心理学报 ›› 2023, Vol. 55 ›› Issue (7): 1099-1114.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2023.01099
收稿日期:
2021-12-30
发布日期:
2023-04-21
出版日期:
2023-07-25
通讯作者:
袁博, E-mail: yuanbopsy@gmail.com;李伟强, E-mail: liweiqiang@nbu.edu.cn
基金资助:
YUAN Bo(), WANG Xiaoping, YIN Jun, LI Weiqiang()
Received:
2021-12-30
Online:
2023-04-21
Published:
2023-07-25
摘要:
基于联结学习理论(associative learning theory), 通过3项实验考察了跨情境(公平−信任)的刺激泛化在面孔信任形成中的作用。实验1a和实验1b分别从直接互动和观察学习视角, 发现了面孔信任形成中跨情境的刺激泛化效应(stimulus generalization effect), 即相比于中等不公平条件, 随着被信任者的面孔与先前互动中公平(不公平)分配者面孔相似度的增加, 个体对其信任程度逐渐增加(降低); 并且这一效应具有不对称性(asymmetry), 对不公平分配者面孔的泛化强度高于对公平分配者面孔的泛化强度。采用漂移扩散模型(Drift-Diffusion Modeling, DDM)分析发现, 不公平条件下的漂移率v显著小于中等不公平或公平条件下的漂移率v, 且大多分布在小于0区间; 表明在对与先前互动中不公平分配者面孔相似的陌生面孔进行信任决策时, 个体更倾向累积不信任的证据。实验2结果发现, 行为意图在刺激泛化效应的产生中起到调节作用; 在无意图条件下, 上述跨情境的刺激泛化效应消失。上述结果表明, 个体采用联结学习机制将不同情境中习得的刺激价值联结泛化到新的互动情境中, 进而指导随后的信任决策。
中图分类号:
袁博, 王晓萍, 尹军, 李伟强. (2023). 跨情境的刺激泛化在面孔信任形成中的作用:基于直接互动与观察学习的视角. 心理学报, 55(7), 1099-1114.
YUAN Bo, WANG Xiaoping, YIN Jun, LI Weiqiang. (2023). The role of cross-situational stimulus generalization in the formation of trust towards face: A perspective based on direct and observational learning. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 55(7), 1099-1114.
[1] |
Allidina, S., & Cunningham, W. A. (2021). Avoidance begets avoidance: A computational account of negative stereotype persistence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 150(10), 2078-2099.
doi: 10.1037/xge0001037 URL |
[2] |
Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D. J., & Bates, D. M. (2008). Mixed- effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language, 59(4), 390-412.
doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.12.005 URL |
[3] |
Bateson, M., Brilot, B., & Nettle, D. (2011). Anxiety: An evolutionary approach. The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 56(12), 707-715.
doi: 10.1177/070674371105601202 URL |
[4] |
Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Finkenauer, C., & Vohs, K. D. (2001). Bad is stronger than good. Review of General Psychology, 5(4), 323-370.
doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.5.4.323 URL |
[5] |
Buckholtz, J. W., Asplund, C. L., Dux, P. E., Zald, D. H., Gore, J. C., Jones, O. D., & Marois, R. (2008). The neural correlates of third-party punishment. Neuron, 60(5), 930-940.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.10.016 pmid: 19081385 |
[6] | Bürkner, P. C. (2017). Brms: An R package for Bayesian multilevel models using Stan. Journal of Statistical Software, 80(1), 1-28. |
[7] |
Chang, L. J., & Sanfey, A. G. (2013). Great expectations: Neural computations underlying the use of social norms in decision- making. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8(3), 277-284.
doi: 10.1093/scan/nsr094 pmid: 22198968 |
[8] | Cushman, F., Dreber, A., Wang, Y., & Costa, J. (2009). Accidental outcomes guide punishment in a “trembling hand” game. PloS one, 4(8), e6699. |
[9] |
Cushman, F., Sheketoff, R., Wharton, S., & Carey, S. (2013). The development of intent-based moral judgment. Cognition, 127(1), 6-21.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2012.11.008 pmid: 23318350 |
[10] | Dayan, P., & Berridge, K. C. (2014). Model-based and model- free Pavlovian reward learning: Revaluation, revision, and revelation. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 14(2), 473-492. |
[11] | de Boeck, P., Bakker, M., Zwitser, R., Nivard, M., Hofman, A., Tuerlinckx, F., & Partchev, I. (2011). The estimation of item response models with the lmer function from the lme4 package in R. Journal of Statistical Software, 39(12), 1-28. |
[12] |
Dufwenberg, M., & Kirchsteiger, G. (2004). A theory of sequential reciprocity. Games and Economic Behavior, 47(2), 268-298.
doi: 10.1016/j.geb.2003.06.003 URL |
[13] |
Earley, R. L. (2010). Social eavesdropping and the evolution of conditional cooperation and cheating strategies. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 365(1553), 2675-2686.
doi: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0147 URL |
[14] | Erev, I., & Roth, A. E. (1998). Predicting how people play games: Reinforcement learning in experimental games with unique, mixed strategy equilibria. American Economic Review, 88(4), 848-881. |
[15] |
Fareri, D. S., Chang, L. J., & Delgado, M. R. (2012). Effects of direct social experience on trust decisions and neural reward circuitry. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 6, 148.
doi: 10.3389/fnins.2012.00148 pmid: 23087604 |
[16] |
FeldmanHall, O., Dunsmoor, J. E., Tompary, A., Hunter, L. E., Todorov, A., & Phelps, E. A. (2018). Stimulus generalization as a mechanism for learning to trust. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115(7), 1690-1697.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1800256115 URL |
[17] |
Forstmann, B. U., Ratcliff, R., & Wagenmakers, E.-J. (2016). Sequential sampling models in cognitive neuroscience: Advantages, applications, and extensions. Annual Review of Psychology, 67(1), 641-666.
doi: 10.1146/psych.2015.67.issue-1 URL |
[18] |
Frith, C. D., & Frith, U. (2012). Mechanisms of social cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 287-313.
doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100449 pmid: 21838544 |
[19] |
Gao, Q. L., & Zhou, Y. (2021). Psychological and neural mechanisms of trust formation: A perspective from computational modeling based on the decision of investor in the trust game. Advances in Psychological Science, 29(1), 178-189.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2021.00178 |
[高青林, 周媛. (2021). 计算模型视角下信任形成的心理和神经机制——基于信任博弈中投资者的角度. 心理科学进展, 29(1), 178-189.]
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2021.00178 |
|
[20] |
Gao, W., Cao, B., Shan, S., Chen, X., Zhou, D., Zhang, X., & Zhao, D. (2007). The CAS-PEAL large-scale Chinese face database and baseline evaluations. IEEE Transactions on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics-Part A: Systems and Humans, 38(1), 149-161.
doi: 10.1109/TSMCA.2007.909557 URL |
[21] |
Gawronski, B., & Quinn, K. A. (2013). Guilty by mere similarity: Assimilative effects of facial resemblance on automatic evaluation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49(1), 120-125.
doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2012.07.016 URL |
[22] |
Germar, M., Schlemmer, A., Krug, K., Voss, A., & Mojzisch, A. (2014). Social influence and perceptual decision making: A diffusion model analysis. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 40(2), 217-231.
doi: 10.1177/0146167213508985 pmid: 24154917 |
[23] |
Grady, C. L., Hongwanishkul, D., Keightley, M., Lee, W., & Hasher, L. (2007). The effect of age on memory for emotional faces. Neuropsychology, 21(3), 371-380.
doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.21.3.371 pmid: 17484600 |
[24] |
Johnson, D. J., Hopwood, C. J., Cesario, J., & Pleskac, T. J. (2017). Advancing research on cognitive processes in social and personality psychology: A hierarchical drift diffusion model primer. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 8(4), 413-423.
doi: 10.1177/1948550617703174 URL |
[25] |
Kocsor, F., & Bereczkei, T. (2017). First impressions of strangers rely on generalization of behavioral traits associated with previously seen facial features. Current Psychology, 36(3), 385-391.
doi: 10.1007/s12144-016-9427-1 URL |
[26] |
Kraus, M. W., Chen, S., Lee, V. A., & Straus, L. D. (2010). Transference occurs across group boundaries. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46(6), 1067-1073.
doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2010.05.003 URL |
[27] | Luo, Q., Huang, L., Hou, Q., Aimaitijiang, R., Zhou, M., Zhou, X., & Chen, S. (2020). Generalization effect of gossip on interpersonal trust. Journal of Psychological Science, 43(1), 165-171. |
[罗秋铃, 黄露霖, 侯庆辉, 热米拉·艾买提江, 周梦哲, 周晓林, 陈双. (2020). 传言对人际信任影响的泛化效应. 心理科学, 43(1), 165-171.] | |
[28] |
Ma, Q., Meng, L., Zhang, Z., Xu, Q., Wang, Y., & Shen, Q. (2015). You did not mean it: Perceived good intentions alleviate sense of unfairness. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 96(3), 183-190.
doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2015.03.011 pmid: 25863263 |
[29] |
Mcauliffe, K., Jordan, J. J., & Warneken, F. (2015). Costly third-party punishment in young children. Cognition, 134, 1-10.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.08.013 pmid: 25460374 |
[30] | Milinski, M. (2016). Reputation, a universal currency for human social interactions. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 371(1687), 20150100. |
[31] |
Mineka, S., & Öhman, A. (2002). Phobias and preparedness: The selective, automatic, and encapsulated nature of fear. Biological Psychiatry, 52(10), 927-937.
pmid: 12437934 |
[32] |
Moretti, L., & Di Pellegrino, G. (2010). Disgust selectively modulates reciprocal fairness in economic interactions. Emotion, 10(2), 169-180.
doi: 10.1037/a0017826 pmid: 20364893 |
[33] |
Offerman, T. (2002). Hurting hurts more than helping helps. European Economic Review, 46(8), 1423-1437.
doi: 10.1016/S0014-2921(01)00176-3 URL |
[34] |
Olsson, A., Knapska, E., & Lindström, B. (2020). The neural and computational systems of social learning. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 21(4), 197-212.
doi: 10.1038/s41583-020-0276-4 pmid: 32221497 |
[35] |
Olsson, A., Nearing, K. I., & Phelps, E. A. (2007). Learning fears by observing others: The neural systems of social fear transmission. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2(1), 3-11.
doi: 10.1093/scan/nsm005 pmid: 18985115 |
[36] | Peirce, J. W. (2009). Generating stimuli for neuroscience using PsychoPy. Frontiers in Neuroinformatics, 2, 10. |
[37] |
Radell, M. L., Sanchez, R., Weinflash, N., & Myers, C. E. (2016). The personality trait of behavioral inhibition modulates perceptions of moral character and performance during the trust game: Behavioral results and computational modeling. PeerJ, 4, e1631.
doi: 10.7717/peerj.1631 URL |
[38] |
Ratcliff, R., & McKoon, G. (2008). The diffusion decision model: Theory and data for two-choice decision tasks. Neural Computation, 20(4), 873-922.
doi: 10.1162/neco.2008.12-06-420 pmid: 18085991 |
[39] |
Rescorla, R. A. (1976). Stimulus generalization: Some predictions from a model of Pavlovian conditioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 2(1), 88-96.
doi: 10.1037/0097-7403.2.1.88 URL |
[40] |
Rescorla, R. A., & Solomon, R. L. (1967). Two-process learning theory: Relationships between Pavlovian conditioning and instrumental learning. Psychological Review, 74(3), 151-182.
doi: 10.1037/h0024475 pmid: 5342881 |
[41] |
Rousseau, D. M., Sitkin, S. B., Burt, R. S., & Camerer, C. (1998). Not so different after all: A cross-discipline view of trust. Academy of Management Review, 23(3), 393-404.
doi: 10.5465/amr.1998.926617 URL |
[42] |
Rozin, P., & Royzman, E. B. (2001). Negativity bias, negativity dominance, and contagion. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 5(4), 296-320.
doi: 10.1207/S15327957PSPR0504_2 URL |
[43] |
Ruff, C. C., & Fehr, E. (2014). The neurobiology of rewards and values in social decision making. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 15(8), 549-562.
doi: 10.1038/nrn3776 pmid: 24986556 |
[44] |
Schechtman, E., Laufer, O., & Paz, R. (2010). Negative valence widens generalization of learning. Journal of Neuroscience, 30(31), 10460-10464.
doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2377-10.2010 pmid: 20685988 |
[45] |
Shang, J., & Li, Y. (2020). The effects of participants’ sex and the facial trustworthiness of proposers on third-party decision-making in a dictator game. PsyCh Journal, 9(6), 877-884.
doi: 10.1002/pchj.v9.6 URL |
[46] |
Singer, T., Seymour, B., O'Doherty, J. P., Stephan, K. E., Dolan, R. J., & Frith, C. D. (2006). Empathic neural responses are modulated by the perceived fairness of others. Nature, 439(7075), 466-469.
doi: 10.1038/nature04271 |
[47] | Son, J.-Y., Bhandari, A., & FeldmanHall, O. (2019). Crowdsourcing punishment: Individuals reference group preferences to inform their own punitive decisions. Scientific Reports, 9(1), 11625. |
[48] |
Sullivan, J. L., & Transue, J. E. (1999). The psychological underpinnings of democracy: A selective review of research on political tolerance, interpersonal trust, and social capital. Annual Review of Psychology, 50(1), 625-650.
doi: 10.1146/psych.1999.50.issue-1 URL |
[49] |
Sun, Z., Ye, C., He, Z., & Yu, W. (2020). Behavioral intention promotes generalized reciprocity: Evidence from the dictator game. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 772.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00772 pmid: 32425857 |
[50] |
Sutter, M. (2007). Outcomes versus intentions: On the nature of fair behavior and its development with age. Journal of Economic Psychology, 28(1), 69-78.
doi: 10.1016/j.joep.2006.09.001 URL |
[51] |
Todorov, A., Baron, S. G., & Oosterhof, N. N. (2008). Evaluating face trustworthiness: A model based approach. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 3(2), 119-127.
doi: 10.1093/scan/nsn009 pmid: 19015102 |
[52] |
Vaish, A., Hepach, R., & Tomasello, M. (2018). The specificity of reciprocity: Young children reciprocate more generously to those who intentionally benefit them. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 167, 336-353.
doi: S0022-0965(17)30432-0 pmid: 29227851 |
[53] |
van’t Wout, M., Kahn, R. S., Sanfey, A. G., & Aleman, A. (2006). Affective state and decision-making in the ultimatum game. Experimental Brain Research, 169(4), 564-568.
doi: 10.1007/s00221-006-0346-5 pmid: 16489438 |
[54] |
Verosky, S. C., & Todorov, A. (2010). Generalization of affective learning about faces to perceptually similar faces. Psychological Science, 21(6), 779-785.
doi: 10.1177/0956797610371965 pmid: 20483821 |
[55] |
Verosky, S. C., & Todorov, A. (2013). When physical similarity matters: Mechanisms underlying affective learning generalization to the evaluation of novel faces. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49(4), 661-669.
doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2013.02.004 URL |
[56] |
Wang, T., Chen, Y., & Lu, J. (2020). The generalization effect in gap evaluation: How large is the gap between you and me? Acta Psychologica Sinica, 52(11), 1327-1339.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2020.01327 |
[王天鸿, 陈宇琦, 陆静怡. (2020). 差距知觉的泛化效应:我和你之间的差距有多大? 心理学报, 52(11), 1327-1339.]
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2020.01327 |
|
[57] |
Wiecki, T. V., Sofer, I., & Frank, M. J. (2013). HDDM: Hierarchical Bayesian estimation of the Drift-Diffusion Model in Python. Frontiers in Neuroinformatics, 7, 14. doi:10.3389/fninf.2013.00014
doi: 10.3389/fninf.2013.00014 pmid: 23935581 |
[58] |
Willis, J., & Todorov, A. (2006). First impressions: Making up your mind after a 100-ms exposure to a face. Psychological Science, 17(7), 592-598.
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01750.x pmid: 16866745 |
[59] |
Wilson, R. K., & Eckel, C. C. (2006). Judging a book by its cover: Beauty and expectations in the trust game. Political Research Quarterly, 59(2), 189-202.
doi: 10.1177/106591290605900202 URL |
[60] |
Xiang, T., Lohrenz, T., & Montague, P. R. (2013). Computational substrates of norms and their violations during social exchange. Journal of Neuroscience, 33(3), 1099-1108.
doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1642-12.2013 pmid: 23325247 |
[61] |
Yu, H., Siegel, J. Z., Clithero, J. A., & Crockett, M. J. (2021). How peer influence shapes value computation in moral decision-making. Cognition, 211, 104641.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2021.104641 URL |
[62] |
Zak, P. J., & Knack, S. (2001). Trust and growth. The Economic Journal, 111(470), 295-321.
doi: 10.1111/1468-0297.00609 URL |
[63] |
Zebrowitz, L. A., & Montepare, J. M. (2008). Social psychological face perception: Why appearance matters. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 2(3), 1497-1517.
doi: 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2008.00109.x pmid: 20107613 |
[64] |
Zhang, Y., Li, H., & Wu, Y. (2020). The application of computational modelling in the studies of moral cognition. Advances in Psychological Science, 28(7), 1042-1055.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2020.01042 |
[张银花, 李红, 吴寅. (2020). 计算模型在道德认知研究中的应用. 心理科学进展, 28(7), 1042-1055.]
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2020.01042 |
|
[65] |
Zheng, X., Guo, W., Chen, M., Jin, J., & Yin, J. (2020). Influence of the valence of social actions on attentional capture: Focus on helping and hindering actions. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 52(5), 584-596.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2020.00584 |
[郑旭涛, 郭文姣, 陈满, 金佳, 尹军. (2020). 社会行为的效价信息对注意捕获的影响: 基于帮助和阻碍行为的探讨. 心理学报, 52(5), 584-596.]
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2020.00584 |
[1] | 王重鸣,邓靖松. 团队中信任形成的映象决策机制[J]. 心理学报, 2007, 39(2): 321-327. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||