. 3. Application (pp. 123-152). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. |
[13] |
Enright R. D., Gassin E. A., & Wu C. R. (1992). Forgiveness: A developmental view. Journal of Moral Education, 21(2), 99-114.
doi: 10.1080/0305724920210202 URL |
[14] |
Fu G., Heyman G. D., Qian M., Guo T., & Lee K. (2015). Young children with a positive reputation to maintain are less likely to cheat. Developmental Science, 19(2), 275-283.
doi: 10.1111/desc.2016.19.issue-2 URL |
[15] | Grocke P., Rossano F., & Tomasello M. (2019). Preschoolers consider (absent) others when choosing a distribution procedure. PLOS ONE, 14(8), Article e0221186. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0221186 |
[16] |
Günsoy C., Cross S. E., Uskul A. K., Adams G., & Gercek- Swing B. (2015). Avoid or fight back? Cultural differences in responses to conflict and the role of collectivism, honor, and enemy perception. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 46(8), 1081-1102.
doi: 10.1177/0022022115594252 URL |
[17] | Hair Jr J. F., Hult G. T. M., Ringle C. M., & Sarstedt M. (2017). A primer on partial least squares structural equation modeling (PLS-SEM) (2th ed. pp. 56-60). Sage publications. |
[18] |
Hook J. N., Worthington E. L., Jr., & Utsey S. O. (2009). Collectivism, forgiveness, and social harmony. The Counseling Psychologist, 37(6), 821-847.
doi: 10.1177/0011000008326546 URL |
[19] |
Izuma K., Saito D.N., Sadato N. (2010). The roles of the medial prefrontal cortex and striatum in reputation processing. Social Neuroscience, 5(2), 133-47.
doi: 10.1080/17470910903202559 pmid: 19705330 |
[20] |
Keltner D., & Anderson C. (2000). Saving face for darwin. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 9(6), 187-192.
doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.00091 URL |
[21] |
Leary M. R., Landel J. L., & Patton K. M. (1996). The motivated expression of embarrassment following a self presentational predicament. Journal of Personality, 64(3), 619-636.
doi: 10.1111/jopy.1996.64.issue-3 URL |
[22] | Li J., Wang W., Yu J., & Zhu L. (2016). Young children’s development of fairness preference. Frontiers in Psychology, 7. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01274 |
[23] |
Louie J. Y., Wang S., Fung J., & Lau A. (2014). Children’s emotional expressivity and teacher perceptions of social competence. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 39(6), 497-507.
doi: 10.1177/0165025414548775 URL |
[24] |
Ma F., Zeng D., Xu F., Compton B. J., & Heyman G. D. (2020). Delay of gratification as reputation management. Psychological Science, 31(9), 1174-1182.
doi: 10.1177/0956797620939940 pmid: 32840460 |
[25] |
Markus H. R., & Kitayama S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98(2), 224-253.
doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.98.2.224 URL |
[26] |
Martínez-Lozano V., Sánchez-Medina J. A., & Goudena P. P. (2011). A cross-cultural study of observed conflicts between young children. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 42(6), 895-907.
doi: 10.1177/0022022110381361 URL |
[27] |
McCullough M. E., Pedersen E. J., Tabak B. A., & Carter E. C. (2014). Conciliatory gestures promote forgiveness and reduce anger in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(30), 11211-11216.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1405072111 URL |
[28] |
Mills T. M. (1953). Power relations in three-person groups. American Sociological Review, 18(4), 351-357.
doi: 10.2307/2087546 URL |
[29] |
Nader-Grosbois N., Houssa M., & Mazzone S. (2013). How could theory of mind contribute to the differentiation of social adjustment profiles of children with externalizing behavior disorders and children with intellectual disabilities? Research in Developmental Disabilities, 34(9), 2642-2660.
doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2013.05.010 pmid: 23751304 |
[30] |
Ohtsubo Y., Matsunaga M., Himichi T., Suzuki K., Shibata E., Hori R., … Ohira H. (2019). Costly group apology communicates a group’s sincere “intention”. Social Neuroscience, 15(2), 244-254.
doi: 10.1080/17470919.2019.1697745 URL |
[31] |
Ohtsubo Y., Matsunaga M., Tanaka H., Suzuki K., Kobayashi F., Shibata E., … Ohira H. (2018). Costly apologies communicate conciliatory intention: An fMRI study on forgiveness in response to costly apologies. Evolution and Human Behavior, 39(2), 249-256.
doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2018.01.004 URL |
[32] |
Oostenbroek J., & Vaish A. (2018). The emergence of forgiveness in young children. Child Development, 90(6), 1969-1986.
doi: 10.1111/cdev.v90.6 URL |
[33] |
Richland L. E., Chan T.-K., Morrison R. G., & Au T. K.-F. (2010). Young children’s analogical reasoning across cultures: Similarities and differences. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 105(1-2), 146-153.
doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2009.08.003 pmid: 19896676 |
[34] |
Rochat P., Dias M. D. G., Guo Liping, Broesch T., Passos- Ferreira C., Winning A., & Berg B. (2009). Fairness in distributive justice by 3- and 5-year-olds across seven cultures. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 40(3), 416-442.
doi: 10.1177/0022022109332844 URL |
[35] |
Shah J. (2003). Automatic for the people: How representations of significant others implicitly affect goal pursuit. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(4), 661-681.
pmid: 12703642 |
[36] |
Shinohara A., Kanakogi Y., Okumura Y., & Kobayashi T. (2022). Children manage their reputation by caring about gossip. Social Development, 31(2), 455-465.
doi: 10.1111/sode.v31.2 URL |
[37] | Tamis-LeMonda C. S., Way N., Hughes D., Yoshikawa H., Kalman R. K., & Niwa E. Y. (2008). Parents’ goals for children: The dynamic coexistence of individualism and collectivism in cultures and individuals. Social Development, 17(1), 183-209. |
[38] |
Tang Y., Harris P. L., Pons F., Zou H., Zhang W., & Xu Q. (2018). The understanding of emotion among young Chinese children. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 42(5), 512-517.
doi: 10.1177/0165025417741366 URL |
[39] |
Vaish A., Carpenter M., & Tomasello M. (2011). Young children’s responses to guilt displays. Developmental Psychology, 47(5), 1248-1262.
doi: 10.1037/a0024462 URL |
[40] |
Vaish A., & Oostenbroek J. (2021). Preferential forgiveness: The impact of group membership and remorse on preschoolers’ forgiveness. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 151(5), 1132-1140.
doi: 10.1037/xge0001114 URL |
[41] |
Vaish A., & Savell S. (2022). Young children value recipients who display gratitude. Developmental Psychology, 58(4), 680-692.
doi: 10.1037/dev0001308 pmid: 35007110 |
[42] |
Van Hoorn J., Van Dijk E., Güroğlu B., & Crone E. A. (2016). Neural correlates of prosocial peer influence on public goods game donations during adolescence. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11(6), 923-933.
doi: 10.1093/scan/nsw013 pmid: 26865424 |
[43] |
Vlachou M., Andreou E., Botsoglou K., & Didaskalou E. (2011). Bully/victim problems among preschool children: A review of current research evidence. Educational Psychology Review, 23(3), 329-358.
doi: 10.1007/s10648-011-9153-z URL |
[44] | Wang X. N., Hao Y., & Su Y. J. (2019). Effect of competition and collaboration on preschoolers’ resource distribution: The role of theory of mind and inhibitory control. Psychological Development and Education, 35(4), 385-392. |
[王笑楠, 郝洋, 苏彦捷. (2019). 竞争和合作对学龄前儿童分配模式的影响: 心理理论和抑制控制的作用. 心理发展与教育, 35(4), 385-392.] | |
[45] | Weiner B. (1986). An attributional theory of motivation and emotion. New York: Springer. |
[46] |
Wu Z., Chen X., Gros-Louis J., & Su Y. (2018). “She is looking at me! Shall I share?” How Chinese and American preschoolers respond to eye gaze during sharing. Social Development, 27(2), 447-460.
doi: 10.1111/sode.2018.27.issue-2 URL |
[47] |
Yau J., Smetana J. G., & Metzger A. (2009). Young Chinese children's authority concepts. Social Development, 18(1), 210-229.
doi: 10.1111/sode.2009.18.issue-1 URL |
[48] |
Zajonc R. B. (1965). Social facilitation. Science, 149(3681), 269-274.
doi: 10.1126/science.149.3681.269 pmid: 14300526 |
[49] | Zhang Q. P., Liu J. L., Huang H., Li J., & Kou Y. (2012). Adolescents’ prosocial intention in conflict situations: The effect of anticipated significant others’ viewpoint. Psychological Development and Education, 28(4), 368-375. |
[张庆鹏, 刘静丽, 黄慧, 黎洁, 寇彧. (2012). 冲突情境中青少年的亲社会意图: 预期重要他人观点的影响. 心理发展与教育, 28(4), 368-375.] | |
[50] | Zhang T., Sun H., & Fu A. Q. (2012). Forgiveness in collectivism culture and its enlightenment to forgiveness intervention. Advances in Psychological Science, 20(2), 265-273. |
[张田, 孙卉, 傅安球. (2012). 集体主义背景下的宽恕研究及其对心理治疗的启示. 心理科学进展, 20(2), 265-273.] | |
[51] |
Zhang W. X., Li. X., Chen G. H., & Cao Y. M. (2021). The relationship between positive parenting and adolescent prosocial behaviour: The mediating role of empathy and the moderating role of the oxytocin receptor gene. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 53(9), 976-991.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00976 |
[张文新, 李曦, 陈光辉, 曹衍淼. (2021). 母亲积极教养与青少年亲社会行为:共情的中介作用与OXTR基因的调节作用. 心理学报, 53(9), 976-991.]
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00976 |
[1] | 李喆, 刘浙豫, 毛珂妤, 李婉婷, 李婷玉, 李晶. 3~6岁幼儿对智能语音助手在不同领域的知识性信任[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(9): 1411-1423. |
[2] | 贾成龙, 吴婷, 孙莉, 秦金亮. 依恋对象回应方式对不同依恋类型幼儿支持提供预期修正的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(3): 455-468. |
[3] | 王阳, 张琳爽, 崔楠楠, 吴岩. 4~6岁幼儿口语产生中句法结构和动词重复的作用:来自句法启动的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(10): 1608-1619. |
[4] | 姚尧, 陈晓湘. 音乐训练对4~5岁幼儿普通话声调范畴感知能力的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(4): 456-468. |
[5] | 贺晓玲, 陈俊. 3~5岁幼儿权力概念多重隐喻的认知发展[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(2): 149-161. |
[6] | 金心怡, 周冰欣, 孟斐. 3岁幼儿的二级观点采择及合作互动的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2019, 51(9): 1028-1039. |
[7] | 李泉, 宋亚男, 廉彬, 冯廷勇. 正念训练提升3~4岁幼儿注意力和执行功能[J]. 心理学报, 2019, 51(3): 324-336. |
[8] | 唐卫海, 钟汝波, 许晓旭, 刘希平. 面孔吸引力和信息正确性对幼儿选择性信任的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2019, 51(1): 71-84. |
[9] | 殷融. 旁观者对集体行动的支持:目标合理性、 行动策略及目标实现预期的作用[J]. 心理学报, 2018, 50(5): 558-571. |
[10] | 黄凤, 丁倩, 魏华, 洪建中. 帖子主题特征对虚拟社区知识分享行为的影响: 旁观者效应的视角[J]. 心理学报, 2018, 50(2): 226-234. |
[11] | 李婷玉, 刘黎, 李宜霖, 朱莉琪. 冲突情境下幼儿的选择性信任和信念修正[J]. 心理学报, 2018, 50(12): 1390-1399. |
[12] | 陈晓;高辛;周晖. 宽宏大量与睚眦必报:宽恕和报复对愤怒的降低作用[J]. 心理学报, 2017, 49(2): 241-252. |
[13] | 李婷玉;刘黎;朱莉琪. 4~6岁幼儿经济博弈中的信任行为及其影响因素[J]. 心理学报, 2017, 49(1): 17-27. |
[14] | 胡清芬;卢静. 幼儿在地图任务中对自我位置与自我朝向的结合[J]. 心理学报, 2016, 48(9): 1143-1150. |
[15] | 邢淑芬;梁熙;岳建宏;王争艳. 祖辈共同养育背景下多重依恋关系及对幼儿社会−情绪性发展的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2016, 48(5): 518-528. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||