心理学报 ›› 2022, Vol. 54 ›› Issue (8): 951-963.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2022.00951
收稿日期:
2021-07-20
发布日期:
2022-06-23
出版日期:
2022-08-25
通讯作者:
张宝山
E-mail:zhangbs@snnu.edu.cn
基金资助:
ZHANG Baoshan(), JIN Dou, MA Mengjia, XU Ran
Received:
2021-07-20
Online:
2022-06-23
Published:
2022-08-25
Contact:
ZHANG Baoshan
E-mail:zhangbs@snnu.edu.cn
摘要:
随着年龄的增长, 对老年人的消极刻板印象和医疗决策逐渐成为了与老年群体越来越相关的两个概念。在此背景下, 很有必要系统地探讨消极刻板印象对老年人医疗决策的效应及其机制。本研究以158名老年人为被试, 通过两个实验考察了消极刻板印象、归因偏差和老年人医疗决策质量间的关系。实验1结果发现, 消极刻板印象负向影响老年人的医疗决策质量, 同时, 归因偏差在刻板印象对老年人医疗决策质量的效应中起到中介作用。实验2结果表明, 减少归因偏差的干预控制可以有效降低刻板印象对医疗决策质量的消极效应。本研究对理解刻板印象效应机制、缓解刻板印象消极效应、以及改善老年人医疗决策质量具有一定的理论意义和实践价值。
中图分类号:
张宝山, 金豆, 马梦佳, 徐冉. (2022). 消极刻板印象对老年人医疗决策的影响及归因偏差的作用. 心理学报, 54(8), 951-963.
ZHANG Baoshan, JIN Dou, MA Mengjia, XU Ran. (2022). The effect of negative aging stereotypes on the quality of medical decision-making and the mediating role of attribution bias. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 54(8), 951-963.
变量 | 威胁组 (n = 40) | 控制组 (n = 38) | t/χ2 | Cohen’s d |
---|---|---|---|---|
年龄M (SD) | 68.45 (7.08) | 67.53 (7.69) | -0.55 | -0.12 |
自评健康M (SD) | 3.50 (0.68) | 3.42 (0.55) | -0.56 | -0.13 |
生活满意M (SD) | 3.88 (0.69) | 3.92 (0.49) | 0.34 | 0.07 |
性别N (%) | ||||
男 | 14 (35.0%) | 12 (31.6%) | 0.10 | |
女 | 26 (65.0%) | 26 (68.4%) | ||
文化程度N (%) | 4.77 | |||
小学 | 5 (12.5%) | 2 (5.3%) | ||
初中 | 15 (37.5%) | 9 (23.7 %) | ||
高中 | 6 (15.0%) | 12 (31.6 %) | ||
本科及以上 | 14 (35.0%) | 15 (39.5 %) | ||
归因偏差M (SD) | 3.33 (1.38) | 1.57 (0.93) | -6.63*** | -1.50 |
决策质量M (SD) | 0.15 (1.44) | 2.61 (1.31) | 7.87*** | 1.79 |
表1 实验1被试信息及相关变量差异检验结果
变量 | 威胁组 (n = 40) | 控制组 (n = 38) | t/χ2 | Cohen’s d |
---|---|---|---|---|
年龄M (SD) | 68.45 (7.08) | 67.53 (7.69) | -0.55 | -0.12 |
自评健康M (SD) | 3.50 (0.68) | 3.42 (0.55) | -0.56 | -0.13 |
生活满意M (SD) | 3.88 (0.69) | 3.92 (0.49) | 0.34 | 0.07 |
性别N (%) | ||||
男 | 14 (35.0%) | 12 (31.6%) | 0.10 | |
女 | 26 (65.0%) | 26 (68.4%) | ||
文化程度N (%) | 4.77 | |||
小学 | 5 (12.5%) | 2 (5.3%) | ||
初中 | 15 (37.5%) | 9 (23.7 %) | ||
高中 | 6 (15.0%) | 12 (31.6 %) | ||
本科及以上 | 14 (35.0%) | 15 (39.5 %) | ||
归因偏差M (SD) | 3.33 (1.38) | 1.57 (0.93) | -6.63*** | -1.50 |
决策质量M (SD) | 0.15 (1.44) | 2.61 (1.31) | 7.87*** | 1.79 |
变量 | 干预组 (n = 40) | 威胁组 (n = 40) | t/χ2 | Cohen’s d |
---|---|---|---|---|
年龄M (SD) | 65.30 (5.42) | 66.13 (6.58) | -0.61 | -0.14 |
自评健康M (SD) | 3.50 (0.68) | 3.50 (0.68) | 0.00 | 0.00 |
生活满意M (SD) | 3.80 (0.79) | 3.85 (0.66) | -0.31 | 0.07 |
性别N (%) | 1.92 | |||
男 | 18 (45.0%) | 12 (30.0%) | ||
女 | 22 (55.0%) | 28 (70.0%) | ||
文化程度N (%) | 4.31 | |||
小学 | 1 (2.5%) | 3 (7.5%) | ||
初中 | 7 (17.5%) | 9 (22.5%) | ||
高中 | 8 (20.0%) | 12 (30.0%) | ||
本科及以上 | 24 (60.0%) | 40 (40.0%) | ||
归因偏差M (SD) | 0.16 (0.26) | 3.96 (4.03) | -5.96*** | -1.33 |
决策质量M (SD) | 3.00 (1.75) | 0.65 (1.85) | 5.83*** | 1.31 |
表2 实验2被试信息及差异检验结果
变量 | 干预组 (n = 40) | 威胁组 (n = 40) | t/χ2 | Cohen’s d |
---|---|---|---|---|
年龄M (SD) | 65.30 (5.42) | 66.13 (6.58) | -0.61 | -0.14 |
自评健康M (SD) | 3.50 (0.68) | 3.50 (0.68) | 0.00 | 0.00 |
生活满意M (SD) | 3.80 (0.79) | 3.85 (0.66) | -0.31 | 0.07 |
性别N (%) | 1.92 | |||
男 | 18 (45.0%) | 12 (30.0%) | ||
女 | 22 (55.0%) | 28 (70.0%) | ||
文化程度N (%) | 4.31 | |||
小学 | 1 (2.5%) | 3 (7.5%) | ||
初中 | 7 (17.5%) | 9 (22.5%) | ||
高中 | 8 (20.0%) | 12 (30.0%) | ||
本科及以上 | 24 (60.0%) | 40 (40.0%) | ||
归因偏差M (SD) | 0.16 (0.26) | 3.96 (4.03) | -5.96*** | -1.33 |
决策质量M (SD) | 3.00 (1.75) | 0.65 (1.85) | 5.83*** | 1.31 |
[1] | Bambara J. K., Griffith H. R., Martin R. C., Faught E., Wadley V. G., & Marson D. C. (2007). Medical decision-making abilities in older adults with chronic partial epilepsy. Epilepsy & Behavior, 10(1), 63-68. |
[2] |
Bouazzaoui B., Follenfant A., Ric F., Fay S., Croizet J. C., Atzeni T., & Taconnat L. (2016). Ageing-related stereotypes in memory: When the beliefs come true. Memory, 24(5), 659-668.
doi: 10.1080/09658211.2015.1040802 pmid: 26057336 |
[3] |
Brelet L., Moták L., Ginet M., Huet N., Izaute M., & Gabaude C. (2016). Enhancing older drivers’ safety: On effects induced by stereotype threat to older adults’ driving performance, working memory and self-regulation. Geriatrics, 1(3), 20.
doi: 10.3390/geriatrics1030020 URL |
[4] |
Bryant C., Bei B., Gilson K., Komiti A., Jackson H., & Judd F. (2012). The relationship between attitudes to aging and physical and mental health in older adults. International Psychogeriatrics, 24(10), 1674-1683.
doi: 10.1017/S1041610212000774 URL |
[5] |
Carr P. B., & Steele C. M. (2010). Stereotype threat affects financial decision making. Psychological Science, 21(10), 1411-1416.
doi: 10.1177/0956797610384146 URL |
[6] | Casey P. R., Psych F. R. C., & Strain J. J. (Eds.). (2015). Trauma-and stressor-related disorders: A handbook for clinicians. American Psychiatric Pub. |
[7] |
Coudin G., & Alexopoulos T. (2010). ‘Help me! I’m old!’ How negative aging stereotypes create dependency among older adults. Aging & Mental Health, 14(5), 516-523.
doi: 10.1080/13607861003713182 URL |
[8] |
Cundiff J. L., & Vescio T. K. (2016). Gender stereotypes influence how people explain gender disparities in the workplace. Sex Roles, 75(3), 126-138.
doi: 10.1007/s11199-016-0593-2 URL |
[9] |
Dijksterhuis A. (2004). Think different: The merits of unconscious thought in preference development and decision making. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87(5), 586-598.
pmid: 15535773 |
[10] | Dionigi R. A. (2015). Stereotypes of aging: Their effects on the health of older adults. Journal of Geriatrics, 2015, 1-9. |
[11] |
Ekstrom L. W. (2012). Liars, medicine, and compassion. Journal of Medicine and Philosophy, 37(2), 159-180.
doi: 10.1093/jmp/jhs007 pmid: 22490238 |
[12] |
Fiske S. T. (1992). Thinking is for doing: Portraits of social cognition from daguerreotype to laserphoto. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63(6), 877-889.
pmid: 1460557 |
[13] |
Franklin A., Liu Y., Li Z., Nguyen V., Johnson T. R., Robinson D., … Zhang J. (2011). Opportunistic decision making and complexity in emergency care. Journal of Biomedical Informatics, 44(3), 469-476.
doi: 10.1016/j.jbi.2011.04.001 pmid: 21511054 |
[14] |
Haggag K., Pope D. G., Bryant-Lees K. B., & Bos M. W. (2019). Attribution bias in consumer choice. The Review of Economic Studies, 86(5), 2136-2183.
doi: 10.1093/restud/rdy054 URL |
[15] | Heider F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. New York: John Wiley. |
[16] | Huang X. C. (2018). The influence and intervention of hostility attribution bias on aggression behavior (Unpublished master’s thesis). Beijing Sport University. |
[ 黄小草. (2018). 敌意归因偏差对攻击行为的影响及干预(硕士学位论文). 北京体育大学.] | |
[17] |
Huang Y. H., Wood S., Berger D. E., & Hanoch Y. (2015). Age differences in experiential and deliberative processes in unambiguous and ambiguous decision making. Psychology and Aging, 30(3), 675-687.
doi: 10.1037/pag0000038 pmid: 26280384 |
[18] |
Inzlicht M., & Kang S. K. (2010). Stereotype threat spillover: How coping with threats to social identity affects aggression, eating, decision making, and attention. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99(3), 467-481.
doi: 10.1037/a0018951 pmid: 20649368 |
[19] |
Jordano M. L., & Touron D. R. (2017). Stereotype threat as a trigger of mind-wandering in older adults. Psychology and Aging, 32(3), 307-313.
doi: 10.1037/pag0000167 pmid: 28333503 |
[20] |
Kiefer A., & Shih M. (2006). Gender differences in persistence and attributions in stereotype relevant contexts. Sex Roles: A Journal of Research, 54(11-12), 859-868.
doi: 10.1007/s11199-006-9051-x URL |
[21] | Koo J. H., & Yang D. (2018). Managerial overconfidence, self-attribution bias, and downwardly sticky investment: Evidence from Korea. Emerging Markets Finance & Trade, 54(1), 144-161. |
[22] |
Kühnen U. (2010). Manipulation checks as manipulation: Another look at the ease-of-retrieval heuristic. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36(1), 47-58.
doi: 10.1177/0146167209346746 URL |
[23] |
Kulik J. A. (1983). Confirmatory attribution and the perpetuation of social beliefs. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44(6), 1171-1181.
doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.44.6.1171 URL |
[24] |
Lamont R. A., Swift H. J., & Abrams D. (2015). A review and meta-analysis of age-based stereotype threat: Negative stereotypes, not facts, do the damage. Psychology and Aging, 30(1), 180-193.
doi: 10.1037/a0038586 pmid: 25621742 |
[25] |
Levy B. (1996). Improving memory in old age through implicit self-stereotyping. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71(6), 1092-1107.
pmid: 8979380 |
[26] |
Levy B., Ashman O., & Dror I. (2000). To be or not to be: The effects of aging stereotypes on the will to live. OMEGA Journal of Death and Dying, 40(3), 409-420.
doi: 10.2190/Y2GE-BVYQ-NF0E-83VR URL |
[27] | Li A. M., Ling W. Q., & Li L. Y. (2009). The implicit facial attractiveness stereotype research in personnel interview circumstances. Journal of Psychological Science, 32(4), 970-973. |
[28] | [ 李爱梅, 凌文辁, 李连雨. (2009). 招聘面试中的内隐相貌刻板印象研究. 心理科学, 32(4), 970-973.] |
[29] | Li S, X., Li X, H., Li G, M., Huang X, Q., & Song Y, M. (2012). Affection of attribution interference on medicine treatment physician action of elderly patients suffered from the original high blood pressure. Journal of Qiqihar University of Medicine, 33(24), 3437-3439. |
[ 李水轩, 李雪华, 黎观妹, 黄锡球, 宋玉梅. (2012). 归因干预对老年原发性高血压患者药物治疗遵医行为的影响. 齐齐哈尔医学院学报, 33(24), 3437-3439.] | |
[30] | Liao Q. M., & Zheng Y. (2007). An experimental research on the influence of different training methods on pupils’ sharing behavior. Journal of Psychological Science, 30(6), 1351-1355. |
[ 廖全明, 郑涌. (2007). 不同训练方法对小学生分享行为影响的实验研究. 心理科学, 30(6), 1351-1355.] | |
[31] |
Löckenhoff C. E. (2017). Aging and decision-making: A conceptual framework for future research a mini review. Gerontology, 64(2), 140-148.
doi: 10.1159/000485247 URL |
[32] |
Marques S., Lima M. L., Abrams D., & Swift H. (2014). Will to live in older people's medical decisions: Immediate and delayed effects of aging stereotypes. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 44(6), 399-408.
doi: 10.1111/jasp.12231 URL |
[33] |
Masicampo E. J., & Baumeister R. F. (2008). Toward a physiology of dual-process reasoning and judgment: Lemonade, willpower, and expensive rule-based analysis. Psychological Science, 19(3), 255-260.
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02077.x pmid: 18315798 |
[34] |
Mather M., Mazar N., Gorlick M. A., Lighthall N. R., Burgeno J., Schoeke A., & Ariely D. (2012). Risk preferences and aging: The “certainty effect” in older adults' decision making. Psychology and Aging, 27(4), 801-816.
doi: 10.1037/a0030174 pmid: 23066800 |
[35] |
Oakes P. J., Turner J. C., & Haslam S. A. (1991). Perceiving people as group members: The role of fit in the salience of social categorizations. British Journal of Social Psychology, 30(2), 125-144.
doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8309.1991.tb00930.x URL |
[36] |
Preacher K. J., & Hayes A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research Methods, 40(3), 879-891.
doi: 10.3758/BRM.40.3.879 URL |
[37] |
Sanjuán P., & Magallares A. (2014). Coping strategies as mediating variables between self-serving attributional bias and subjective well-being. Journal of Happiness Studies, 15(2), 443-453.
doi: 10.1007/s10902-013-9430-2 URL |
[38] |
Schmader T., & Johns M. (2003). Converging evidence that stereotype threat reduces working memory capacity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(3), 440-452.
doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.3.440 pmid: 14498781 |
[39] |
Steele C. M., & Aronson J. (1995). Stereotype threat and the intellectual test performance of African Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69(5), 797-811.
doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.69.5.797 pmid: 7473032 |
[40] |
Stein R., Blanchard-Fields F., & Hertzog C. (2002). The effects of age-stereotype priming on the memory performance of older adults. Experimental Aging Research, 28(2), 169-181.
doi: 10.1080/03610730252800184 URL |
[41] | Stewart T. L., Chipperfield J. G., Perry R. P., & Weiner B. (2012). Attributing illness to 'old age:' Consequences of a self-directed stereotype for health and mortality. Psychology & Health, 27(8), 881-897. |
[42] |
Stewart T. L., Latu I. M., Kawakami K., & Myers A. C. (2010). Consider the situation: Reducing automatic stereotyping through situational attribution training. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46(1), 221-225.
doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2009.09.004 URL |
[43] |
Trent E. S., Viana A. G., Raines E. M., Woodward E. C., Candelari A. E., Storch E. A., & Zvolensky M. J. (2019). Interpretation biases and childhood anxiety: The moderating role of parasympathetic nervous system reactivity. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 48(3), 419-433.
doi: 10.1007/s10802-019-00605-7 URL |
[44] |
Tsoulos I. G., Mitsi G., Stavrakoudis A., & Papapetropoulos S. (2019). Application of machine learning in a parkinson's disease digital biomarker dataset using neural network construction (NNC) methodology discriminates patient motor status. Frontiers in ICT, 6, 1-7.
doi: 10.3389/fict.2019.00001 URL |
[45] |
Vassilopoulos S. P., Banerjee R., & Prantzalou C. (2009). Experimental modification of interpretation bias in socially anxious children: Changes in interpretation, anticipated interpersonal anxiety, and social anxiety symptoms. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47(12), 1085-1089.
doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2009.07.018 pmid: 19679299 |
[46] | Wang D. W., & Liu Y. F. (2008). An experimental research on the effects of attributive style and time pressure on purchasing decision. Journal of Psychological Science, 31(4), 905-908. |
[ 王大伟, 刘永芳. (2008). 归因风格、时间压力对购买决策影响的实验研究. 心理科学, 31(4), 905-908.] | |
[47] |
Weiss B. D., Berman E. A., Howe C. L., & Fleming R. B. (2012). Medical decision-making for older adults without family. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 60(11), 2144-2150.
doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2012.04212.x URL |
[48] | Xu L. L., Wu P. P., & He W. (2018). Meta-stereotype threat effects on intergroup relations among impoverished undergraduates: A moderated mediation model of intergroup anxiety and self-esteem. Psychological Development and Education, 34(4), 426-433. |
[ 徐璐璐, 吴佩佩, 贺雯. (2018). 贫困大学生元刻板印象威胁对群际关系的影响: 群际焦虑的中介和自尊的调节作用. 心理发展与教育, 34(4), 426-433.] | |
[49] | Yang T., & Ren X. P. (2016). The impact of gender orientation of names on female mate preferences. Journal of Psychological Science, 39(5), 1190-1196. |
[ 杨婷, 任孝鹏. (2016). 不同性别定向的名字对女性择偶偏好的影响. 心理科学, 39(5), 1190-1196.] |
[1] | 高玉林, 唐晓雨, 刘思宇, 王爱君, 张明. 内源性空间线索有效性对老年人视听觉整合的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(5): 671-684. |
[2] | 孟海江, 陈蕾, 王刚, 张剑. 不同形式运动锻炼老年人运动皮层突触可塑性的差异:来自TMS的研究证据[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(10): 1653-1661. |
[3] | 温芳芳, 柯文琳, 何赛飞, 佐斌, 李兰心, 马书瀚, 王晶. 群体身份变换性对老年人印象更新的影响:共同内群体认同的中介作用[J]. 心理学报, 2022, 54(9): 1059-1075. |
[4] | 王堂生, 杨春亮, 钟年. 记忆的前向测试效应对老年人学习新事物的作用[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(11): 1266-1277. |
[5] | 吴国婷, 张敏强, 倪雨菡, 杨亚威, 漆成明, 吴健星. 老年人孤独感及其影响因素的潜在转变分析 *[J]. 心理学报, 2018, 50(9): 1061-1070. |
[6] | 彭华茂, 毛晓飞. 抑制对老年人舌尖现象的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2018, 50(10): 1142-1150. |
[7] | 姚若松, 郭梦诗, 叶浩生. 社会支持对老年人社会幸福感的影响机制:希望与孤独感的中介作用[J]. 心理学报, 2018, 50(10): 1151-1158. |
[8] | 张奇林;周艺梦. 中国城镇低龄退休老年人工作与幸福感的关系[J]. 心理学报, 2017, 49(4): 472-481. |
[9] | 周爱保;刘沛汝;张彦驰;尹玉龙. 老年人的朋友参照效应[J]. 心理学报, 2015, 47(9): 1143-1151. |
[10] | 王大华;杨小洋;王岩;Richard B. Miller. 老年人夫妻依恋的测量及与一般依恋的关系[J]. 心理学报, 2015, 47(9): 1133-1142. |
[11] | 陈栩茜;张积家;朱云霞. 言语产生老化中的抑制损伤:来自不同任务的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2015, 47(3): 329-343. |
[12] | 肖红蕊;龚先旻;王大华. 老年人错误记忆的情绪效价和时间效应[J]. 心理学报, 2014, 46(7): 922-930. |
[13] | 任娜,佐斌,侯飞翔,汪国驹. 情境效应或自动化加工:大学生对老年人的内隐态度[J]. 心理学报, 2012, 44(6): 777-788. |
[14] | 王大华,佟雁,周丽清,申继亮. 亲子支持对老年人主观幸福感的影响机制[J]. 心理学报, 2004, 36(01): 78-82. |
[15] | 张卫东. 老年人特殊生活事件的应对意向与情境性应对[J]. 心理学报, 1998, 30(4): 401-408. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||