心理学报 ›› 2024, Vol. 56 ›› Issue (5): 542-554.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2024.00542
收稿日期:
2023-01-17
发布日期:
2024-03-06
出版日期:
2024-05-25
通讯作者:
王汉林, E-mail: wanghanlin@hebtu.edu.cn
基金资助:
REN Weicong1, YANG Ting2, WANG Hanlin1()
Received:
2023-01-17
Online:
2024-03-06
Published:
2024-05-25
摘要:
通过行为与脑电实验技术, 比较外语词汇学习过程中, 语言符号与知觉符号表征对词汇记忆编码与再认的影响过程, 从而考察语义表征对词汇习得的影响机制。行为结果表明, 语言符号与知觉符号表征条件下被试对词汇学习效果的主观判断无显著差异, 但后者比前者有更高的词汇再认正确率。脑电结果表明, 对于词汇编码阶段, 相较语言符号表征, 知觉符号表征在编码晚期诱发更正的LPC成分; 对于词汇再认阶段, 知觉符号表征条件诱发更大N400成分, 且脑电时频分析表明该条件下出现更为明显的μ波抑制及θ波功率增强现象。研究结果综合表明, 与语言符号表征便捷但非模态化的语义加工相比, 知觉符号表征加深了词汇晚期编码的深度, 并通过知觉模拟, 利用多模态信息提高了词汇的形象化再认, 从而推动了再认阶段的语义检索, 最终内隐性地提高了词汇学习效果。
中图分类号:
任维聪, 杨婷, 王汉林. (2024). 语言符号与知觉符号表征对外语词汇习得的影响比较:来自行为与脑电的实验证据. 心理学报, 56(5), 542-554.
REN Weicong, YANG Ting, WANG Hanlin. (2024). Different effects of linguistic and perceptual symbolic representations on foreign language vocabulary learning: Evidence from behavioral and EEG data. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 56(5), 542-554.
[1] |
Barsalou, L. W. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22(4), 577-660.
pmid: 11301525 |
[2] |
Baumeister, J. C., Foroni, F., Conrad, M., Rumiati, R. I., & Winkielman, P. (2017). Embodiment and emotional memory in first vs. second language. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 394.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00394 pmid: 28386240 |
[3] |
Bi, Y. C. (2021). Dual coding of knowledge in the human brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 25(10), 883-895.
doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2021.07.006 pmid: 34509366 |
[4] |
Borghesani, V., & Piazza, M. (2017). The neuro-cognitive representations of symbols: The case of concrete words. Neuropsychologia, 105, 4-17.
doi: S0028-3932(17)30239-7 pmid: 28648571 |
[5] |
Brunsdon, V. E. A., Bradford, E. E. F., & Ferguson, H. J. (2019). Sensorimotor mu rhythm during action observation changes across the lifespan independently from social cognitive processes. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 38, 100659.
doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2019.100659 URL |
[6] |
Capone, N. C., & McGregor, K. K. (2005). The effect of semantic representation on toddlers' word retrieval. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 48(6), 1468-1480.
doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2005/102) URL |
[7] |
Casasanto, D., & de Bruin, A. (2019). Metaphors we learn by: Directed motor action improves word learning. Cognition, 182, 177-183.
doi: S0010-0277(18)30251-8 pmid: 30296659 |
[8] | Chen, Y., Guo, C., & Jiang, Y. (2011). Neural basis for successful encoding and retrieval of prospective memory. Science China Life Sciences, 41(4), 332-339. |
[陈燕妮, 郭春彦, 姜扬. (2011). 成功的前瞻记忆编码与提取的神经机制研究. 中国科学:生命科学, 41(04), 332-339.] | |
[9] | Cohen, J. (2013). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Routledge. |
[10] |
Comesana, M., Perea, M., Piñeiro, A., & Fraga, I. (2009). Vocabulary teaching strategies and conceptual representations of words in L2 in children: Evidence with novice learners. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 104(1), 22-33.
doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2008.10.004 pmid: 19084237 |
[11] |
Craik, F. I., & Tulving, E. (1975). Depth of processing and the retention of words in episodic memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 104(3), 268-294.
doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.104.3.268 URL |
[12] |
Debnath, R., Salo, V. C., Buzzell, G. A., Yoo, K. H., & Fox, N. A. (2019). Mu rhythm desynchronization is specific to action execution and observation: Evidence from time- frequency and connectivity analysis. Neuroimage, 184, 496-507.
doi: S1053-8119(18)31858-5 pmid: 30248457 |
[13] |
Dove, G. (2009). Beyond perceptual symbols: A call for representational pluralism. Cognition, 110(3), 412-431.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2008.11.016 pmid: 19135654 |
[14] |
Eschmann, K. C. J., Bader, R., & Mecklinger, A. (2020). Improving episodic memory: Frontal-midline theta neurofeedback training increases source memory performance. Neuroimage, 222, 117219.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117219 URL |
[15] |
Foroni, F. (2015). Do we embody second language? Evidence for ‘partial’ simulation during processing of a second language. Brain and Cognition, 99, 8-16.
doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2015.06.006 URL |
[16] |
Fox, N. A., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Yoo, K. H., Bowman, L. C., Cannon, E. N., Vanderwert, R. E.,... van Ijzendoorn, M. H. (2016). Assessing human mirror activity with EEG mu rhythm: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 142(3), 291-313.
doi: 10.1037/bul0000031 pmid: 26689088 |
[17] |
Garbin, G., Costa, A., Sanjuan, A., Forn, C., Rodriguez- Pujadas, A., Ventura, N.,... Ávila, C. (2011). Neural bases of language switching in high and early proficient bilinguals. Brain and Language, 119(3), 129-135.
doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2011.03.011 pmid: 21550652 |
[18] | Guan, Q. (2019). Embodied linguistics——Language science in the era of artificial intelligence. Beijing: Science Press. |
[官群. (2019). 具身语言学——人工智能时代的语言科学. 北京: 科学出版社.] | |
[19] | Guo, T., & Peng, D. (2002). The accessing mechanism of the less proficient Chinese-English bilinguals’ conceptual representation. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 35(1), 23-28. |
[郭桃梅, 彭聃龄. (2002). 非熟练中-英双语者的第二语言的语义通达机制. 心理学报, 35(1), 23-28.] | |
[20] |
Hald, L. A., de Nooijer, J., van Gog, T., & Bekkering, H. (2016). Optimizing word learning via links to perceptual and motoric experience. Educational Psychology Review, 28(3), 495-522.
doi: 10.1007/s10648-015-9334-2 URL |
[21] |
Herweg, N. A., Solomon, E. A., & Kahana, M. J. (2020). Theta Oscillations in Human Memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 24(3), 208-227.
doi: S1364-6613(19)30294-3 pmid: 32029359 |
[22] |
Holcomb, P. J., Kounios, J., Anderson, J. E., & West, W. C. (1999). Dual-coding, context-availability, and concreteness effects in sentence comprehension: An electrophysiological investigation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 25(3), 721-742.
doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.25.3.721 URL |
[23] |
Hsieh, L. T., & Ranganath, C. (2014). Frontal midline theta oscillations during working memory maintenance and episodic encoding and retrieval. Neuroimage, 85, 721-729.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.08.003 URL |
[24] |
Hulstijn, J. (2002). Towards a unified account of the representation, processing and acquisition of second language knowledge. Second Language Research, 18(3), 193-223.
doi: 10.1191/0267658302sr207oa URL |
[25] |
Kutas, M., & Federmeier, K. D. (2000). Electrophysiology reveals semantic memory use in language comprehension. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(12), 463-470.
doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01560-6 pmid: 11115760 |
[26] |
Lai, V. T., Howerton, O., & Desai, R. H. (2019). Concrete processing of action metaphors: Evidence from ERP. Brain Research, 1714, 202-209.
doi: S0006-8993(19)30128-3 pmid: 30853628 |
[27] |
Laszlo, S., & Federmeier, K. D. (2011). The N400 as a snapshot of interactive processing: Evidence from regression analyses of orthographic neighbor and lexical associate effects. Psychophysiology, 48(2), 176-186.
doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.01058.x pmid: 20624252 |
[28] |
Linck, J. A., Kroll, J. F., & Sunderman, G. (2009). Losing access to the native language while immersed in a second language: Evidence for the role of inhibition in second-language learning. Psychological Science, 20(12), 1507-1515.
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02480.x pmid: 19906121 |
[29] |
Louwerse, M., & Hutchinson, S. (2012). Neurological evidence linguistic processes precede perceptual simulation in conceptual processing. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 385.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00385 pmid: 23133427 |
[30] | Louwerse, M., He, X., Deng, Y., Zhao, X., & Xue, Y. (2017). Symbol interdependency in language processing:Interactions between language statistics and perceptual simulation. Journal of South China Normal University (Social Science Edition), (2), 51-60. |
[Louwerse, M., 何先友, 邓玉梅, 赵雪汝, 薛颖. (2017). 语言加工中的符号相互依存: 语言统计和知觉模拟的交互作用. 华南师范大学学报(社会科学版), (2), 51-60.] | |
[31] |
Louwerse, M. M. (2011). Symbol interdependency in symbolic and embodied cognition. Topics in Cognitive Science, 3(2), 273-302.
doi: 10.1111/j.1756-8765.2010.01106.x pmid: 25164297 |
[32] | Ma, Z. (2015). The motivation and path of conceptual reconstruction in second language vocabulary acquisition. Journal of PLA University of Foreign Languages, 38(3), 74-80. |
[马拯. (2015). 二语词汇习得中概念重构的理据及其路径. 解放军外国语学院学报, 38(3), 74-80.] | |
[33] |
Macedonia, M., Repetto, C., Ischebeck, A., & Mueller, K. (2019). Depth of encoding through observed gestures in foreign language word learning. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 33.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00033 pmid: 30761033 |
[34] |
Maouene, J., Hidaka, S., & Smith, L. (2008). Body parts and early-learned verbs. Cognitive Science, 32(7), 1200-1216.
doi: 10.1080/03640210802019997 pmid: 21585449 |
[35] |
Markman, A. B., & Dietrich, E. (2000). Extending the classical view of representation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(12), 470-475.
pmid: 11115761 |
[36] |
Muller, O., Duñabeitia, J. A., & Carreiras, M. (2010). Orthographic and associative neighborhood density effects: What is shared, what is different? Psychophysiology, 47(3), 455-466.
doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2009.00960.x pmid: 20102534 |
[37] |
Mulligan, N. W., & Hornstein, S. L. (2003). Memory for actions: Self-performed tasks and the reenactment effect. Memory & Cognition, 31(3), 412-421.
doi: 10.3758/BF03194399 URL |
[38] |
Nittono, H., Suehiro, M., & Hori, T. (2002). Word imageability and N400 in an incidental memory paradigm. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 44(3), 219-229.
pmid: 12031296 |
[39] |
Öttl, B., Dudschig, C., & Kaup, B. (2017). Forming associations between language and sensorimotor traces during novel word learning. Language and Cognition, 9(1), 156-171.
doi: 10.1017/langcog.2016.5 URL |
[40] |
Paivio, A. (1991). Dual coding theory: Retrospect and current status. Canadian Journal of Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie, 45(3), 255-287.
doi: 10.1037/h0084295 URL |
[41] |
Pineda, J. A. (2005). The functional significance of mu rhythms: Translating "seeing" and "hearing" into "doing". Brain Research Reviews, 50(1), 57-68.
doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2005.04.005 URL |
[42] |
Pulvermüller, F. (2013). How neurons make meaning: Brain mechanisms for embodied and abstract-symbolic semantics. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(9), 458-470.
doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.06.004 pmid: 23932069 |
[43] |
Reilly, J., Peelle, J. E., Garcia, A., & Crutch, S. J. (2016). Linking somatic and symbolic representation in semantic memory: The dynamic multilevel reactivation framework. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23(4), 1002-1014.
doi: 10.3758/s13423-015-0824-5 URL |
[44] | Rosburg, T., Johansson, M., Weigl, M., & Mecklinger, A. (2015). How does testing affect retrieval-related processes? An event-related potential (ERP) study on the short-term effects of repeated retrieval. Cognitive Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 15(1), 195-210. |
[45] |
Rugg, M. D., Mark, R. E., Walla, P., Schloerscheidt, A. M., Birch, C. S., & Allan, K. (1998). Dissociation of the neural correlates of implicit and explicit memory. Nature, 392(6676), 595-598.
doi: 10.1038/33396 |
[46] |
Schwartz, A. I., Yeh, L.-H., & Shaw, M. P. (2008). Lexical representation of second language words: Implications for second language vocabulary acquisition and use. The Mental Lexicon, 3(3), 309-324.
doi: 10.1075/ml URL |
[47] |
Shen, M., Xie, J., Zhang, K., Li, Y., Zeng, C., & Wang, R. (2014). The spatial metaphor of bilingual affective concepts processing. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 46(11), 1671-1681.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2014.01671 |
[沈曼琼, 谢久书, 张昆, 李莹, 曾楚轩, 王瑞明. (2014). 二语情绪概念理解中的空间隐喻. 心理学报, 46(11), 1671-1681.] | |
[48] |
Solomon, E. A., Lega, B. C., Sperling, M. R., & Kahana, M. J. (2019). Hippocampal theta codes for distances in semantic and temporal spaces. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 116(48), 24343-24352.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1906729116 pmid: 31723043 |
[49] |
Wang, B., Li, Z., Wu, L., & Zhang, J. (2019). Effects of embodied simulation on understanding Chinese body action verbs. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 51(12), 1291-1305.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2019.01291 |
[王斌, 李智睿, 伍丽梅, 张积家. (2019). 具身模拟在汉语肢体动作动词理解中的作用. 心理学报, 51(12), 1291-1305.]
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2019.01291 |
|
[50] |
Wang, H., Jiang, Z., Feng, X., & Lu, Z. (2020). Spatial iconicity of moral concepts: Co-dependence of linguistic and embodied symbols. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 52(2), 128-138.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2020.00128 |
[王汉林, 蒋泽亮, 冯晓慧, 鲁忠义. (2020). 道德概念的空间形象性: 语言因素和具身因素的共同作用. 心理学报, 52(2), 128-138.]
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2020.00128 |
|
[51] | Wang, R. M., Yang, J., & Li, L. (2016). Second language learning. Shanghai: East China Normal University Press. |
[王瑞明, 杨静, 李利. (2016). 第二语言学习. 上海: 华东师范大学出版社.] | |
[52] | Wu, L., Mo, L., & Wang, R. (2007). Dynamic mental simulations during comprehension of motion descriptions. Advances in Psychological Science, 15(4), 605-612. |
[伍丽梅, 莫雷, 王瑞明. (2007). 有关运动的语言理解的知觉模拟. 心理科学进展, 15(4), 605-612.] | |
[53] | Zhang, C., Yao, R., & Guan, Q. (2015). Brain mechanisms of foreign language learning difficulty: Evidence from ERP and fMRI studies. Chinese Journal of Special Education, (7), 39-46. |
[张冲, 姚茹, 官群. (2015). 外语学习困难的脑机制研究: 来自ERP和fMRI的证据及启示. 中国特殊教育, (7), 39-46.] |
[1] | 石如彬, 谢久书, 杨梦情, 王瑞明. 语言和情境对具体概念感知运动仿真的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2022, 54(6): 583-594. |
[2] | 刘洁, 李瑾琪, 申超然, 胡小惠, 赵庭浩, 关青, 罗跃嘉. 数学焦虑个体近似数量加工的神经机制:一项EEG研究[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(8): 958-970. |
[3] | 王汉林, 蒋泽亮, 冯晓慧, 鲁忠义. 道德概念的空间形象性:语言因素和具身因素的共同作用[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(2): 128-138. |
[4] | 卢伟, 黄尔齐, 原晋霞. 基于定量脑电图的音乐和灯光颜色对情绪的影响 *[J]. 心理学报, 2018, 50(8): 880-891. |
[5] | 谷传华;王亚丽;吴财付;谢祥龙;崔承珠;王亚娴;王婉贞;胡碧颖;周宗奎. 社会创造性的脑机制:状态与特质的EEG α波活动特点[J]. 心理学报, 2015, 47(6): 765-773. |
[6] | 夏瑞雪;周爱保;李世峰;徐科朋;任德云;朱婧. 观点采择在内隐情绪加工中的调节作用[J]. 心理学报, 2014, 46(8): 1094-1102. |
[7] | 修利超;谈诚;蒋依涵;周仁来;陈善广. 45天-6°头低位卧床对个体额区EEG偏侧化和情绪的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2014, 46(7): 942-950. |
[8] | 沈曼琼;谢久书;张昆;李莹;曾楚轩;王瑞明. 二语情绪概念理解中的空间隐喻[J]. 心理学报, 2014, 46(11): 1671-1681. |
[9] | 王锃; 鲁忠义. 道德概念的垂直空间隐喻及其对认知的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2013, 45(5): 538-545. |
[10] | 宋宜琪;张积家;许峥烨. 汉语讲话者的时间隐喻的视觉-运动通道效应[J]. 心理学报, 2013, 45(11): 1200-1216. |
[11] | 鲁忠义,巴晓娜,李兴芬. 语言理解中情绪的动态模拟[J]. 心理学报, 2012, 44(8): 995-1003. |
[12] | 张积家,宋宜琪. 盲人的时间水平方向隐喻的通道特异性[J]. 心理学报, 2012, 44(1): 40-50. |
[13] | 陈栩茜,张积家. 时间隐喻在汉语时间量词语义加工中的作用[J]. 心理学报, 2011, 43(08): 863-877. |
[14] | 张志杰,彭春花,黄希庭. 时距估计中的锚定效应[J]. 心理学报, 2008, 40(02): 160-165. |
[15] | 王瑞明,莫雷,李利,王穗苹,吴俊. 言语理解中的知觉符号表征与命题符号表征[J]. 心理学报, 2005, 37(02): 143-150. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||