[1] |
Boseovski J. J., & Thurman S. L. (2014). Evaluating and approaching a strange animal: Children’s trust in informant testimony. Child Development, 85(2), 824-834.
doi: 10.1111/cdev.12156
pmid: 24032359
|
[2] |
Clément F., Koenig M., & Harris P. (2004). The ontogenesis of trust. Mind & Language, 19(4), 360-379.
doi: 10.1111/mila.2004.19.issue-4
URL
|
[3] |
Corriveau K., & Harris P. L. (2009). Preschoolers continue to trust a more accurate informant 1 week after exposure to accuracy information. Developmental Science, 12(1), 188-193.
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00763.x
pmid: 19120427
|
[4] |
Corriveau K. H., Einav S., Robinson E. J., & Harris P. L. (2014). To the letter: Early readers trust print-based over oral instructions to guide their actions. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 32(3), 345-358.
doi: 10.1111/bjdp.12046
pmid: 24814164
|
[5] |
Danovitch J. H., & Alzahabi R. (2013). Children show selective trust in technological informants. Journal of Cognition and Development, 14(3), 499-513.
doi: 10.1080/15248372.2012.689391
URL
|
[6] |
Druga S., Williams R., Breazeal C., & Resnick M. (2017, June). "Hey Google is it ok if I eat you?" Initial explorations in child-agent interaction. In Proceedings of the 2017 conference on interaction design and children (pp. 595-600). Stanford California.
|
[7] |
Einav S., Levey A., Patel P., & Westwood A. (2020). Epistemic vigilance online: Textual inaccuracy and children’s selective trust in webpages. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 38(4), 566-579.
doi: 10.1111/bjdp.v38.4
URL
|
[8] |
Einav S., Robinson E. J., & Fox A. (2013). Take it as read: Origins of trust in knowledge gained from print. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 114(2), 262-274.
doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2012.09.016
pmid: 23151397
|
[9] |
Girouard-Hallam L. N., & Danovitch J. H. (2022a). Children’s trust in and learning from voice assistants. Developmental Psychology, 58(4), 646-661.
doi: 10.1037/dev0001318
URL
|
[10] |
Girouard-Hallam L. N., & Danovitch J. H. (2022b). Children’s judgments about asking for past, present, and future information from Google and a person. In Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, 44(44). California.
|
[11] |
Girouard-Hallam L. N., Streble H. M., & Danovitch J. H. (2021). Children's mental, social, and moral attributions toward a familiar digital voice assistant. Human Behavior and Emerging Technologies, 3(5), 1118-1131.
doi: 10.1002/hbe2.v3.5
URL
|
[12] |
Jaswal V. K. (2006). Preschoolers favor the creator's label when reasoning about an artifact's function. Cognition, 99(3), 83-92.
pmid: 16185679
|
[13] |
Jaswal V. K., Carrington Croft A., Setia A. R., & Cole C. A. (2010). Young children have a specific, highly robust bias to trust testimony. Psychological Science, 21(10), 1541-1547.
doi: 10.1177/0956797610383438
pmid: 20855905
|
[14] |
Jaswal V. K., Pérez-Edgar K., Kondrad R. L., Palmquist C. M., Cole C. A., & Cole C. E. (2014). Can't stop believing: Inhibitory control and resistance to misleading testimony. Developmental Science, 17(6), 965-976.
doi: 10.1111/desc.12187
pmid: 24806881
|
[15] |
Kahn Jr P. H., Gary H. E., & Shen S. (2013). Children's social relationships with current and near-future robots. Child Development Perspectives, 7(1), 32-37.
doi: 10.1111/cdep.12011
URL
|
[16] |
Keil F. C., Stein C., Webb L., Billings V. D., & Rozenblit L. (2008). Discerning the division of cognitive labor: An emerging understanding of how knowledge is clustered in other minds. Cognitive Science, 32(2), 259-300.
doi: 10.1080/03640210701863339
pmid: 19759842
|
[17] |
Kinzler K. D., Corriveau K. H., & Harris P. L. (2011). Children’s selective trust in native-accented speakers. Developmental Science, 14(1), 106-111.
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00965.x
URL
|
[18] |
Koenig M. A., Clément F., & Harris P. L. (2004). Trust in testimony: Children’s use of true and false statements. Psychological Science, 15(10), 694-698.
doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00742.x
URL
|
[19] |
Koenig M. A., & Harris P. L. (2005). Preschoolers mistrust ignorant and inaccurate speakers. Child Development, 76(6), 1261-1277.
pmid: 16274439
|
[20] |
Landrum A. R., Mills C. M., & Johnston A. M. (2013). When do children trust the expert? Benevolence information influences children's trust more than expertise. Developmental Science, 16(4), 622-638.
doi: 10.1111/desc.12059
pmid: 23786479
|
[21] |
Li Q.-G., Heyman G. D., Xu F., & Lee K. (2014). Young children’s use of honesty as a basis for selective trust. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 117, 59-72.
doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2013.09.002
URL
|
[22] |
Li T. Y., Liu L., Li Y. L., & Zhu L. Q. (2018). Preschoolers’ selective trust and belief revision in conflicting situation. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 50(12), 1390-1399.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2018.01390
URL
|
|
[李婷玉, 刘黎, 李宜霖, 朱莉琪. (2018). 冲突情境下幼儿的选择性信任和信念修正. 心理学报, 50(12), 1390-1399.]
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2018.01390
|
[23] |
Liu D., Vanderbilt K. E., & Heyman G. D. (2013). Selective trust: Children's use of intention and outcome of past testimony. Developmental Psychology, 49(3), 439-445.
doi: 10.1037/a0031615
pmid: 23339589
|
[24] |
Moar J., & Escherich M. (2020). Hey siri, how will you make money. Juniper Research. Retrieved January 27, 2023, from https://www.juniperresearch.com/whitepapers/hey-siri-how-will-you-make-money
|
[25] |
Murray G. W. (2021). Who is more trustworthy, Alexa or mom?: Children’s selective trust in a digital age. Technology, Mind, and Behavior, 2(3), 1-12.
|
[26] |
Okanda M., & Itakura S. (2010). Do bilingual children exhibit a yes bias to yes-no questions? Relationship between children’s yes bias and verbal ability. International Journal of Bilingualism, 14(2), 227-235.
doi: 10.1177/1367006910363061
URL
|
[27] |
Oranç C., & Ruggeri A. (2021). “Alexa, let me ask you something different” children’s adaptive information search with voice assistants. Human Behavior and Emerging Technologies, 3(4), 595-605.
doi: 10.1002/hbe2.v3.4
URL
|
[28] |
Rideout V. (2017). The common sense census: Media use by kids age zero to eight. San Francisco, CA: Common Sense Media.
|
[29] |
Rideout V. (2020). The common sense census: Media use by kids age zero to eight. San Francisco, CA: Common Sense Media.
|
[30] |
Tong Y., Wang F., & Danovitch J. (2020). The role of epistemic and social characteristics in children’s selective trust: Three meta-analyses. Developmental Science, 23(2), e12895.
|
[31] |
Toyama N. (2017). Development of the selection of trusted informants in the domain of illness. Infant & Child Development, 26(6), e2039.
|
[32] |
Xu F., Evans A. D., Li C., Li Q., Heyman G., & Lee K. (2013). The role of honesty and benevolence in children’s judgments of trustworthiness. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 37(3), 257-265.
doi: 10.1177/0165025413479861
URL
|
[33] |
Xu Y., & Warschauer M. (2020, April). What are you talking to?: Understanding children's perceptions of conversational agents. In Proceedings of the 2020 CHI conference on human factors in computing systems (pp. 1-13). Honolulu HI.
|
[34] |
Yarosh S., Thompson S., Watson K., Chase A., Senthilkumar A., Yuan Y., & Brush A. B. (2018, June). Children asking questions: Speech interface reformulations and personification preferences. In Proceedings of the 17th ACM conference on interaction design and children (pp. 300-312). Trondheim Norway.
|
[35] |
Zhang J., Li T. M., Zheng Q. T., Zhou S. Z., & Xiong H. X. (2020). Preschoolers’ selective trust characteristics of knowledge in different domains in the clue conflict situation. Journal of Psychological Science, 43(2), 371-377.
|
|
[张璟, 李婷敏, 郑沁婷, 周双珠, 熊红星. (2020). 线索冲突情境下幼儿对不同领域知识的选择性信任特点. 心理科学, 43(2), 371-377.]
|
[36] |
Zhang Y. H., & Zhu L. Q. (2014). Epistemic trust: How preschoolers selectively learn from others. Advances in Psychological Science, 22(1), 86-96.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2014.00086
|
|
[张耀华, 朱莉琪. (2014). 认识性信任: 学龄前儿童的选择性学习. 心理科学进展, 22(1), 86-96.]
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2014.00086
|