心理学报 ›› 2023, Vol. 55 ›› Issue (3): 421-434.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2023.00421
• "中国人应对历史危机的心理特征与行为表现"专栏 • 上一篇 下一篇
收稿日期:
2021-03-31
发布日期:
2022-12-22
出版日期:
2023-03-25
通讯作者:
纪丽君,吴莹
E-mail:lijunji@queensu.ca;nolung@163.com
基金资助:
JI Li-Jun1(), WU Ying2(), YANG Yiyin3
Received:
2021-03-31
Online:
2022-12-22
Published:
2023-03-25
Contact:
JI Li-Jun, WU Ying
E-mail:lijunji@queensu.ca;nolung@163.com
摘要:
时间知觉是社会结构和文化传统的映射, 具有深刻的社会文化意涵。本文聚焦时间知觉广度这一概念, 探讨中国人的时间知觉广度及其对自我连续性和社会变化观等心理过程的影响。通过综合讨论诸多实验发现提出, 中国人具有较大的时间知觉广度, 包括具有显著的过去取向和未来取向。较大的时间知觉广度提升了中国人跨时间的自我连续性, 并且影响着中国人看待世事变化的变化观, 这些特点又进一步影响到其它心理过程, 例如对苦难和幸福的解读, 在股票市场中的决策等。在一定程度上, 这些特点可能是中国人在应对不确定性和重大危机时表现出更强的心理韧性和复原力的心理基础。本研究还指出中国人时间知觉广度具有独特的文化意义, 包括整体性思维、过去现在未来互嵌互动和双向延展、与自我连续性和社会结构相关联以及具有道德属性等。基于此, 本研究提出了时间知觉广度的文化心理比较模型, 并分析了时间知觉广度对自我同一性和文化同一性的影响结果。希望本文能为进一步研究“中国人应对历史危机的心理特征与行为表现”提供科学依据。
纪丽君, 吴莹, 杨宜音. (2023). 中国人的时间知觉广度. 心理学报, 55(3), 421-434.
JI Li-Jun, WU Ying, YANG Yiyin. (2023). The breath of temporal information focus among Chinese people. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 55(3), 421-434.
[1] |
Avnet T., & Sellier A. L. (2011). Clock time versus event time: Temporal culture or self-regulation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47(3), 665-667.
doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2011.01.006 URL |
[2] |
Bond M. H. (1988). Finding universal dimensions of individual variation in multicultural studies of values: The Rokeach and Chinese value surveys. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55(6), 1009-1015.
doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.55.6.1009 URL |
[3] | Briley D. A. (2009). Looking forward, looking back:Cultural differences and similarities in time orientation. In E. R. S. Wyer, C. Y. Chiu, & Y. Y. Hong(Eds.),Understanding culture: Theory, research and application (pp. 311-318). New York, NY: Psychology Press. |
[4] | Chandler M. (1994). Self-continuity in suicidal and non- suicidal adolescents. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 64(1), 55-70. |
[5] | Chandler M. J., Lalonde C. E., Sokol B., & Hallett D. (2003). Personal persistence, identity development, and suicide: A study of native and non-native North American adolescents. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 68(2), 1-156. |
[6] |
Chen C., & Stevenson H. W. (1995). Motivation and mathematics achievement: A comparative study of Asian-American, Caucasian-American, and East Asian high school students. Child Development, 66(4), 1215-1234.
doi: 10.2307/1131808 URL |
[7] |
Chen M. K. (2013). The effect of language on economic behavior: Evidence from savings rates, health behaviors, and retirement assets. American Economic Review, 103(2), 690-731.
pmid: 29524925 |
[8] | Cheng H., & Schweitzer J. C. (1996). Cultural values reflected in Chinese and US television commercials. Journal of Advertising Research, 36(3), 27-46. |
[9] |
Cheng H. P., O’ Leary E., & Page R. C. (1995). A cross-cultural comparison of the worldviews of American, Chinese (from Taiwan), and Irish graduate counseling students and implications for counseling. Counseling and Values, 40(1), 45-54.
doi: 10.1002/j.2161-007X.1995.tb00386.x URL |
[10] |
Cheung F. M., Leung K., Zhang J. X., Sun H. F., Gan Y. Q., Song W. Z., & Xie D. (2001). Indigenous Chinese personality constructs: Is the Five-Factor Model complete? Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 32(4), 407-433.
doi: 10.1177/0022022101032004003 URL |
[11] |
Cheung F. M., van de Vijver, F. J. R., & Leong, F. T. L. (2011). Toward a new approach to the study of personality in culture. American Psychologist, 66(7), 593-603.
doi: 10.1037/a0022389 pmid: 21261408 |
[12] |
Choi I., Dalal R., Kim-Prieto C., & Park H. (2003). Culture and judgment of causal relevance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(1), 46-59.
pmid: 12518970 |
[13] | de Grazia S. (1972). Time and work. In H. Yaker, H. Osmond, & F. Cheek, (Eds.), The future of time (p. 467). Garden City, NY: Doubleday & Co. |
[14] | Du W. M. (2002). Three tones in Chinese philosophy. In Q. Y. Guo, & W. L. Zheng(Eds.). The collected writings of Du Weiming (Vol. 5). Wuhan, China: Wuhan Publishing House. |
[ 杜维明. (2002). 试谈中国哲学中的三个基调. 见: 郭齐勇, 郑文龙 (编), 杜维明文集(卷五). 武汉出版社.] | |
[15] |
Dweck C. S. (2017). Is psychology headed in the right direction? Yes, No, and Maybe. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 12, 656-659.
doi: 10.1177/1745691616687747 pmid: 28727965 |
[16] |
Fang X., Sauter D. A., & Van Kleef G. A. (2018). Seeing mixed emotions: The specificity of emotion perception from static and dynamic facial expressions across cultures. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 49(1), 130-148.
doi: 10.1177/0022022117736270 pmid: 29386689 |
[17] | Filipcova B., & Filipee J. (1986). Society and concepts of time. International Social Science Journal, 107(3), 19-32. |
[18] |
Gao X. (2016). Cultural differences between East Asian and North American in temporal orientation. Review of General Psychology, 20(1), 118-127.
doi: 10.1037/gpr0000070 URL |
[19] | He G. B., Zhang W., & Cheng L. (2010). The impact of Chinese cultural values on happiness: Mediating effect of future orientation. Chinese Journal of Applied Psychology. 16(3), 250-256. |
[ 何贵兵, 张玮, 成龙. (2010). 中国文化价值观和未来时间取向对幸福感的影响. 应用心理学, 16(3), 250-256.] | |
[20] | Hofstede G. (2001). Culture’s consequences: Comparing values, behaviors, institutions, An organizations across nations. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.n |
[21] |
Hong Y. Y., Morris M. W., Chiu C. Y., & Benet-Martinez V. (2000). Multicultural minds: A dynamic constructivist approach to culture and cognition. American Psychologist. 55(7), 709-720.
doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.55.7.709 pmid: 10916861 |
[22] | Hsu F. L. K. (1949). Under the ancestors’ shadow: Chinese culture and personality. New York: Columbia University Press. |
[23] | Hsu F. L. K. (1960). Cultural differences between East and West and their significance for world today. Tsing Hua Journal of Chinese Studies, 2(1), 216-237. |
[ 许烺光. (1960). 东西方文化的差异及其对今日世界的意义. 清华学报, 2(1), 216-237.] | |
[24] | Huang G. G. (2006). Confucian relationalism: Cultural reflection and paradigm reconstruction. Beijing: Peking University Press. |
[ 黄光国. (2006). 儒家关系主义: 文化反思与典范重建. 北京: 北京大学出版社.] | |
[25] | Huang S., & Chen J. Y. (2019). Revisiting the extended present hypothesis: Chinese speakers’ perception of time. Chinese Journal of Psychology, 61(2), 131-150. |
[26] |
Huang X. T., Sun C. H., & Hu W. F. (1998). The psychological structure of past time. Journal of Psychological Science, 21(1), 1-4.
doi: 10.52547/JPS.21.109.1 URL |
[ 黄希庭, 孙承惠, 胡维芳. (1998). 过去时间的心理结构. 心理科学, 21(1), 1-4.] | |
[27] |
Jegadeesh N., & Titman S. (1993). Returns to buying winners and selling losers: Implications for stock market efficiency. The Journal of Finance, 48(1), 65-91.
doi: 10.1111/j.1540-6261.1993.tb04702.x URL |
[28] | Ji L. J. (2005). Culture and lay theories of change. In R. M. Sorrentino, D. Cohen, J. Olson, & M. Z. Hillsdale (Eds.), Culture and ssocial bbehavior: The ttenth Ontario ssymposium (pp. 117-135). N J: Lawrence Erlbaum. |
[29] |
Ji L. J. (2008). The leopard cannot change his spots, or can he? Culture and the development of lay theories. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34(5), 613-622.
doi: 10.1177/0146167207313935 URL |
[30] | Ji L. J., & Chan E. (2017). Chinese thinking styles and religion. In R. G. Hornbeck, J. L. Barrett, & M. Kang(Eds.), Religious ccognition in China (pp. 35-54). Springer International. |
[31] |
Ji L. J., Guo T., Zhang Z., & Messervey D. (2009). Looking into the past: Cultural differences in perception and representation of past information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96(4), 761-769.
doi: 10.1037/a0014498 URL |
[32] |
Ji L. J, Hong E., Guo T., Zhang Z., Su Y., & Li Y. (2019). Culture, psychological proximity to the past and future, and self-continuity. European Journal of Social Psychology, 49(4), 735-747.
doi: 10.1002/ejsp.2544 |
[33] |
Ji L. J., Imtiaz F., Su Y., Zhang Z., Bowie A., & Chang B. (2022). Culture, aging, self-continuity, and life satisfaction. Journal of Happiness Studies, 23(8), 3843-3864.
doi: 10.1007/s10902-022-00568-5 URL |
[34] |
Ji L. J., Khei M., Yap S., Wang X., Zhang Z., & Hou Y. (2021). Cultural differences in the construal of suffering and the COVID-19 pandemic. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 12(6), 1039-1047.
doi: 10.1177/1948550620958807 URL |
[35] |
Ji L. J., Nisbett R. E., & Su. Y. (2001). Culture, change and prediction. Psychological Science, 12(6), 450-456.
pmid: 11760130 |
[36] |
Ji L. J., Peng K., & Nisbett R. E. (2000). Culture, control, and perception of relationships in the environment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78(5), 943-955.
doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.78.5.943 pmid: 10821200 |
[37] |
Ji L. J., Vaughan-Johnston T., Zhang Z., Jacobson J., Zhang N., & Huang X. (2021). Context and cultural differences in positive thinking. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 52(5), 449-467.
doi: 10.1177/00220221211020442 URL |
[38] |
Ji L. J., Zhang Z., & Guo T. (2008). To buy or to sell: Cultural differences in stock market decisions based on price trends. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 21(4), 399-413.
doi: 10.1002/bdm.595 URL |
[39] |
Ji L. J., Zhang Z., Usborne E., & Guan Y. (2004). Optimism across cultures: In response to the severe acute respiratory syndrome outbreak. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 7(1), 25-34.
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-839X.2004.00132.x URL |
[40] |
Jiang D. L., Cheung W. Y., Wildschut T., & Sedikides C. (2021). Nostalgia, reflection, brooding: Psychological benefits and autobiographical memory functions. Consciousness and Cognition, 90(8), 103107.
doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2021.103107 URL |
[41] | Lampinen J. M., Odegard T. N., & Leding J. K. (2004). Diachronic disunity. In D. R. Beike, J. M. Lampinen, & D. A. Behrend(Eds.), The self and memory (pp. 227-253). New York: Psychology Press. |
[42] | Levine R. (1997). A geography of time. New York: Basic Books. |
[43] | Li J. (2015). Cultural foundations of learning: East and West. Shanghai: East China Normal University Press. |
[ 李瑾. (2015). 文化溯源: 东方与西方的学习理念. 上海: 华东师范大学出版社.] | |
[44] |
Liu C., & Lee Y. (2001). Does the momentum strategy work universally? Evidence from the Japanese stock market. Asia-Pacific Financial Markets, 8(4), 321-339.
doi: 10.1023/A:1020691528681 URL |
[45] | Lee A., Ji LJ., & Bu N. (2022). The impact of self-construals and thinking styles on business and management practices:A comparison of east Asia and north America. In: T. Wu, & N. Bu (Eds), International business in the New Asia-Pacific. Advances in theory and practice of emerging markets(pp. 127-157). Springer. |
[46] |
Lowe A. C. T., & Corkindale D. R. (1998). Differences in “cultural values” and their effects on responses to marketing stimuli: A cross-cultural study between Australians and Chinese from the People’s Republic of China. European Journal of Marketing, 32(9/10), 843-867.
doi: 10.1108/03090569810232291 URL |
[47] |
Lu M., Yang X., Fung H. H., & Hamamura T. (2020). Is positive emotion an amplifier or a buffer? It depends: Dialectical thinking moderates the impact of positive emotion on intergroup conflicts. Emotion, 20(4), 700-712.
doi: 10.1037/emo0000572 pmid: 30869936 |
[48] |
Maddux W. W., & Yuki M. (2006). The “ripple effect”: Cultural differences in perceptions of the consequences of events. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32(5), 669-683.
doi: 10.1177/0146167205283840 URL |
[49] |
Markus H. R., & Kitayama S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98(2), 224-253.
doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.98.2.224 URL |
[50] |
Masuda T., & Nisbett R. E. (2001). Attending holistically versus analytically: Comparing the context sensitivity of Japanese and Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(5), 922-934.
pmid: 11708567 |
[51] |
Morris M. W., & Peng K. (1994). Culture and cause: American and Chinese attributions for social and physical events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67(6), 949-971.
doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.67.6.949 URL |
[52] | Naito T., & Gielen U. P. (2005). The changing Japanese family:A psychological portrait. In J. L. Roopnarine, & U. P. Gielen(Eds.), Families in global perspective (pp. 63-84). Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon. |
[53] | Nisbett R. E. (2003). The geography of tthought: How Asians and Westerners think differently... and why. New York: Free Press. |
[54] |
Nisbett R. E., Peng K., Choi I., & Norenzayan A. (2001). Culture and systems of thought: Holistic versus analytic cognition. Psychological Review, 108(2), 291-310.
doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.108.2.291 pmid: 11381831 |
[55] |
Peetz J., & Wilson A. E. (2013). The post-birthday world: Consequences of temporal landmarks for temporal self appraisal and motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104(2), 249-266.
doi: 10.1037/a0030477 URL |
[56] |
Peng K., & Nisbett R. E. (1999). Culture, dialectics, and reasoning about contradiction. American Psychologist, 54(9), 741-754.
doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.54.9.741 URL |
[57] | Peng X., & Wang X. Q. (2011). The research about relationship between Confucian coping and life meaning searching and assertion. Chinese Journal of Behavioral Medicine and Brain Science, 20(5), 456-457. |
[58] |
Sedikides C., Wildschut T., Routledge C., & Arndt J. (2015). Nostalgia counteracts self-discontinuity and restores self- continuity. European Journal of Social Psychology, 45(1), 52-61.
doi: 10.1002/ejsp.2073 URL |
[59] |
Shipp A. J., & Aeon B. (2019). Temporal focus: Thinking about the past, present, and future. Current Opinion in Psychology, 26(4), 37-43.
doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2018.04.005 URL |
[60] |
Spencer-Rodgers J., Peng K., Wang L., & Hou Y. (2004). Dialectical self-esteem and East-West differences in psychological well-Being. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30(11), 1416-1432.
doi: 10.1177/0146167204264243 pmid: 15448306 |
[61] |
Stanley J. T., Zhang X., Fung H. H., & Isaacowitz D. M. (2013). Cultural differences in gaze and emotion recognition: Americans contrast more than Chinese. Emotion, 13(1), 36-46.
doi: 10.1037/a0029209 pmid: 22889414 |
[62] | Stevenson H. W., & Stigler J. W. (1992). The Learning gap: Why our schools are failing and what we can learn from Japanese and Chinese education. New York: Summit Books. |
[63] | Stevenson H. W., & Zusho A. (2002). Adolescence in China and Japan:Adapting to a changing environment. In B. B. Brown, R. Larson, & T. S. Saraswathi(Eds.), The world’s youth: Adolescence in eight regions of the globe (pp. 141-170). New York: Cambridge University Press. |
[64] |
Talhelm T., Zhang X., Oishi S., Shimin C., Duan D., Lan X., & Kitayama S. (2014). Large-scale psychological differences within China explained by rice versus wheat agriculture. Science, 344(6184), 603-608.
doi: 10.1126/science.1246850 pmid: 24812395 |
[65] |
Wang P. P. & He J. M. (2020). Effects of episodic foresight on intertemporal decision-making. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 52(1), 38-54.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2020.00038 URL |
[ 王盼盼, 何嘉梅. (2020). 情景预见对跨期决策的影响机制. 心理学报, 52(1), 38-54.] | |
[66] |
Wang Q. (2004). The emergence of cultural self-constructs: Autobiographical memory and self-description in European American and Chinese children. Developmental Psychology, 40(1), 3-15.
doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.40.1.3 pmid: 14700460 |
[67] | Wang Y. H., & Zhao X. J. (2001). Empirical study on momentum strategy and reversal strategy in China’s stock market. Economic Research Journal, (6), 56-61+89. |
[ 王永宏, 赵学军. (2001). 中国股市“惯性策略”和“反转策略”的实证分析. 经济研究, (6), 56-61+89.] | |
[68] | Wu Y., Yang Y. Y., Wei X. J. & Chen E. (2017). What determines “giving birth to a son”: The social transformation of how institution and culture affect women's fertility choices. Journal of Chinese Sociology, (4), 1-22. |
[69] | Xu X. Z. & Zheng C. Y. (2006). An empirical study with analysis on the momentum effect of China’s stock market. Economic Research Journal, (1), 85-99. |
[ 徐信忠, 郑纯毅. (2006). 中国股票市场动量效应成因分析. 经济科学, (1), 85-99.] | |
[70] | Yang C. F. (2001). Chinese people's view of self:the "big self" and "small self" of "self". In C. F. Yang(Ed.), How to understand the Chinese: Culture and personal essays (pp. 365-404). Taiwan, China: Yuan-Liou Publishing Co., Ltd. |
[ 杨中芳. (2001). 中国人的人己观:“自己”的“大我”与“小我”. 见:杨中芳编. 如何理解中国人: 文化与个人论文集(pp.365-404). 台湾: 远流出版事业股份有限公司.] | |
[71] |
Yang K. S., & Bond M. H. (1990). Exploring implicit personality theories with indigenous or imported constructs: The Chinese case. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58(6), 1087-1095.
pmid: 2391641 |
[72] | Yang X. (2016). The change of city elder filial piety expectations: A case study in Hangzhou. Journal of Graduate School of Chinese Academy of Social Science, (1), 140-144. |
[ 杨曦. (2016). 城市老人对孝行期待变化研究——以杭州市上城区老人个案为基础的考察. 中国社会科学院研究生院学报, (1), 140-144.] | |
[73] | Yang Y. Y. (2005). "One of us" (zijiren): Case study on the classification of Chinese relations. Chinese Social Psychological Review, 1, 181-205. |
[ 杨宜音. (2005). “自己人”: 一项有关中国人关系分类的个案研究. 中国社会心理学评论, 1, 181-205.] | |
[74] | Yang Y. Y. (2014). The moral meaning and life purpose in daily life: The conceptualization of the C. F. Yang's Zhongyong action-deliberation system. Chinese Social Psychological Review, 8, 256-274. |
[ 杨宜音. (2014). 日常生活的道德意义和生命意义——兼谈中庸实践思维概念的构念化. 中国社会心理学评论, 8, 256-274.] | |
[75] |
Yang Z., Ji L. J., Yang Y., Wang Y., Zhu L., & Cai H. (2021). Meaning making helps cope with COVID-19: A longitudinal study. Personality and Individual Difference, 174(3), 110670.
doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2021.110670 URL |
[76] |
Yap S., Lee A., Ji L.J., Li Y., & Dong Y. (2021). Cultural differences in people’s psychological response to COVID-19. Frontiers in Psychology, 12(6), 636062.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.636062 URL |
[77] |
Yau O. H. M. (1988). Chinese cultural values: Their dimensions and marketing implications. European Journal of Marketing, 22(5), 44-57.
doi: 10.1108/EUM0000000005285 URL |
[78] |
Zhang J., & Bond M. H. (1998). Personality and filial piety among college students in two Chinese societies: The added value of indigenous constructs. Journal of Cross- Cultural Psychology, 29(3), 402-417.
doi: 10.1177/0022022198293002 URL |
[79] | Zhang N., Ji. L. J., & Guo T. (2018). Culture and lay theories of change. In J. Spencer-Rogers & K. Peng(Eds.), The psychological and cultural foundations of East Asian thinking(pp. 81-104). Oxford University Press. |
[80] |
Zhang S., van Doorn J., & Leeflang P. (2014). Does the importance of value, brand and relationship equity for customer loyalty differ between eastern and western cultures? International Business Review, 23(1), 284-292.
doi: 10.1016/j.ibusrev.2013.05.002 URL |
[81] | Zheng Y., & Huang X. T. (2000). Self-integration on time perspective: Ⅱ. experimental research for the psychological mechanism. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 32(2), 36-39. |
[ 郑涌, 黄希庭. (2000). 时间透视的自我整合:Ⅱ. 心理功能机制的实验研究. 心理学报, 32(2), 36-39.] | |
[82] |
Zhu Y., Zhang L., Fan J., & Han S. (2007). Neural basis of cultural influence on self-representation. Neuroimage, 34(3), 1310-1316.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.08.047 pmid: 17134915 |
[83] |
Zimbardo P. G., & Boyd J. N. (1999). Putting time in perspective: A valid, reliable individual-differences metric. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77(6), 1271-1288.
doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.77.6.1271 URL |
[84] |
Zimbardo P. G., Keough K. A., & Boyd J. N. (1997). Present time perspective as a predictor of risky driving. Personality and Individual Differences, 23(6), 1007-1023.
doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(97)00113-X URL |
[1] | 王琳, 陈增祥, 何云. 传承动机对金融冒险行为的影响:未来自我连续性的中介[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(8): 1004-1016. |
[2] | 王鹏, 王晓田, 高娟, 黎夏岚, 徐静. 适应性时间管理:死亡意识对时间知觉和跨期决策的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2019, 51(12): 1341-1350. |
[3] | 彭明, 金文颖, 蔡梦菲, 周宗奎. 网络词语与非网络词语的知觉差异:时间知觉、 空间距离知觉与知觉范围[J]. 心理学报, 2017, 49(7): 866-874. |
[4] | 白学军;高晓雷;高蕾;王永胜. 藏语阅读知觉广度的眼动研究[J]. 心理学报, 2017, 49(5): 569-576. |
[5] | 刘扬;孙彦. 时间分解效应及其对跨期决策的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2016, 48(4): 362-370. |
[6] | 闫国利,王丽红,巫金根,白学军. 不同年级学生阅读知觉广度及预视效益的眼动研究[J]. 心理学报, 2011, 43(03): 249-263. |
[7] | 陈有国,张志杰,黄希庭,郭秀艳,袁宏,张甜. 时间知觉的注意调节:一项ERP研究[J]. 心理学报, 2007, 39(06): 1002-1011. |
[8] | 刘瑞光,黄希庭. 运动视觉信息中时间知觉线索的实验研究[J]. 心理学报, 1999, 31(1): 15-20. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||